Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-r5fsc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-04T09:22:49.203Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Child-centered behaviors of caregivers with 12-month-old infants: Associations with passive joint engagement and later language

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 July 2006

CAROL HAMER TRAUTMAN
Affiliation:
University of Texas at Dallas, Callier Center for Communication Disorders
PAMELA ROSENTHAL ROLLINS
Affiliation:
University of Texas at Dallas, Callier Center for Communication Disorders

Abstract

This study investigates three aspects of social communication in 12-month-old infants and their caregivers: (a) caregiver conversational style, (b) caregiver gesture, and (c) infant engagement. Differences in caregiver behavior during passive joint engagement were associated with language outcomes. Although total mean duration of infant time in passive joint engagement was negatively associated with later language, caregiver contingent comments (CCCs) addressed to infants during passive joint engagements related to language learning. CCC utterances were found to co-occur with gesture, suggesting that CCC is an inherently multimodal conversational style. The positive association between CCCs during passive joint engagements and later language suggests that caregiver behavior is important, even at times when infants are not actively engaged with the caregiver.

Type
Articles
Copyright
2006 Cambridge University Press

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adamson L.1995. Communication development during infancy. Madison, WI: Brown & Benchmark.
Bakeman R., & Adamson L.1984. Coordinating attention to people and objects in mother–infant and peer–infant interaction. Child Development, 55, 12781289.Google Scholar
Bates E., Bretherton I., & Snyder L.1988. From first words to grammar: Individual differences and dissociable mechanisms. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Bruner J.1983. Child's talk: Learning to use Language. New York: Norton.
Carpenter M., Nagell K., & Tomasello M.1998. Social cognition, joint attention, and communicative competence from 9 to 15 months of age. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 63, 2781.Google Scholar
DeCasper A. J., & Fifer W. P.1980. Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mothers' voices. Science, 208, 11741176.Google Scholar
Fantz R. L.1963. Pattern vision in newborn infants. Science, 140, 296297.Google Scholar
Fenson L., Dale P., Reznick J. S., Thal D., Bates E., Hartung J., et al. 1993. The MacArthur Communicative Development Inventories: User's guide and technical manual. San Diego, CA: Singular Publishing Group.
Gogate L., Bahrick L., & Watson J.2000. A study of multimodal motherese: The role of temporal synchrony between verbal labels and gestures. Child Development, 71, 878894.Google Scholar
Gogate L., Walker-Andrews A., & Bahrick L.2001. The intersensory origins of word comprehension: An ecological–dynamic systems view. Developmental Science, 4, 137.Google Scholar
Hart B., & Risley T.1999. The social world of children: Learning to talk. Baltimore, MD: Brookes.
Huttenlocher J., Haight W., Bryk A., Seltzer M., & Lyons T.1991. Early vocabulary growth: Relation to language input and gender. Developmental Psychology, 27, 236248.Google Scholar
Iverson J., Capirci O., Longobardi E., & Caselli M.1999. Gesturing in mother–child interactions. Cognitive Development, 14, 5775.Google Scholar
Landis J., & Koch G.1977. The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics, 33, 159174.Google Scholar
MacWhinney B.1991. The CHILDES Project: Computational tools for analyzing talk. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Ninio A., & Snow C. E.1996. Pragmatic development. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.
Ninio A., Snow C. E., Pan B. A., & Rollins P. R.1994. Classifying communicative acts in children's interactions. Journal of Communication Disorders, 27, 157187.Google Scholar
Ring E. D., & Fenson L.2000. The correspondence between parent report and child performance for receptive and expressive vocabulary beyond infancy. First Language, 20, 141159.Google Scholar
Rollins P. R.2003. Caregivers' contingent comments to 9-month infants: Relationships with later language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 221234.Google Scholar
Rollins P. R., & Snow C. E.1998. Shared attention and grammatical development in typical children and children with autism. Journal of Child Language, 25, 653673.Google Scholar
Scarborough H.1990. Index of productive syntax. Applied Pycholinguistics, 11, 122.Google Scholar
Schmidt C. L., & Lawson K. R.2002. Caregiver attention-focusing and children's attention-sharing behaviors as predictors of later verbal IQ in very low birthweight children. Journal of Child Language, 29, 322.Google Scholar
Thal D. J., & Katich J.1996. Predicaments in early identification of specific language impairment: Does the early bird always catch the worm? In K. Cole, P. Dale & D. Thal (Eds.), Assessment of communication and language (pp. 128). Baltimore, MD: Paul H. Brookes.
Thal D. J., O'Hanlon L., Clemmons M., & Fralin L.1999. Validity of a parent report measure of vocabulary and syntax for preschool children with language impairment. Journal of Speech Language & Hearing Research, 42, 482496.Google Scholar
Tomasello M.1999. The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Tomasello M., & Farrar M. J.1986. Joint attention and early language. Child Development, 57, 14541463.Google Scholar
Tomasello M., & Todd J.1983. Joint attention and lexical acquisition style. First Language, 4, 197212.Google Scholar
Trevarthen C.1979. Communication and cooperation in early infancy: A description of primary intersubjectivity. In M. Bullowa (Ed.), Before speech: The beginning of interpersonal communication (pp. 321347). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.
Trevarthen C.1988. Universal cooperative motives: How infants begin to know the language and culture of their parents. In G. Jahoda & I. M. Lewis (Eds.), Acquiring culture: Cross cultural studies in child development (pp. 3790). London: Croom Helm.
Vygotsky L. S.1978. Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Werner H., & Kaplan B.1963. Symbol formation. New York: Wiley.