Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-rcrh6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-03T19:09:27.510Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Phonological abilities of Brazilian street poets

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Antonio Roazzi
Affiliation:
Oxford University
Ann Dowker
Affiliation:
Oxford University
Peter Bryant*
Affiliation:
Oxford University
*
Peter Bryant, Department of Experimental Psychology, University of Oxford, South Parks Road, Oxford 0X1 3UD, England

Abstract

This study deals with repentistas: oral poets in the northeast of Brazil, most of whom have had limited schooling. Twenty-four repentistas, 27 non-repentistas from a similar background, and 38 university students were given a rhyme production task. The repentistas produced about three times as many rhymes as the SES-matched non-repentistas and over one-and-a-half times as many as the students. They did not, however, differ significantly from the nonrepentistas or students in regard to auditory memory or phonological segmentation; they were similar to non-repentistas and considerably worse than students on tests of IQ and reading speed. Thus, the rhyming ability of the repentistas appears to be both highly developed and dissociated from certain other language skills.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1993

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Baddeley, A. (1976). The psychology of memory. New York: Basic.Google Scholar
Bradley, L. L. & Bryant, P. E. (1983). Categorizing sounds and learning to read: A causal connection. Nature, 301, 419421.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bryant, P. E. & Bradley, L. (1985). Children's reading problems. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Bryant, P. E., Maclean, M. E., Bradley, L. L. & Crossland, J. (1990). Rhyme and alliteration, phoneme detection and learning to read. Developmental Psychology, 26, 429438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, R. & Butterworth, B. (1985). Phonological dyslexia and dysgraphia in a highly literate subject: A developmental case with associated deficits of phonemic processing and awareness. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 37A, 435476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carraher, T. N., Carraher, D. W. & Schliemann, A. D. (1985). Mathematics in the streets and in the schools. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 3, 2129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chi, M. T. H., Glaser, R. & Rees, E. (1982). Expertise in problem-solving. In Sternberg, R. S. (Ed.), Advances in the psychology of human intelligence (Vol. 1). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Chukovsky, K. (1968). From two to five. Berkeley: University of California Press. (Translation of 1959 edition; first Russian edition published 1925.)Google Scholar
Conrad, R. (1971). The chronology of the development of covert speech in children. Developmental Psychology, 5, 398405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dowker, A. D. (1989). Rhyme and alliteration in poems elicited from young children. Journal of Child Language, 16, 181202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Emeneau, M. B. (1964). Oral poets of South India: The Todas. In Hymes, D. (Ed.), Language in culture and society: A reader in linguistics and anthropology. New York: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. & Chase, W. G. (1981). Skilled memory. In Anderson, J. R. (Ed.), Cognitive skills and their acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A. & Staszewski, J. J. (1989). Skilled memory and expertise: Mechanisms of exceptional performance. In Klahr, D. & Kotowsky, K. (Eds.), Complex information processing: The impact of Herbert A. Simon. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Finnegan, R. (1977). Oral poetry. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Finnegan, R. (1978). The Penguin book of oral poetry. Harmondsworth: Penguin.Google Scholar
Gardner, H. (1983). Frames of mind. London: Heinemann.Google Scholar
Guilford, J. P. (1956). The Structure of intellect. Psychological Bulletin, 53, 267293.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hermelin, B. & O'Connor, N. (1986). Idiot savant calendrical calculators: Rules and regularities. Psychological Medicine, 16, 885893.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hulme, C. (1984). Developmental differences in the effects of acoustic similarity on memory span. Developmental Psychology, 20, 650652.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lenel, J. & Cantor, J. (1981). Rhyme recognition and phonemic perception in young children. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 10, 5767.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liberman, I. Y., Shankweiler, D., Liberman, A. M., Fowler, C. & Fisher, W. F. (1977). Phonetic segmentation and recording in the beginning reader. In Reber, A. S. & Scarborough, D. A. (Eds.), Towards a psychology of reading. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lord, A. B. (1960). The singer of tales. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lukens, H. T. (1894). Preliminary report on the learning of language. Pedagogical Seminary, 3, 424460.Google Scholar
Lundberg, I., Frost, J. & Petersen, O. (1988). Effects of an extensive program for stimulating phonological awareness in preschool children. Reading Research Quarterly, 23,263284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maclean, M. E., Bryant, P. E. & Bradley, L. L. (1987). Rhymes, nursery rhymes and reading in early childhood. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 33, 255282.Google Scholar
Mann, V. A. (1986). Phonological awareness: The role of reading experience. Cognition, 24,6592.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morais, J. (1988, November). Constraints on the development of phonemic awareness.Paper presented in the Symposium on Phonological Processing in Literacy, to honour the research of Isabelle Y. Libermann, 39th Annual Conference of the Orton Society,Tampa, FL.Google Scholar
Morais, J., Cary, L., Alegria, J. & Bertelson, P. (1979). Does awareness of speech as a sequence of phones arise spontaneously? Cognition, 7, 323331.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O'Connor, N. & Hermelin, B. (1984). Idiot savant calendrical calculators: Maths or memory? Psychological Medicine, 24, 801806.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Read, C., Zhang, Y., Nie, H. & Ding, B. (1986). The ability to manipulate speech sounds depends on knowing alphabetic spelling. Cognition, 24, 3144.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schieffelin, B. B. (1983). Talking like birds: Sounds play in a cultural perspective. In Ochs, E. & Schieffelin, B. B. (Eds.), Acquiring conversational competence. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Scribner, S. (1984). Studying working intelligence. In Rogoff, B., & Lave, J. (Eds.), Everyday cognition: Its development in social context. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Schwartz, J. I. (1977). Metalinguistic awareness: A study of verbal play in young children. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Educational Research Association.Google Scholar
Sloboda, J. A., Hermelin, B. & O'Connor, N. (1985). An exceptional musical memory. Music Perception, 3, 155170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spearman, C. (1927). The abilities of man. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Tavares, B. (1982). Cantadores. Olinda, Brazil: Casa da Crianca.Google Scholar
Thurstone, L. (1938). Primary mental abilities (Psychometric Monographs No. 1). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Wallace, W. T. & Rubin, D. C. (1988). “The Wreck of the Old 97”; A real event remembered in song. In Neisser, U. & Winograd, E. (Eds.), Remembering reconsidered: Ecological and traditional approaches to the study of memory. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar