Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T19:47:03.025Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Gesture and sign

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 November 2008

Marc Marschark*
Affiliation:
National Technical Institute for the Deaf, Rochester Institute of Technology
*
Marc Marschark, Center for Research, Teaching, and Learning, NTID-RIT, 52 Lomb Memorial rial Drive, Rochester, NY, 14623

Abstract

The relationship of gesticulation to speech has received considerable theoretical and empirical attention from investigators interested in the verbal status of gesture, its use in prelinguistic children, and the role of gestures in social and pragmatic communication. The relationship of gesticulation to sign language, in contrast, has received less attention. Although the gestures of deaf children have been investigated in the contexts of language acquisition and linguistic flexibility, the functions of gestures used by deaf versus hearing individuals have not been examined. One difficulty for such a study stems from the fact that gesture and sign language occur in the same modality. Gesture and sign are considered here with an eye toward determining those aspects of manual communication that are specific to users of signed languages and those in common with users of oral languages. This examination reveals that gestures produced by deaf individuals can be distinguished from the sign language in which they are embedded, both in terms of their privilege of occurrence and their semantic and pragmatic functions.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 1994

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

REFERENCES

Ackerman, J., Kyle, J., Woll, B., & Ezra, M. (1990). Lexical acquisition in sign and speech: Evidence from a longitudinal study of infants in deaf families. In Lucas, C. (Ed.), Sign language research: Theoretical issues (pp. 337345). Washington, DC: Gaullaudet University Press.Google Scholar
Acredolo, L., & Goodwyn, S. (1988). Symbolic gesturing in normal infants. Child Development. 59, 450466.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Acredolo, L., & Goodwyn, S. (1990). Sign language among hearing infants: The spontaneous development of symbolic gestures (pp. 6878). In Volterra, V. & Erting, C. J. (Eds.), From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children. New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, E. (1979). The emergence of symbols: Cognition and communication in infancy. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Bretherton, I., Shore, C., & McNew, S. (1983). Names, gestures, and objects: The role of context in the emergence of symbols. In Nelson, K. E. (Ed.), Children's language (Vol. 4, pp. 59124). New York: Gardner Press.Google Scholar
Bates, E., Thal, D., Whitesell, K., Fenson, L., & Oakes, L. (1989). Integrating language and gesture in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 25, 10041019.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bekken, K. (1989). Is there “motherese”in gesture? Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Chicago.Google Scholar
Bonvillian, J. D., & Folven, R. J. (1993). Sign language acquisition: Developmental aspects. In Marschark, M. & Clark, M. D. (Eds.), Psychological perspectives on deafness (pp. 229265). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Bonvillian, J. D., Orlansky, M. D., & Novack, L. L. (1983). Developmental milestones: Sign language acquisition, and motor development. Child Development, 54, 14351445.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruner, J. (1975). From communication to language: A psychological perspective. Cognition, 3, 255287.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caselli, M. C. (1990). Communicative gestures and first words. In Volterra, V. & Erting, C. J. (Eds.), From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children (pp. 5667). New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caselli, M. C., & Volterra, V. (1990). From communication to language in hearing and deaf children. In Volterra, V. & Erting, C. J. (Eds.), From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children (pp. 263277). New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Church, R. B., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1986). The mismatch between gesture and speech as an index of transitional knowledge. Cognition, 23, 4371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, R. A. (1978). The transition from action to gesture. In Lock, A. (Ed.), Action, gesture, and symbol: The emergence of language (pp. 4255). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Cohen, E., Namir, L., & Schlesinger, I. M. (1977). A new dictionary of sign language. The Hague: Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dobrich, W., & Scarborough, H. (1984). Form and function in early communication: Language and pointing gestures. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 38, 475490.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Efron, D. (1941). Gestures, race, and culture. The Hague: Mouton.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (1969). The repertoire of nonverbal behavioral categories. Semiotica, 1, 4998.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, A. D. (1988). Strange bedfellows: Deafness, language, and the sociology of knowledge. Symbolic Interaction, 11, 235255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, M., & Rubin, K. (1979). Hand gestures as a communicative mode in school-aged children. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 135, 189196.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Everhart, V. S., & Marschark, M. (1988). Linguistic flexibility in signed and written language productions of deaf children. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 46, 174193.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feyereisen, P., & de, Lannoy J.-P. (1991). Gestures and speech. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Folven, R. J., & Bonvillian, J. R. (1985). Nonlinguistic gestures and early sign language acquisition. Paper presented at the biennial meetings of the Society for Research in Child Development, Toronto.Google Scholar
Folven, R. J., & Bonvillian, J. R. (1987). The onset of referential signing in children. Paper presented at the biennial meetings of the Society for Research in Child Development, Baltimore.Google Scholar
Folven, R. J., & Bonvillian, J. R. (1991). The transition from nonreferential to referential language in children acquiring American Sign Language. Developmental Psychology, 27, 806816.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hockett, C. F., & Altmann, S. A. (1968). A note on design features. In Sebeok, T. A. (Ed.), Animal communication (pp. 6172). Bloomington: Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Holmes, K. M., & Holmes, D. W. (1980). Signed and spoken language development in a hearing child of hearing parents. Sign Language Studies, 28, 239254.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., & Mylander, C. (1984). Gestural communication in deaf children: The effects and noneffects of parental input on early language development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 49(Serial No. 207).CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendon, A. (1983). Word and gesture. Paper presented at the Fifth International Conference on Culture and Communications, Philadelphia.Google Scholar
Kendon, A. (1988). How gestures can become like words. In Poyatos, F. (Ed.), Cross-cultural perspectives in nonverbal communication (pp. 131141). Toronto: Hogrefe.Google Scholar
Kimura, D. (1981). Neural mechanisms in manual signing. Sign Language Studies, 33,291312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klima, E., & Bellugi, U. (1979). The signs of language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard.Google Scholar
Lock, A. (1978). The emergence of language. In Lock, A. (Ed.), Action, gesture, and symbol: The emergence of language (pp. 4255). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Marschark, M. (1993a). Origins and interactions in social, cognitive, and language development of deaf children. In Marschark, M. & Clark, M. D. (Eds.), Psychological perspectives on deafness (pp. 726). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Marschark, M. (1993b). Psychological development of deaf children. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marschark, M., & Clark, D. (1987). Linguistic and nonlinguistic creativity of deaf children. Developmental Review, 7, 2238.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marschark, M., Everhart, V. S., & Dempsey, P. R. (1991). Nonliteral content in the language of deaf, hearing, and bilingual mothers. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 37, 305323.Google Scholar
Marschark, M., Everhart, V. S., Martin, J., & West, S. A. (1987). Identifying linguistic creativity in deaf and hearing children. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 2, 281306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marschark, M., & Nall, L. (1985). Metaphoric competence in cognitive and language development. In Reese, H. (Ed.), Advances in child development and behavior (pp. 4982). New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Marschark, M., & West, S. A. (1985). Creative language abilities of deaf children. Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 28, 7378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marschark, M., West, S. A., Nall, L., & Everhart, V. (1986). Development of creative language devices in signed and oral production. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 41, 534550.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marslen-Wilson, W., Levy, E., & Tyler, L. K. (1982). Producing interpretable discourse: The establishment and maintenance of reference. In Jarvella, R. & Klein, W. (Eds.), Speech, place, and action (pp. 339378). Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
McIntire, M. L. (1977). The acquisition of American Sign Language hand configurations. Sign Language Studies, 16, 247266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeil, D. (1985). So you think gestures are nonverbal? Psychological Review, 92, 350371.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeil, D. (1987). So you do think gestures are nonverbal! Reply to Feyereisen (1987). Psychological Review, 94, 499504.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeil, D. (1992). Hand and mind. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
McNeil, D. (1993). The circle from gesture to sign. In Marschark, M. & Clark, M. D. (Eds.), Psychological perspectives on deafness (pp. 153183). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
McNeill, D., & Levy, E. (1982). Conceptual representations in language activity and gesture. In Jarvella, R. & Klein, W. (Eds.), Speech, place, and action (pp. 271295). Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
McNeill, D., Levy, B. T., & Pedelty, L. L. (1990). Speech and gesture. In Hammond, G. R. (Ed.), Advances in Psychology: Cerebral control of speech and limb movements (pp. 171224). Amsterdam: Elsevier/North Holland.Google Scholar
Meier, R. P., & Newport, B. L. (1990). Out of the hands of babes: On a possible sign advantage in language acquisition. Language, 66, 123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morrel-Samuels, P., & Krause, R. (1991). Gestures precede speech: A temporal interval predicted by word familiarity. Paper presented at annual meeting of the Psychonomic Society, San Francisco.Google Scholar
Myklebust, H. E. (1960). The psychology of deafness. New York: Grune & Stratton.Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (1973). Structure and strategy in learning to talk. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 38(149).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, K. E., Loncke, F., & Camarata, S. (1993). Implications of research on deaf and hearing children's language learning. In Marschark, M. & Clark, D. (Eds.), Psychological perspectives on deafness (pp. 123152). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Orlansky, M. D., & Bonvillian, J. D. (1984). The role of iconicity in early sign language acquisition. Journal of Speech and Hearing Disorders, 49, 287292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ortony, A. (1975). Why metaphors are necessary and not just nice. Educational Theory, 25, 4553.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Petitto, L. A. (1987). On the autonomy of language and gesture: Evidence from the acquisition of personal pronouns in American Sign Language. Cognition, 27, 152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Petitto, L. A. (1988). “Language” in the prelinguistic child. In Kessel, F. S. (Ed.), The development of language and language researchers: Essays in honor of Roger Brown. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Petitto, L. A., & Charron, F. (1991). The acquisition of Langue des Signes Quebecoise (LSQ): The first fifty signs. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1962). Plays, dreams, and imitation in childhood. New York: Norton.Google Scholar
Poizner, H., Klima, E. S., & Bellugi, U. (1987). What the hands reveal about the brain. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Prinz, P. M., & Prinz, E. A. (1979). Simultaneous acquisition of ASL and spoken English. Sign Language Studies, 25, 283296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pylyshyn, Z. (1977). What does it take to bootstrap a language? In Macnamara, J. (Ed.), Language, learning, and thought. New York: Academic.Google Scholar
Read, B., & Cherry, L. (1978). Preschool children's production of directive forms. Discourse Processes, 1, 233245.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de, Saussure F. (1974). Course in general linguistics. Glasgow: Fontana.Google Scholar
Shore, C., Bates, E., Bretherton, I., Beeghly, M., & O'Connell, B. (1990). Vocal and gestural symbols: Similarities and differences from 13 to 28 months. In Volterra, V. & Erting, C. J. (Eds.), From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children (pp. 7992). New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Volterra, V., & Erting, C. J. (Eds.). (1990). From gesture to language in hearing and deaf children. New York: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vosniadou, S. (1987). Children and metaphors. Child Development, 58, 870885.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Werner, H., & Kaplan, B. (1963). Symbol formation. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Wilbur, R. B. (1987). American Sign Language: Linguistic and applied dimensions. Boston: Little, Brown.Google Scholar