Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T13:39:22.606Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Age of word acquisition effects in treatment of children with phonological delays

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 June 2011

JUDITH A. GIERUT*
Affiliation:
Indiana University
MICHELE L. MORRISETTE
Affiliation:
Indiana University
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Judith A. Gierut, Department of Speech and Hearing Sciences, 200 South Jordan Avenue, Bloomington, IN 47405-7002. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

The effects of the age of acquisition (AoA) of words were examined in the clinical treatment of 10 preschool children with phonological delays. Using a single-subject multiple-baseline experimental design, children were enrolled in one of four conditions that varied the AoA of the treated words (early vs. late acquired) relative to their corresponding word frequency (high vs. low frequency). Phonological generalization to treated and untreated sounds in error served as the dependent variable. Results showed that late acquired words induced greater generalization, with an effect size four times greater than early acquired words, whereas the effects of word frequency were minimized. Results are discussed relative to hypotheses about the role of AoA in language acquisition and the relevance of this variable for phonological learning.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2011

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Aitchison, J., & Straf, M. (1981). Lexical storage and retrieval: A developing skill? Linguistics, 19, 751795.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
American Speech–Language–Hearing Association Audiologic Assessment Panel 1996. (1997). Guidelines for audiologic screening. Rockville, MD: ASHA.Google Scholar
Beeson, P. M., & Robey, R. R. (2006). Evaluating single-subject treatment research: Lessons learned from the aphasia literature. Neuropsychology Review, 16, 161169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Belke, E., Brysbaert, M., Meyer, A. S., & Ghyselinck, M. (2005). Age of acquisition effects in picture naming: Evidence for a lexical–semantic competition hypothesis. Cognition, 96, B45B54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bird, H., Franklin, S., & Howard, D. (2001). Age of acquisition and imageability ratings for a large set of words, including verb and function words. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 33, 7379.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brown, G. D., & Watson, F. L. (1987). First in, first out: Word learning age and spoken word frequency as predictors of word familiarity and word naming latency. Memory & Cognition, 15, 208216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brysbaert, M., Van Wijnendaele, I., & DeDeyne, S. (2000). Age-of-acquisition of words is a significant variable in semantic tasks. Acta Psychologica, 104, 215226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burgess, C., & Livesay, K. (1998). The effect of corpus size in predicting reaction time in a basic word recognition task: Moving on from Kučera and Francis. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, and Computers, 30, 272277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Busk, P. L., & Serlin, R. C. (1992). Meta-analysis for single-case research. In Kratochwill, T. R. & Levin, J. R. (Eds.), Single-case research design and analysis (pp. 187212). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Campbell, J. M. (2004). Statistical comparison of four effect sizes for single-subject designs. Behavior Modification, 28, 234246.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carroll, J. B., & White, M. N. (1973). Age-of-acquisition norms for 220 picturable nouns. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12, 563576.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Charles-Luce, J., & Luce, P. A. (1990). Similarity neighbourhoods of words in young children's lexicons. Journal of Child Language, 17, 205215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Dewhurst, S. A., Hitch, G. J., & Barry, C. (1998). Separate effects of word frequency and age of acquisition in recognition and recall. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 284298.Google Scholar
Dinnsen, D. A. (1984). Methods and empirical issues in analyzing functional misarticulation. In Elbert, M., Dinnsen, D. A., & Weismer, G. (Eds.), Phonological theory and the misarticulating child (ASHA Monographs No. 22, pp. 517). Rockville, MD: American Speech–Language–Hearing Association.Google Scholar
Dollaghan, C. A. (1994). Children's phonological neighbourhoods: Half empty or half full? Journal of Child Language, 21, 257272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dunn, L. M., & Dunn, L. M. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (3rd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Elbert, M., Dinnsen, D. A., & Powell, T. W. (1984). On the prediction of phonologic generalization learning patterns. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 49, 309317.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, A. W., & Morrison, C. M. (1998). Real age-of-acquisition effects in lexical retrieval. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 515523.Google ScholarPubMed
Farwell, C. B. (1976). Some strategies in the early production of fricatives. Papers and Reports on Child Language Development, 12, 97104.Google Scholar
Ferguson, C. A., & Farwell, C. B. (1975). Words and sounds in early language acquisition. Language, 51, 419439.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garlock, V. M., Walley, A. C., & Metsala, J. L. (2001). Age-of-acquisition, word frequency and neighborhood density effects on spoken word recognition by children and adults. Journal of Memory and Language, 45, 468492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gerhand, S., & Barry, C. (1998). Word frequency effects in oral reading are not merely age-of-acquisition effects in disguise. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 24, 267283.Google Scholar
Gerhand, S., & Barry, C. (1999a). Age-of-acquisition and frequency effects in speeded word naming. Cognition, 73, B27B36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerhand, S., & Barry, C. (1999b). Age of acquisition, word frequency, and the role of phonology in the lexical decision task. Memory & Cognition, 27, 592602.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gierut, J. A. (1985). On the relationship between phonological knowledge and generalization learning in misarticulating children. Bloomington, IN: IULC Publications.Google Scholar
Gierut, J. A. (2008). Fundamentals of experimental design and treatment. In Dinnsen, D. A. & Gierut, J. A. (Eds.), Optimality theory, phonological acquisition and disorders (pp. 93118). London: Equinox.Google Scholar
Gierut, J. A., & Dale, R. A. (2007). Comparability of lexical corpora: Word frequency in phonological generalization. Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 21, 423433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gierut, J. A., & Morrisette, M. L. (1998). Lexical properties in implementation of sound change. In Greenhill, A., Hughes, M., Littlefield, H., & Walsh, H. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 22nd Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (pp. 257268). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Press.Google Scholar
Gierut, J. A., Morrisette, M. L., & Champion, A. H. (1999). Lexical constraints in phonological acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 26, 261294.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gierut, J. A., Morrisette, M. L., Hughes, M. T., & Rowland, S. (1996). Phonological treatment efficacy and developmental norms. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 27, 215230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilhooly, K. J., & Gilhooly, M. L. (1980). The validity of age-of-acquisition ratings. British Journal of Psychology, 71, 105110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilhooly, K. J., & Logie, R. H. (1980). Age-of-acquisition, imagery, concreteness, familiarity, and ambiguity measures for 1,944 words. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments and Computers, 12, 395427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilhooly, K. J., & Watson, F. L. (1981). Word age-of-acquisition effects: A review. Current Psychological Reviews, 1, 269286.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldman, R., & Fristoe, M. (2000). Goldman–Fristoe Test of Articulation (2nd ed.). Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Ingram, D. (1997). The categorization of phonological impairment. In Hodson, B. & Edwards, M. L. (Eds.), Perspectives in applied phonology (pp. 1941). Gaithersburg, MD: Aspen.Google Scholar
Jarvis, L. H., Merriman, W. E., Barnett, M., Hanba, J., & Van Haitsma, K. S. (2004). Input that contradicts young children's strategy for mapping novel words affects their phonological and semantic interpretation of other novel words. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 392406.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jescheniak, J. D., & Levelt, W. J. M. (1994). Word frequency effects in speech production: Retrieval of syntactic information and of phonological form. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 824843.Google Scholar
Jorm, A. F. (1991). The validity of word age-of-acquisition ratings: A longitudinal study of a child's word knowledge. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 9, 437444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jusczyk, P. W., Luce, P. A., & Charles-Luce, J. (1994). Infants’ sensitivity to phonotactic patterns in the native language. Journal of Memory and Language, 33, 630645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelly, M. H., & Martin, S. (1994). Domain-general abilities applied to domain-specific tasks: Sensitivities to probabilities in perception, cognition, and language. In Gleitman, L. & Landau, B. (Eds.), The acquisition of the lexicon (pp. 105140). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kirk, S. A., McCarthy, J. J., & Kirk, W. D. (1968). Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Abilities—Revised. Chicago: University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Kučera, H., & Francis, W. N. (1967). Computational analysis of present-day American English. Providence, RI: Brown University.Google Scholar
Leonard, L. B., & Ritterman, S. I. (1971). Articulation of /s/ as a function of cluster and word frequency of occurrence. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 14, 476485.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewis, M. B. (1999a). Age of acquisition in face categorisation: Is there an instance-based account? Cognition, 71, B23B39.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, M. B. (1999b). Are age-of-acquisition effects cumulative-frequency effects in disguise? A reply to Moore, Valentine and Turner (1999). Cognition, 72, 311316.Google Scholar
Logan, J. S. (1992). A computational analysis of young children's lexicons (Tech. Rep. No. 8). Bloomington, IN: Indiana University, Speech Research Laboratory.Google Scholar
Luce, P. A. (1986). A computational analysis of uniqueness points in auditory word recognition. Perception & Psychophysics, 39, 155158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luce, P. A., & Pisoni, D. B. (1998). Recognizing spoken words: The neighborhood activation model. Ear and Hearing, 19, 136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luce, P. A., Pisoni, D. B., & Goldinger, S. D. (1990). Similarity neighborhoods of spoken words. In Altmann, G. T. M. (Ed.), Cognitive models of speech processing: Psycholinguistic and computational perspectives (pp. 122147). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Mack, M., & Lieberman, P. (1985). Acoustic analysis of words produced by a child from 46 to 149 weeks. Journal of Child Language, 12, 527550.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Macken, M. A., & Barton, D. (1980). A longitudinal study of the voicing contrast in American English word-initial stops, as measured by voice onset time. Journal of Child Language, 7, 4174.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Magnuson, J. S., Tanenhaus, M. K., Aslin, R. N., & Dahan, D. (2003). The time course of spoken word learning and recognition: Studies with artificial lexicons. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 132, 202227.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marascuilo, L. A., & Serlin, R. C. (1988). Statistical methods for the social and behavioral sciences. New York: W. H. Freeman.Google Scholar
McReynolds, L. V., & Kearns, K. P. (1983). Single-subject experimental designs in communicative disorders. Baltimore: University Park Press.Google Scholar
Metsala, J. L. (1997a). An examination of word frequency and neighborhood density in the development of spoken word recognition. Memory & Cognition, 25, 4756.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Metsala, J. L. (1997b). Spoken word recognition in reading disabled children. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 159169.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Metsala, J. L., & Walley, A. C. (1998). Spoken vocabulary growth and the segmental restructuring of lexical representations: Precursors to phonemic awareness and early reading ability. In Metsala, J. L. & Ehri, L. C. (Eds.), Word recognition in beginning literacy (pp. 89120). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Monaghan, J., & Ellis, A. W. (2002). Age of acquisition and the completeness of phonological representations. Reading and Writing, 15, 759788.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moore, V., Valentine, T., & Turner, J. (1999). Age-of-acquisition and cumulative frequency have independent effects. Cognition, 72, 305309; discussion 311316.Google ScholarPubMed
Moore, W. H., Burke, J., & Adams, C. (1976). The effects of stimulability on the articulation of /s/ relative to cluster and word frequency of occurrence. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 19, 458466.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, P. E. (1981). Age of acquisition, imagery, recall, and the limitations of multiple-regression analysis. Memory & Cognition, 9, 277282.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morrisette, M. L., & Gierut, J. A. (2002). Lexical organization and phonological change in treatment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 45, 143159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morrisette, M. L., McAllister, K., & Gierut, J. A. (2009, November). Vocabulary size and phonological complexity. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the American Speech–Language–Hearing Association, New Orleans, LA.Google Scholar
Morrison, C. M., Chappell, T. D., & Ellis, A. W. (1997). Age of acquisition norms for a large set of object names and their relation to adult estimates and other variables. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology, 3, 528559.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morrison, C. M., & Ellis, A. W. (1995). Roles of word frequency and age of acquisition in word naming and lexical decision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 21, 116133.Google Scholar
Morrison, C. M., Ellis, A. W., & Quinlan, P. T. (1992). Age of acquisition, not word frequency, affects object naming, not object recognition. Memory & Cognition, 20, 705714.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Munson, B., & Solomon, N. P. (2004). The effect of phonological neighborhood density on vowel articulation. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 10481058.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oldfield, R. C., & Wingfield, A. (1965). Response latencies in naming objects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 17, 273281.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Olive, M. L., & Smith, B. W. (2005). Effect size calculations and single subject designs. Educational Psychology, 25, 313324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Olswang, L. B. (1998). Treatment efficacy research. In Frattali, C. M. (Ed.), Measuring outcomes in speech–language pathology (pp. 134150). New York: Thieme.Google Scholar
Plunkett, K., & Marchman, V. (1991). U-shaped learning and frequency effects in a multi-layered perception: Implications for child language acquisition. Cognition, 38, 43102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robbins, J., & Klee, T. (1987). Clinical assessment of oropharyngeal motor development in young children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 52, 271277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roid, G. H., & Miller, L. J. (1997). Leiter International Performance Scale—Revised. Chicago: Stoelting.Google Scholar
Schlosser, R. W. (2005). Meta-analysis of single-subject research: How should it be done? International Journal of Language and Communication Disorders, 40, 375378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shriberg, L. D., Kwiatkowski, J., & Gruber, F. A. (1994). Developmental phonological disorders II: Short-term speech–sound normalization. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 37, 11271150.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, B. L., McGregor, K. K., & Demille, D. (2006). Phonological development in lexically precocious 2-year-olds. Applied Psycholinguistics, 27, 355375.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snodgrass, J. G., & Vanderwart, M. (1980). A standardized set of 260 pictures: Norms for name agreement, image agreement, familiarity, and visual complexity. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 6, 174215.Google ScholarPubMed
Storkel, H. L. (2001). Learning new words: Phonotactic probability in language development. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 13211337.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Storkel, H. L. (2004). Methods for minimizing the confounding effects of word length in the analysis of phonotactic probability and neighborhood density. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 14541468.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Storkel, H. L., & Morrisette, M. L. (2002). The lexicon and phonology: Interactions in language acquisition. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 33, 2437.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tainturier, M. J., Johnson, E., Tamminen, J., & Thierry, G. (2003). An ERP investigation of age-of-acquisition effects in spoken word recognition. Brain and Language, 87, 1516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, C. K. (2007). Complexity in language learning and treatment. American Journal of Speech–Language Pathology, 16, 35.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tyler, A. A., & Edwards, M. L. (1993). Lexical acquisition and acquisition of initial voiceless stops. Journal of Child Language, 20, 253273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vitevitch, M. S. (1997). Tongue twisters reveal neighborhood density effects in speech production. Research on Spoken Language Processing, 21, 18.Google Scholar
Walley, A. C. (1993). The role of vocabulary development in children's spoken word recognition and segmentation ability. Developmental Review, 13, 286350.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walley, A. C., & Metsala, J. L. (1992). Young children's age-of-acquisition estimates for spoken words. Memory & Cognition, 20, 171182.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walley, A. C., Metsala, J. L., & Garlock, V. M. (2003). Spoken vocabulary growth: Its role in the development of phoneme awareness and early reading ability. Reading and Writing, 16, 520.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wexler, K., & Culicover, P. (1980). Formal principles of language acquisition. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wiig, E. H., Secord, W., & Semel, E. (1992). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals—Preschool. San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Brace.Google Scholar
Williams, K. T. (1997). Expressive Vocabulary Test. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar