Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-vdxz6 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-30T19:52:44.445Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

A graphomorphemic approach to identifying and selecting a set of high utility, stable affixes common to the technical vocabulary of science

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 July 2014

KRISTIN NELLENBACH*
Affiliation:
University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
JENNIFER ZOSKI
Affiliation:
University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
JOY DIAMOND
Affiliation:
University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
KAREN ERICKSON
Affiliation:
University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Kristin Nellenbach, Department of Allied Health Sciences, Center for Literacy and Disability Studies, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Room 1110 Bondurant Hall, 321 South Columbia Street, Chapel Hill, NC 27599–7335. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Adolescents often learn science vocabulary through reading. This vocabulary is frequently characterized by multisyllabic words derived from Greek and Latin roots. While most adolescents have acquired the decoding skills to read these multisyllabic words, many students, particularly those with disabilities, cannot engage in independent word learning because they lack the skills to decode these multisyllabic words. Graphomorphemic elements of words, including affixes, support effective decoding and can eventually support word learning. This article describes an approach used to identify the most frequently occurring, stable affixes within science words so that they could be used in “big word” decoding instruction. To illustrate the approach, a subset of high frequency science words and a list of high utility, stable affixes are provided.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2014 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Abbott, S. P., & Berninger, V. M. (1999). It's never too late to remediate: Teaching word recognition to students with reading disabilities in grades 4–7. Annals of Dyslexia, 49, 223250.Google Scholar
Adams, M. J. (1990). Beginning to read: Thinking and learning about print. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Alegre, M., & Gordon, P. (1999). Frequency effects and the representational status of regular inflections. Journal of Memory and Language, 40, 4161.Google Scholar
Archer, A. L., Gleason, M. M., & Vachon, V. L. (2003). Decoding and fluency: Foundation skills for struggling older readers. Learning Disability Quarterly, 26, 89101.Google Scholar
Bar-Ilan, L., & Berman, R. A. (2007). Developing register differentiation: The Latinate–Germanic divide in English. Linguistics, 45, 135.Google Scholar
Beck, I. L., McKeown, M. G., & Kucan, L. (2008). Creating robust vocabulary: Frequently asked questions and extended examples. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Berninger, V. W., Abbott, R. D., Thomson, J., Wagner, R., Swanson, H. L., Wijsman, E. M., et al. (2006). Modeling phonological core deficits within a working memory architecture in children and adults with developmental dyslexia. Scientific Studies of Reading, 10, 165198.Google Scholar
Berninger, V. W., Nagy, W., Carlisle, J., Thomson, J., Hoffer, D., Abbott, S. et al. (2003). Effective treatment for dyslexics in grades 4–6: Behavioral and brain evidence. In Foorman, B. R. (Ed.), Preventing and remediating reading difficulties: Bringing science to scale. Timonium, MD: York Press.Google Scholar
Bhattacharya, A. (2006). Syllable-based reading strategy for mastery of scientific information. Remedial and Special Education, 27, 116123.Google Scholar
Bhattacharya, A., & Ehri, L. C. (2004). Graphosyllabic analysis helps adolescent struggling readers read and spell words. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 37, 331348.Google Scholar
BNC Consortium. (2007). British National Corpus (Version 3, XML ed.). Distributed by Oxford University Computing Services on behalf of the BNC Consortium. http://www.natcorp.ox.ac.uk/ Google Scholar
Carlisle, J. F., & Stone, C. A. (2005). Exploring the role of morphemes in word reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 40, 428449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carnegie Council on Advancing Adolescent Literacy. (2010). Time to act: An agenda for advancing adolescent literacy for college and career success. New York: Carnegie Corporation of New York.Google Scholar
Coxhead, A. (2000). A new academic word list (AWL). TESOL Quarterly, 34, 213238.Google Scholar
Coxhead, A., & Hirsch, D. (2007). A pilot science-specific word list. Revue francasie de linguistique appliquee, 12, 6578.Google Scholar
Cunningham, P. M. (1998). The multisyllabic word dilemma: Helping students build meaning, spell, and read “big” words. Reading & Writing Quarterly, 14, 189218.Google Scholar
Cunningham, P. M., & Hall, , (1998). Month-by-month phonics for the upper grades. Greensboro, NC: Carson-Dellosa.Google Scholar
Curtis, M. E. (2004). Adolescents who struggle with word identification: Research and practice. In Jetton, T. L. & Dole, J. A. (Eds.), Adolescent literacy research and practice (pp. 119134). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Davies, M. (2008). The corpus of contemporary American English: 450 million words, 1990–present. Retrieved from http://corpus.byu.edu/coca/ Google Scholar
Deacon, S. H., Whalen, R., & Kirby, J. (2011). Do children see the danger in dangerous? Grade 4, 6, and 8 children's reading of morphologically complex words. Applied Psycholinguistics, 32, 467481.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fang, Z. (2005). Scientific literacy: A functional linguistic perspective. Science Education, 89, 335347.Google Scholar
Fang, Z. (2006). The language demands of science reading in middle school. International Journal of Science Education, 28, 491520.Google Scholar
Fang, Z. (2008). Going beyond the Fab Five: Helping students cope with the unique linguistic challenges of expository reading in intermediate grades. Journal of Adolescent and Adult Literacy, 51, 476487.Google Scholar
Georgiou, G., Parrila, R., Liao, C. (2008). Rapid naming speed and reading across languages that vary in orthographic consistency. Reading and Writing, 21, 885903.Google Scholar
Goodwin, A. P., Gilbert, J. K., & Cho, S-J. (2013). Morphological contributions to adolescent word reading: An item response approach. Reading Research Quarterly, 48, 3960. doi: 10.1002/rrq.037 Google Scholar
Green, T. M. (2008). The Greek and Latin roots of English. Lanham, MD: Rowman and Littlefield.Google Scholar
Henry, M. K. (1989). Children's word structure knowledge: Implications for decoding and spelling instruction. Reading and Writing, 1, 135152.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henry, M. K. (1990). Words. Austin, TX: Pro-Ed.Google Scholar
Henry, M. K. (2010). Unlocking literacy: Effective decoding and spelling instruction (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar
Hiebert, E. H., & Cervetti, G. N. (2012). What differences in narrative and information texts mean for the learning and instruction of vocabulary. In Kame’enui, E. J. & Baumann, J. F. (Eds.), Vocabulary instruction: Research to practice (2nd ed., pp. 322344). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Kamil, M. (2003). Adolescents and literacy: Reading for the 21st century. Washington, DC: Alliance for Excellent Education.Google Scholar
Lowe, C. (2011, Winter). Top10 reasons for Latin. Classical Teacher. Retrieved October 18, 2012, from http://www.memoriapress.com/articles/Top10-Reasons-for-Latin.html Google Scholar
Mann, V. A., & Singson, M. (2003). Linking morphology knowledge to English decoding ability: Large effects of little suffixes. In Assink, E. & Sandra, D. (Eds.), Reading complex words: Cross-language studies (pp. 125). New York: Kluwer Academic.Google Scholar
McCarthy, E. (2012, August 1). Macroevolution.net. Online dictionary of biological and medical roots, prefixes, and suffixes. Retrieved from http://www.macroevolution.net Google Scholar
McCutchen, D., & Logan, B. (2011). Inside incidental word learning: Children's strategic use of morphological information to infer word meanings. Reading Research Quarterly, 46, 334339.Google Scholar
McCutchen, D., Logan, B., & Biangardi-Orpe, U. (2009). Making meaning: Children's sensitivity to morphological information during word reading. Reading Research Quarterly, 44, 360376.Google Scholar
National Assessment of Educational Progress. (2006). The nation's report card: Science 2005. Retrieved October 8, 2011, from the National Center for Educational Statistics at http://nces.ed.gov/nationsreportcard/pdf/main2005/2006466.pdf Google Scholar
National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. (2000). Report of the National Reading Panel. Teaching children to read: an evidence-based assessment of the scientific research literature on reading and its implications for reading instruction. Retrieved from http://www.nichd.nih.gov/publications/nrp/smallbook.htm Google Scholar
National Research Council. (2007). Rising above the gathering storm: Energizing and employing America for a brighter economic future. Washington, DC: National Academies Press. Retrieved October 5, 2012, from http://www.nap.edu/catalog.php?record_id=11463 Google Scholar
National Research Council. (2010). Rising above the gathering storm, revisited: Rapidly approaching Category 5. Washington, DC: National Academies Press. Retrieved October 5, 2012, from http://www.nap.edu/catalog.php?record_id=12999 Google Scholar
National Research Council. (2012). A framework for K–12 science education: Practices, crosscutting concepts, and core ideas. Washington, DC: National Academies Press.Google Scholar
Perfetti, C. (2007). Reading ability: Lexical quality to comprehension. Scientific Studies of Reading, 11, 257383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reed, D. (2008). A synthesis of morphology interventions and effects on reading outcomes for students in grades K–12. Learning Disabilities Research and Practice, 23, 3649.Google Scholar
Shanahan, T., & Shanahan, C. (2012). What is disciplinary literacy and why does it matter? Topics in Language Disorders, 32, 718.Google Scholar
Shefelbine, J. (1990). A syllabic-unit approach to teaching decoding of polysyllabic words to fourth- and sixth-grade disabled readers. In Zutell, J. & McCormick, S. (Eds.), Literacy theory and research: Analysis from multiple paradigms (pp. 223230). Chicago: National Reading Conference.Google Scholar
Templeton, S. (1983). Using the spelling/meaning connection to develop word knowledge in older students. Journal of Reading, 27, 815.Google Scholar
Templeton, S. (1991). Teaching and learning the English spelling system: Reconceptualizing method and purpose. Elementary School Journal, 92, 185201.Google Scholar
Templeton, S. (2012). The vocabulary–spelling connection and generative instruction. In Kame’enui, E. J. & Baumann, J. F. (Eds.), Vocabulary instruction: Research to practice (pp. 116138). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Templeton, S., Bear, D. R., Invernizzi, M., & Johnston, F. (2010). Vocabulary their way: Word study with middle and secondary students. Boston: Pearson.Google Scholar
Treiman, R., Mullennix, J., Bijeljac-Babic, R. & Richmond-Welty, E. D. (1995). The special role of rimes in the description, use, and acquisition of English orthography. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 107136.Google Scholar
Wellington, J., & Osborne, J. (2001). Language and literacy in science education. Philadelphia, PA: Open University Press.Google Scholar
White, T. G., Power, M. A., & White, S. (1989). Morphological analysis: Implications for teaching and understanding vocabulary growth. Reading Research Quarterly, 24, 283304.Google Scholar
White, T. G., Sowell, J., & Yanagihara, A. (1989). Teaching elementary students to use word part cues. Reading Teacher, 42, 302308.Google Scholar
Yap, M. J., & Balota, D. A. (2009). Visual word recognition of multisyllabic words. Journal of Memory and Language, 60, 502529.Google Scholar
Supplementary material: File

Nellenbach Supplementary Material

Appendix

Download Nellenbach Supplementary Material(File)
File 748.5 KB