Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T19:21:58.317Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Children with autism align syntax in natural conversation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  16 January 2015

ZOË HOPKINS*
Affiliation:
University of Sussex
NICOLA YUILL
Affiliation:
University of Sussex
BILL KELLER
Affiliation:
University of Sussex
*
ADDRESS FOR CORRESPONDENCE Zoë Hopkins, School of Psychology, University of Sussex, Falmer, Brighton BN1 9QH, UK. E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Previous experimental work has shown that verbal children with an autism spectrum disorder (ASD) converge linguistically, or align, with an interlocutor, and to the same extent as typical children. However, it is not known whether ASD children align in natural conversation. The studies presented in this paper aimed to address this issue. We measured syntactic alignment in ASD children, first using an experimental task, and second in natural conversation. We found that ASD and typical children aligned to the same extent in both tasks, suggesting that experimental findings about alignment in ASD are ecologically valid. We argue, however, that the experimental measurement of alignment overstates the prevalence of syntactic alignment in children's conversations.

Type
Articles
Copyright
Copyright © Cambridge University Press 2015 

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Allen, M. L., Haywood, S., Rajendran, G., & Branigan, H. (2011). Evidence for syntactic alignment in children with autism. Developmental Science, 14, 540548. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2010.01001.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, D. K., Lord, C., Risi, S., DiLavore, P. S., Shulman, C., Thurm, A., et al. (2007). Patterns of growth in verbal abilities among children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 75, 594604. doi:10.1037/0022-006X.75.4.594CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron-Cohen, S., Leslie, A. M., & Frith, U. (1985). Does the autistic child have a “theory of mind”? Cognition, 21, 3746. doi:10.1016/0010-0277(85)90022-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barr, D. J., Levy, R., Scheepers, C., & Tily, H. J. (2013). Random effects structure for confirmatory hypothesis testing: Keep it maximal. Journal of Memory and Language, 68, 255278. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2012.11.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bates, D., Maechler, M., & Bolker, B. (2011). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes. R package version 0.999375-39. Retrieved from http://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lme4Google Scholar
Biber, D., Johansson, S., Leech, G., Conrad, S., & Finegan, E. (1999). The Longman grammar of spoken and written English. Computational Linguistics, 27, 132139.Google Scholar
Bock, K., & Griffin, Z. M. (2000). The persistence of structural priming: Transient activation or implicit learning? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 129, 177192. doi:10.1037/0096-3445.129.2.177CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boddaert, N., Belin, P., Chabane, N., Poline, J.-B., Barthélémy, C., Mouren-Simeoni, M.-C., et al. (2003). Perception of complex sounds: Abnormal pattern of cortical activation in autism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 20572060. doi:10.1176/appi.ajp.160.11.2057CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Branigan, H. P., Pickering, M. J., Pearson, J., & McLean, J. F. (2010). Linguistic alignment between people and computers. Journal of Pragmatics, 42, 23552368. doi:10.1016/j.pragma.2009.12.012CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Branigan, H. P., Pickering, M. J., Pearson, J., McLean, J. F., & Brown, A. (2011). The role of beliefs in lexical alignment: Evidence from dialogs with humans and computers. Cognition, 121, 4157. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2011.05.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Capps, L., Kehres, J., & Sigman, M. (1998). Conversational abilities among children with autism and children with developmental delays. Autism, 2, 325344. doi:10.1177/1362361398024002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chang, F., Dell, G., & Bock, K. (2006). Becoming syntactic. Psychological Review, 113, 234272.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chevallier, C., Kohls, G., Troiani, V., Brodkin, E. S., & Schultz, R. T. (2012). The social motivation theory of autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16, 231239. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2012.02.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, H. H., & Marshall, C. R. (1981). Definite reference and mutual knowledge. In A. K. Joshi, B. Webber, & I. Sag (Eds.), Elements of discourse understanding (pp 1063). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Dale, R., & Spivey, M. J. (2006). Unravelling the dyad: Using recurrence analysis to explore patterns of syntactic coordination between children and caregivers in conversation. Language Learning, 56, 391430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Delaney, H. D., & Maxwell, S. E. (1981). On using analysis of covariance in repeated measures designs. Multivariate Behavioral Research, 16, 105123. doi:10.1207/s15327906mbr1601_6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dickson, W. P. (1982). Two decades of referential communication research: A review and meta-analysis. In Brainerd, C. J. & Pressley, M. (Eds.), Verbal processes in children (pp. 133). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Diehl, J. J., & Paul, R. (2012). Acoustic differences in the imitation of prosodic patterns in children with autism spectrum disorders. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6, 123134. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2011.03.012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dixon, P. (2008). Models of accuracy in repeated-measures designs. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 447456. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2007.11.004CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunn, L. M., Dunn, D. M., & Styles, B. (2009). British Picture Vocabulary Scale (3rd ed.). London: GL Assessment.Google Scholar
Emerich, D. M., Creaghead, N. A., Grether, S. M., Murray, D., & Grasha, C. (2003). The comprehension of humorous materials by adolescents with high-functioning autism and Asperger's syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33, 253257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fletcher-Watson, S., Leekam, S. R., Benson, V., Frank, M. C., & Findlay, J. M. (2009). Eye-movements reveal attention to social information in autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia, 47, 248257. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.07.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garrod, S., & Clark, A. (1993). The development of dialogue co-ordination skills in schoolchildren. Language and Cognitive Processes, 8, 101116. doi:10.1080/01690969308406950CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garrod, S., & Pickering, M. J. (2004). Why is conversation so easy? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 811. doi:10.1016/j.tics.2003.10.016CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gerard, J., Keller, F., & Palpanas, T. (2010). Corpus evidence for age effects on priming in child language. Paper presented at the 32nd annual conference of the Cognitive Science Society, Portland, OR.Google Scholar
Gries, S. T. (2005). Syntactic priming: A corpus-based approach. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 34, 365–99. doi:10.1007/s10936-005-6139-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gropen, J., Pinker, S., Hollander, M., Goldberg, R., & Wilson, R. (1989). The learnability and acquisition of the dative alternation in English. Language, 65, 203257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grossi, D., Marcone, R., Cinquegrana, T., & Gallucci, M. (2013). On the differential nature of induced and incidental echolalia in autism. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 57, 903912. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2788.2012.01579.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanley, M., McPhillips, M., Mulhern, G., & Riby, D. M. (2012). Spontaneous attention to faces in Asperger syndrome using ecologically valid static stimuli. Autism. Advance online publication. doi:10.1177/1362361312456746Google ScholarPubMed
Happé, F. G. (1993). Communicative competence and theory of mind in autism: A test of relevance theory. Cognition, 48, 101119.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartsuiker, R., & Kolk, H. H. J. (1998). Syntactic persistence in Dutch. Language and Speech, 41, 143184.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartung, J. R. (1970). A review of procedures to increase verbal imitation skills and functional speech in autistic children. Journal of Speech & Hearing Disorders, 35, 203217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Healey, P. G. T., Purver, M., & Howes, C. (2012). Structural divergence in dialogue. Unpublished manuscript.Google Scholar
Hobson, R. P. (1986). The autistic child's appraisal of expressions of emotion: A further study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 27, 671680. doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.1986.tb00191.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hobson, R. P., & Hobson, J. A. (2008). Dissociable aspects of imitation: A study in autism. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 101, 170185. doi:10.1016/j.jecp.2008.04.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howes, C., Healey, P., & Purver, M. (2010). Tracking lexical and syntactic alignment in conversation. Processes of Cognitive Science, 9, 20042009.Google Scholar
Ingersoll, B. (2008). The social role of imitation in autism: Implications for the treatment of imitation deficits. Infants & Young Children, 21, 107119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jaeger, T. F. (2008). Categorical data analysis: Away from ANOVAs (transformation or not) and towards logit mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 59, 434446. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2007.11.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kamio, Y., Robins, D., Kelley, E., Swainson, B., & Fein, D. (2007). Atypical lexical/semantic processing in high-functioning pervasive developmental disorders without early language delay. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37, 11161122.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaufman, A. S., & Kaufman, N. L. (2004). Kaufman Brief Intelligence Test (2nd ed.). Bloomington, IL: Pearson.Google Scholar
Kidd, E. (2011). Individual differences in syntactic priming in language acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 33, 393418. doi:10.1017/S0142716411000415CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., & Volkmar, F. (2003). The enactive mind, or from actions to cognition: Lessons from autism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 358B, 345360. doi:10.1098/rstb.2002.1202CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., Volkmar, F., & Cohen, D. (2002). Visual fixation patterns during viewing of naturalistic social situations as predictors of social competence in individuals with autism. Archives of General Psychiatry, 59, 809816. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.59.9.809CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leinonen, E. K., & Letts, C. (1997). Referential communication tasks: Performance by normal and pragmatically impaired children. European Journal of Disorders of Communication, 32, 5365.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levelt, W. J. M., & Kelter, S. (1982). Surface form and memory in question answering. Cognitive Psychology, 14, 78106.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Magonde, M., & Keller, B. (2014). Syntactic alignment software. Unpublished manuscript, University of Sussex, Department of Informatics.Google Scholar
Marsh, L., Pearson, A., Ropar, D., & Hamilton, A. (2013). Children with autism do not overimitate. Current Biology, 223, R266R268. doi:10.1016/j.cub.2013.02.036CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Menyuk, P., & Quill, K. (1985). Semantic problems in autistic children. In Schopler, E. & Mesibov, G. (Eds.), Communication problems in autism. New York: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Messenger, K., Branigan, H. P., McLean, J. F., & Sorace, A. (2012). Is young children's passive syntax semantically constrained? Evidence from syntactic priming. Journal of Memory and Language, 66, 568587. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2012.03.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2009). Priming third-party ostracism increases affiliative imitation in children. Developmental Science, 12, F1F8. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00820.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pearlman-Avnion, S., & Eviatar, Z. (2002). Narrative analysis in developmental social and linguistic pathologies: Dissociation between emotional and informational language use. Brain & Cognition, 48, 494499.Google ScholarPubMed
Persicke, A., Tarbox, J., Ranick, J., & St. Clair, M. (2013). Teaching children with autism to detect and respond to sarcasm. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 7, 193198. doi:10.1016/j.rasd.2012.08.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M. J., & Branigan, H. P. (1998). The representation of verbs: Evidence from syntactic priming in language production. Journal of Memory and Language, 39, 633651. doi:10.1006/jmla.1998.2592CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pickering, M. J., & Ferreira, V. S. (2008). Structural priming: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 134, 427459. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.3.427CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2004a). Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 169190, discussion 190–226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roland, D., Dick, F., & Elman, J. L. (2007). Frequency of basic English grammatical structures: A corpus analysis. Journal of Memory and Language, 57, 348379. doi:10.1016/j.jml.2007.03.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ross, D. E. (2002). Replacing faulty conversational exchanges for children with autism by establishing a functionally equivalent alternative response. Education and Training in Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities, 37, 343362.Google Scholar
Rowland, C. F., Chang, F., Ambridge, B., Pine, J. M., & Lieven, E. V. M. (2012). The development of abstract syntax: Evidence from structural priming and the lexical boost. Cognition, 125, 4963. doi:10.1016/j.cognition.2012.06.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rutter, M., Bailey, A., & Lord, C. (2003). Social Communication Questionnaire (SCQ). Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services.Google Scholar
Shih, P., Shen, M., Ottl, B., Keehn, B., Gaffrey, M. S., & Muller, R. A. (2010). Atypical network connectivity for imitation in autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia, 48, 29312939.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slocombe, K. E., Alvarez, I., Branigan, H. P., Jellema, T., Burnett, H. G., Fischer, A., et al. (2012). Linguistic alignment in adults with and without Asperger's syndrome. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders. Advance online publication. doi:10.1007/s10803-012-1698-2Google Scholar
Stewart, S. L. K., Corcoran, R., & Drake, R. J. (2008). Alignment and theory of mind in schizophrenia. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 13, 431448. doi:10.1080/13546800802405610CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stockbridge, M. D., Happé, F. G. E., & White, S. J. (2013). Impaired comprehension of alternating syntactic constructions in autism. Autism Research. Advance online publication. doi:10.1002/aur.1348Google ScholarPubMed
Tager-Flusberg, H. (1981). On the nature of linguistic functioning in early infantile autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 11, 4556.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tager-Flusberg, H. (1996). Current theory and research on language and communication in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 26, 169172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tager-Flusberg, H., & Anderson, M. (1991). The development of contingent discourse ability in autistic children. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 32, 11231134.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vivanti, G., & Hamilton, A. (2014). Imitation in autism spectrum disorders. In Volkmar, F., Paul, R., Rogers, S., & Pelphrey, K. (Eds.), The handbook of autism and developmental disorders: Vol. 1. Diagnosis, development and brain mechanisms (4th ed., pp. 278301). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Volden, J. (2002). Features leading to judgements of inappropriacy in the language of speakers with autism: A preliminary study. Journal of Speech and Language—Language Pathology and Audiology, 26, 138146.Google Scholar
Wald, A. (1943). Test of statistical hypotheses concerning several parameters when the number of observations is large. Transactions of the American Mathematical Society, 54, 426482.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wheeldon, L. R., & Smith, M. C. (2003). Phrase structure priming: A short-lived effect. Language and Cognitive Processes, 18, 431442.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitehouse, A. J. O., & Bishop, D. V. M. (2008). Do children with autism “switch off” to speech sounds? An investigation using event-related potentials. Developmental Science, 11, 516524. doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00697.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wiggins, L. D., Bakeman, R., Adamson, L. B., & Robins, D. L. (2007). The utility of the Social Communication Questionnaire in screening for autism in children referred for early intervention. Focus on Autism and Other Developmental Disabilities. Advance online publication. doi:10.1177/10883576070220010401CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zaroff, C. M., & Uhm, S. Y. (2012). Prevalence of autism spectrum disorders and influence of country of measurement and ethnicity. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 47, 395398.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed