Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-hc48f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T19:35:06.401Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Interactions between bilingual effects and language impairment: Exploring grammatical markers in Spanish-speaking bilingual children

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  25 November 2015

ANNY P. CASTILLA-EARLS*
Affiliation:
University of Houston
MARÍA ADELAIDA RESTREPO
Affiliation:
Arizona State University
ANA TERESA PÉREZ-LEROUX
Affiliation:
University of Toronto
SHELLEY GRAY
Affiliation:
Arizona State University
PAUL HOLMES
Affiliation:
Ashland University
DANIEL GAIL
Affiliation:
SUNY Buffalo
ZIQIANG CHEN
Affiliation:
SUNY Buffalo
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Anny P. Castilla-Earls, Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, University of Houston, 4455 Cullen Boulevard, Room 100, Houston, TX 77204-6018. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

This study examines the interaction between language impairment and different levels of bilingual proficiency. Specifically, we explore the potential of articles and direct object pronouns as clinical markers of primary language impairment (PLI) in bilingual Spanish-speaking children. The study compared children with PLI and typically developing (TD) children matched on age, English language proficiency, and mother's education level. Two types of bilinguals were targeted: Spanish-dominant children with intermediate English proficiency (asymmetrical bilinguals), and near-balanced bilinguals. We measured children's accuracy in the use of direct object pronouns and articles with an elicited language task. Results from this preliminary study suggest language proficiency affects the patterns of use of direct object pronouns and articles. Across language proficiency groups, we find marked differences between TD and PLI, in the use of both direct object pronouns and articles. However, the magnitude of the difference diminishes in balanced bilinguals. Articles appear more stable in these bilinguals and, therefore, seem to have a greater potential to discriminate between TD bilinguals from those with PLI. Future studies using discriminant analyses are needed to assess the clinical impact of these findings.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

American Speech–Language–Hearing Association. (2004). Guidelines for the audiologic assessment of children from birth to 5 years of age. Rockville, MD: Author. Retrieved from http://www.asha.org/policy Google Scholar
Anderson, R. (2001). Lexical morphology and verb use in child first language loss: A preliminary case study investigation. International Journal of Bilingualism, 5, 377401.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, R., & Souto, S. (2005). The use of articles by monolingual Puerto Rican Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. Applied Linguistics, 26, 621647.Google Scholar
Anderson, R. T. (1999). Loss of gender agreement in L1 attrition: Preliminary results. Bilingual Research Journal, 23, 319338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Auza, A. (2009). Are all articles difficult for Spanish-speaking children with language impairment? Poster presented at the ASHA Annual Convention, New Orleans, LA.Google Scholar
Barragan, B., Restrepo, L., Castilla-Earls, A., Olivares, W., & Gray, S. (2013). Performance of low-income Spanish-speaking children in English-only schools on the CELF-4 Spanish. Oral presentation at the ASHA Convention, Chicago.Google Scholar
Bedore, L., & Leonard, L. (2001). Grammatical morphology deficits in Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44, 905924.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bedore, L., & Leonard, L. (2005). Verb inflections and noun phrase morphology in the spontaneous speech of Spanish-speaking children with specific language impairment. Applied Linguistics, 26, 195225.Google Scholar
Bedore, L., & Peña, E. (2008). Assessment of bilingual children for identification of language impairment: Current findings and implications for practice. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 11, 129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2001). Faces of bilingualism. In Bialystok, E. (Ed.), Bilingualism in development: Language, literacy, and cognition (pp. 134151). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Castilla, A. P. (2008). Developmental measures of morphosyntactic acquisition in monolingual 3-, 4-, and 5-year-old Spanish-speaking children. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Toronto.Google Scholar
Castilla, A. P., & Pérez-Leroux, A. T. (2010). Omissions and substitutions in early Spanish clitics. Journal of Language Acquisition, 17, 225.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chierchia, G. (1998). Reference to kinds across languages. Natural Language Semantics, 6, 339405.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Crawford, J. (2007). The decline of bilingual education: How to reverse a troubling trend. International Multilingual Research Journal, 1, 3337.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cuza, A., & Pérez-Tattam, R. (2015). Grammatical gender selection and phrasal word order in child heritage Spanish: A feature reassembly approach. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. Advance online publication.Google Scholar
García, O., & Bartlett, L. (2007). A speech community model of bilingual education: Educating Latino newcomers in the U.S. International Journal of Bilingual Education and Bilingualism, 10, 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gutierrez-Clellen, V. F., Restrepo, M. A., & Simon-Cereijido, G. (2006). Evaluating the discriminant accuracy of a grammatical measure with Spanish-speaking children. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 49, 12091223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hart, B., & Risley, T. (1995). Meaningful differences in the everyday experience of young American children. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar
Hoff, E. (2006). How social contexts support and shape language development. Developmental Review, 26, 5588.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ionin, T., & Montrul, S. (2010). The role of L1-transfer in the interpretation of articles with definite plurals in L2-English. Language Learning, 60, 877925.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobson, P. F. (2012). The effects of language impairment on the use of direct object pronouns and verb inflections in heritage Spanish speakers: A look at attrition, incomplete acquisition, and maintenance. Bilingualism, Language, and Cognition, 15, 122138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacobson, P. F., & Schwartz, R. G. (2002). Morphology in incipient bilingual Spanish-speaking preschool children with specific language impairment. Applied Psycholinguistics, 23, 2341.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jia, G. X. (1998). Beyond brain maturation: The critical period hypothesis in second language acquisition revisited. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, New York University.Google Scholar
Kaufman, A., & Kaufman, S. (2004). Kaufman Assessment Battery for Children (2nd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Pearson.Google Scholar
Kohnert, K. (2010). Bilingual children with primary language impairment: Issues, evidence and implications for clinical actions. Journal of Communication Disorders, 43, 456473.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kupisch, T. (2007). Determiners in bilingual German–Italian children: What they tell us about the relation between language influence and language dominance. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 10, 5778.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leonard, L. B. (1998). Children with specific language impairment. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google ScholarPubMed
McCullagh, P., & Nelder, J. A. (1989). Generalized linear models. London: Chapman & Hall.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S. (2008). Incomplete acquisition in bilingualism: Re-examining the age factor. Philadelphia, PA: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Montrul, S., & Potowski, K. (2007). Command of gender agreement in school-age Spanish–English bilingual children. International Journal of Bilingualism, 11, 301328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan, G. P., Restrepo, M. A., & Auza, A. (2012). Spanish morphology of bilingual and monolingual children with and without language impairment? Journal of Bilingualism, 16, 578596.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Müller, N. (2003). (In)vulnerable domains in multilingualism. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nicoladis, E., & Marchak, K. (2011). Le carte blanc or la carte blanche? Bilingual children's acquisition of French adjective agreement. Language Learning, 61, 734758.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J. (2007). Bilingual children with specific language impairment: Theoretical and applied issues. Applied Psycholinguistics, 28, 512564.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., Crago, M., & Genesee, F. (2005). Domain-general versus domain-specific accounts of specific language impairment: Evidence from bilingual children's acquisition of object pronouns. Language Acquisition, 13, 3362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., Crago, M., Genesee, F., & Rice, M. (2003). Bilingual children with specific language impairment: How do they compare with their monolingual peers? Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 46, 115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paradis, J., & Genesee, F. (1996). Syntactic acquisition in bilingual children: Autonomous or interdependent? Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 18, 125.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paradis, J., Genesee, F., & Crago, M. (2011). Dual language development and disorders: A handbook on bilingualism and second language learning (2nd ed.). Baltimore, MD: Brookes.Google Scholar
Pearson, B. Z., Fernandez, S., & Oller, D. K. (1993). Lexical development in bilingual infants and toddlers: Comparison to monolingual norms. Language Learning, 43, 93120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peña, E. D., Bedore, L. M., Iglesias, A., Gutierrez-Clellen, V., & Goldstein, J. (2015). Bilingual English Spanish Oral Screener. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Peña, E. D., Gillam, R. B., Bedore, L. M., & Bohman, T. (2013). Risk for poor performance on a language screening measure for bilingual preschoolers and kindergarteners. American Journal of Speech Language Pathology, 20, 302314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, A. T., Castilla-Earls, A. P., & Bruner, J. (2012). General and specific effects of lexicon in grammar: Determiner and object pronoun omission in child Spanish. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 55, 313327.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pérez-Leroux, A. T., Munn, A., Schmitt, C., & DeIrish, M. (2004). Learning definite determiners: Genericity and definiteness in English and Spanish. In Brugos, A., Micciulla, L., & Smith, C. E. (Eds.), Supplement to the Proceedings of the 28th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development. Retrieved from http://www.bu.edu/bucld/proceedings/supplement/vol28/ Google Scholar
Pérez-Leroux, A. T., Pirvulescu, M., & Roberge, Y. (2009). Bilingualism as a window into the language faculty: The acquisition of objects in French-speaking children in bilingual and monolingual contexts. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 12, 97112.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pirvulescu, M., Roberge, Y., Thomas, D., Pérez-Leroux, A. T., & Strik, N. (2013). Bilingual effects: Exploring object omission in pronominal languages. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition. Advance online publication.Google Scholar
Restrepo, M. A. (1998). Identifiers of predominantly Spanish-speaking children with language impairment. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 41, 13981411.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Restrepo, M. A., Castilla, A. P., Schwanenflugel, P., Pritchett, S., Hamilton, C., & Arboleda, A. (2010). Sentence length, complexity and growth in Spanish-speaking children attending English-only and bilingual preschool programs. Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 41, 313.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Restrepo, M. A., & Gutierrez-Clellen, V. F. (2001). Article production in bilingual children with specific language impairment. Journal of Child Language, 28, 433452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rice, M. L., & Wexler, K. (1996). Toward tense as a clinical marker of specific language impairment in English-speaking children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 39, 12391257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sakamoto, Y., Ishiguro, M., & Kitagawa, G. (1986). Akaike information criterion statistics, D. Tokyo: Reidel.Google Scholar
Schaeffer, J. (1997). Direct object scrambling and clitic placement in Dutch and Italian child language. Doctoral dissertation, University of California, Los Angeles.Google Scholar
Sherkina-Lieber, M., Pérez-Leroux, A. T., & Johns, A. (2011). Grammar without speech production: The case of Labrador Inuttitut heritage receptive bilinguals. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 14, 301317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smyk, E., Restrepo, M. A., Gorin, J., & Gray, S. (2013). Development and validation of the Spanish–English Language Proficiency Scale (SELPS). Language, Speech, and Hearing Services in Schools, 44, 252265.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Torrens, V., & Escobar, L. (2009). Specific language impairment in Spanish and Catalan. In Grinstead, J. (Ed.), Perspectives on typical and atypical Hispanic child language development. Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (2004). The Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (4th ed.). London: Pearson Assessment.Google Scholar
Wiig, E. H., Secord, W., & Semel, E. (2004). Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals 2: Spanish edition. San Antonio, TX: Pearson.Google Scholar