Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jn8rn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-16T19:25:24.217Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

That brachycephalic look: Infant-like facial appearance in short-muzzled dog breeds

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 January 2023

Elizabeth S Paul*
Affiliation:
Bristol Veterinary School, University of Bristol, Langford House, Langford BS40 5DU, UK
Rowena MA Packer
Affiliation:
Department of Clinical Sciences and Services, The Royal Veterinary College, Hatfield, Herts AL9 7TA, UK
Paul D McGreevy
Affiliation:
School of Environmental and Rural Science, Faculty of Science, Agriculture, Business and Law, University of New England, Armidale, NSW 2351, Australia
Emily Coombe
Affiliation:
Bristol Veterinary School, University of Bristol, Langford House, Langford BS40 5DU, UK Positive Dog Training, Long Ashton, Bristol, UK
Elsa Mendl
Affiliation:
Bristol Veterinary School, University of Bristol, Langford House, Langford BS40 5DU, UK
Vikki Neville
Affiliation:
Bristol Veterinary School, University of Bristol, Langford House, Langford BS40 5DU, UK
*
Author for correspondence: Elizabeth S Paul, Email: [email protected]
Rights & Permissions [Opens in a new window]

Abstract

Brachycephalic dog breeds are highly popular, yet their conformation-related disorders represent a major welfare concern. It has been suggested that the current popularity of such breeds can be explained by their cute, infant-like facial appearances. The concept of ‘kindchenschema’ refers to the observation that certain physical features of infant humans and other animals can automatically stimulate positive and nurturant feelings in adult observers. But the proposal that brachycephalic dogs possess heightened ‘kindchenschema’ facial features, even into adulthood, has never been formally investigated. Here, we hypothesised that relative muzzle shortening across a range of breeds would be associated with known ‘kindchenschema’ facial features, including a relatively larger forehead, larger eyes and smaller nose. Relative fronto-facial feature sizes in exemplar photographs of adult dogs from 42 popular breeds were measured and associated with existing data on the relative muzzle length and height-at-withers of the same breeds. Our results show that, in adulthood, shorter-muzzled breeds have relatively larger (taller) foreheads and relatively larger eyes (i.e. area of exposed eyeball relative to overall face area) than longer-muzzled breeds, and that this effect is independent of breed size. In sum, brachycephalic dog breeds do show exaggeration of some, but not all, known fronto-facial ‘kindchenschema’ features, and this may well contribute to their apparently cute appearance and to their current popularity as companion animals. We conclude that the challenge of addressing conformation-related disorders in companion dogs needs to take account of the cute, ‘kindchenschema’ looks that many owners are likely to be attracted to.

Type
Research Article
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BY
This is an Open Access article, distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0), which permits unrestricted re-use, distribution and reproduction, provided the original article is properly cited.
Copyright
© The Author(s), 2023. Published by Cambridge University Press on behalf of The Universities Federation for Animal Welfare

Introduction

Brachycephalic dogs, with their flattened faces and shortened muzzles, have long been popular pets (Skipper Reference Skipper, Packer and O’Neill2021). But the unprecedented rise in ownership of breeds such as the Pug, Boston terrier and French bulldog in recent years has prompted numerous researchers and organisations to voice concerns about the health and welfare consequences of such facial morphologies (Asher et al. Reference Asher, Diesel, Summers, McGreevy and Collins2009; Rooney & Sargan Reference Rooney and Sargan2009; Bateson Reference Bateson2016; Brachycephalic Working Group 2018; British Veterinary Association 2020a,b). Discourse has focused largely on a single, central conflict: between the clinical problems associated with extreme muzzle shortening (O’Neill et al. Reference O’Neill, Jackson, Guy, Church, McGreevy, Thompson and Brodbelt2015, Reference O’Neill, Brodbelt, Church and Sanchez2017, Reference O’Neill, Skipper, Kadhim, Church, Brodbelt and Packer2019, Reference O’Neill, Packer, Lobb, Church, Brodbelt and Pegram2020; Packer et al. Reference Packer, Hendricks and Burn2015a,Reference Packer, Hendricks, Tivers and Burnb; Packer & Tivers Reference Packer and Tivers2015; Liu et al. Reference Liu, Troconis, Kalmar, Price, Wright, Adams, Sargan and Ladlow2017; Seppanen et al. Reference Seppanen, Kaimio, Schildt, Lilja-Maula, Hyytiainen, Molsa, Morelius, Rajamaki, Lappalainen and Rantala2019; Fawcett et al. Reference Fawcett, Barrs, Awad, Child, Brunel, Mooney, Martinez-Taboada, McDonald and McGreevy2019) and public demand for extremely brachycephalic breeds, not least because of their putatively appealing, cute appearance (Packer et al. Reference Packer, O’Neill, Fletcher and Farnworth2019, Reference Packer, O’Neill, Fletcher and Farnworth2020). Recent research has indicated that many owners understand that brachycephalia can have adverse consequences for health but choose to keep such breeds anyway (Packer et al. Reference Packer, O’Neill, Fletcher and Farnworth2020). This may be partly due to the perceived positive behavioural traits of some of these breeds (Packer et al. Reference Packer, O’Neill, Fletcher and Farnworth2020), with owners’ denial of their dogs’ health problems (Packer et al. Reference Packer, Hendricks and Burn2012), or with the paradoxical appeal of disabled dogs (Sandøe et al. Reference Sandøe, Kondrup, Bennett, Forkman, Meyer, Proschowsky, Serpell and Lund2017; Serpell Reference Serpell2019). Either way, there is little doubt that some aspects of the appearance of brachycephalic animals is also a crucial driver of ownership (e.g. Waller et al. Reference Waller, Peirce, Caeiro, Scheider, Burrows, McCune and Kaminski2013; Packer et al. Reference Packer, Murphy and Farnworth2017, Reference Packer, O’Neill, Fletcher and Farnworth2020).

To explore why brachycephalic dogs are so popular, the current study was designed to investigate the precise nature of the ‘brachycephalic look’ in contemporary dog breeds. Commentators and researchers appear to agree that such breeds carry the appealing facial features associated with human perceptions of cuteness, and that these features are a key determinant of the popularity of short-muzzled dogs (e.g. Serpell Reference Serpell2002; McGreevy et al. Reference McGreevy, Georgevsky, Carrasco, Valenzuela, Duffy and Serpell2013; Packer et al. Reference Packer, Murphy and Farnworth2017; Sandøe et al. Reference Sandøe, Kondrup, Bennett, Forkman, Meyer, Proschowsky, Serpell and Lund2017; O’Neill et al. Reference O’Neill, Sahota, Brodbelt, Church, Packer and Pegram2022). But these claims have never been formally investigated. Do brachycephalic dogs really look cute? And if so, in what ways?

The focus of this paper is the ‘kindchenschema’ effect (also known as the cute effect); a well-studied phenomenon in psychological research in which infantile features in images of both children and animals are regularly found to provoke positive and affectionate feelings in human observers, and to prompt careful and nurturant behaviours (for a recent review, see Kringelbach et al. Reference Kringelbach, Stark, Alexander, Bornstein and Stein2016). Our aim here is to discover whether relative muzzle shortening across a range of dog breeds is linked to the key facial features known to be appealing to, and to elicit nurturant behaviours in, human observers. Specifically, we sought to discover whether muzzle shortening in the sagittal plane of dogs’ heads (i.e. in profile) is statistically associated with known ‘kindchenschema’ features in the coronal plane (i.e. fronto-facial features such as the relative size of eyes and forehead) (see Figure 1). To do this, we investigated the links between two datasets: (i) pre-existing anatomical measures of relative muzzle shortening across a range of popular dog breeds; and (ii) photograph-based measures of fronto-facial features of the same breeds, collected for this study. We hypothesised that shorter relative muzzle length in the sagittal profiles of breeds would be linked with more infantile feature proportions in the frontal (coronal) planes of their faces.

Figure 1. Sagittal and coronal plane line-drawings of (from left to right): adult human, infant human, adult French bulldog and adult Siberian husky.

The ‘kindchenschema’ effect

People’s perceptions of cuteness (i.e. of pleasant and appealing youthfulness) are based upon the detection of a wide range of features associated with infancy, including various aspects of body shape, voice, skin texture, hair colour and movement style (Berry & McArthur Reference Berry and McArthur1986; Kringelbach et al. Reference Kringelbach, Stark, Alexander, Bornstein and Stein2016). However, the infantile features of greatest influence are in the face. The cranium of newborn humans is relatively larger than the midface, and grows more slowly with age, leading to a distinctive pattern of change in the both the sagittal and coronal planes of humans’ heads as they develop from infancy to adulthood (Pittenger & Shaw Reference Pittenger and Shaw1975; Berry & McArthur Reference Berry and McArthur1986). As a result, the facial appearance of infants is distinctively different from that of adults, with babies exemplifying relatively larger eyes (i.e. the area of the orbit that is visible), smaller noses and taller, more protruding foreheads (Lorenz Reference Lorenz1943, Reference Lorenz1971; see also Berry & McArthur Reference Berry and McArthur1986; Kringelbach et al. Reference Kringelbach, Stark, Alexander, Bornstein and Stein2016). Early studies of the effects of these ‘kindchenschema’ facial features demonstrated that youthful variants of line-drawn sagittal-plane images, with relatively smaller, lower- and mid-face regions, can heighten people’s cuteness perceptions of human children, animals, and even of cartoon depictions of inanimate objects (Gardner & Wallach Reference Gardner and Wallach1965; Alley Reference Alley1981; Berry & McArthur Reference Berry and McArthur1985). That said, the vast majority of psychological research into ‘kindchenschema’ and their effects on adult observers has focused on the coronal plane of the head (fronto-facial features: Fullard & Reiling Reference Fullard and Reiling1976; Sternglanz et al. Reference Sternglanz, Gray and Murakami1977; Hildebrandt & Fitzgerald Reference Hildebrandt and Fitzgerald1979; Alley Reference Alley1981; Berry & McArthur Reference Berry and McArthur1985; McKelvie Reference McKelvie1993; Brosch et al. Reference Brosch, Sander and Scherer2007; Glocker et al. Reference Glocker, Langleben, Ruparel, Loughead, Gur and Sachser2009; Parsons et al. Reference Parsons, Young, Kumari, Stein and Kringelbach2011; Senese et al. Reference Senese, De Falco, Bornstein, Caria, Buffolino and Venuti2013; Esposito et al. Reference Esposito, Nakazawa, Ogawa, Stival, Putnick and Bornstein2015; Almanza-Sepdulveda et al. Reference Almanza-Sepdulveda, Dudin, Wonch, Steiner, Feinberg, Fleming and Hall2018). For example, images manipulated to convey more infant-like features, such as large, rounded eyes and small, ‘button’ noses are rated as more attractive, more cute, and induce more careful and care-giving behaviour in observers, than those with more adult forms. These sorts of associations between coronal-plane infantile morphology and cuteness attributions among adult care-givers, do not occur by chance. Human reproduction is thought to depend, at least to some extent, on the ‘kindchenschema’ effect, aiding the maximisation of care-giving to younger offspring (Kringlebach et al. Reference Kringelbach, Stark, Alexander, Bornstein and Stein2016). And, given that the front of the face is the key point of focus for human social and emotional perception processing, it is not surprising that fronto-facial infantile features represent prime releasing stimuli for such behaviour (e.g. Lacruz et al. Reference Lacruz, Stringer, Kimbel, Wood, Harvati, O’Higgins, Bromage and Arsuaga2019), with eye-tracking studies indicating that visual attention to this part and plane of the face is central to human cuteness perceptions (Borgi et al. Reference Borgi, Cogliati-Dezza, Brelsford, Meints and Cirulli2014).

‘Kindchenschema’ in dogs: Sagittal plane, coronal plane, or both?

The changes that occur during cranio-facial growth in human infants are mirrored across numerous species. As a result, the young of many animals can readily generate ‘kindchenschema’ effects among human observers, and experimentally enhancing images of animals’ facial features in an infantile manner — e.g. enlarging the eyes or forehead — can magnify these effects (e.g. Archer & Monton Reference Archer and Monton2011; Lehmann et al. Reference Lehmann, Huis in‘t Veld and Vingerhoets2013; Chersini et al. Reference Chersini, Hall and Wynne2018). There is also evidence that similar processes persist in generating both human- and animal-focused ‘kindchenschema’ effects (Golle et al. Reference Golle, Lisibach, Mast and Lobmaier2013).

As noted above, most experimental investigations of the influence of infantile morphology on cuteness perceptions, in both animal and human images, have focused upon features in the coronal plane of the face, although sagittal plane manipulations have also been found to also generate ‘kindchenschema’ effects (Gardner & Wallach Reference Gardner and Wallach1965; Alley Reference Alley1981; Berry & McArthur Reference Berry and McArthur1985). The facial profiles of brachycephalic dog breeds are noteworthy because their adult heads look somewhat infantile — sharing some of the features of both puppies and human infants, with a relatively small, non-protruding mid-face and more prominent forehead (see Figure 1). So sagittal-plane features such as these may be all that is needed for people to perceive brachycephalic dogs as cute (e.g. Serpell Reference Serpell2002; Serpell & Paul Reference Serpell, Paul, Salmon and Shackelford2011). However, another possibility is that short muzzles in dogs are also associated with infantile facial features in the coronal plane of the face, and that short muzzles may contribute to people’s perceptions of cuteness in brachycephalic breeds through this route as well. For example, it is possible that many brachycephalic breeds have not only relatively short muzzles, but also relative larger eyes, small noses and large, tall foreheads; i.e. known coronal-plane (fronto-facial) infantile features with strong ‘kindchenschema’ effects.

Measurement of muzzle length and fronto-facial ‘kindchenschema’ features

In recent years, brachycephalia in the veterinary context has focused on the relative length of the muzzle, which has been investigated using anatomical measurements in the sagittal and transverse planes of affected dogs’ heads (e.g. Georgevsky et al. Reference Georgevsky, Carrasco, Valenzuela and McGreevy2014; Packer et al. Reference Packer, Hendricks, Tivers and Burn2015b, based on Sutter et al. Reference Sutter, Mosher, Gray and Ostrander2008). To obtain measures of the fronto-facial (‘kindchenschema’) features of different breeds in the coronal plane of the head (i.e. features as would normally be observed by looking directly at a dog’s face), a wide range of photographic images were used here, sourced via the internet. Using these images, the actual ‘looks’ of the dogs, as seen by owners and potential purchasers in the real world, could be established (i.e. incorporating, but not separately measuring, anatomical features including skull morphology, soft tissue thickness, and fur thickness). By using externally sourced photographic images, we were able to access a broad group of breed exemplars. The fronto-facial features considered here were those that have previously been found to be associated with infancy and cuteness attributions. They were those that could also be readily applied to facial measurements, from photographs, in dogs: relative forehead size, relative eye size, relative nose size. Relatively large (taller) foreheads, large eyes, and small noses are all associated with infancy in humans and with ‘kindchenschema’ effects in experimental studies (Sternglanz et al. Reference Sternglanz, Gray and Murakami1977; Hildebrandt & Fitzgerald Reference Hildebrandt and Fitzgerald1979; Alley Reference Alley1981; Berry & McArthur Reference Berry and McArthur1985; McKelvie Reference McKelvie1993; Glocker et al. Reference Glocker, Langleben, Ruparel, Loughead, Gur and Sachser2009). Additionally, some studies have found that eye shape and degree of eye separation (hypertelorism) can also influence people’s judgements, so these, as well as nose shape, were also included in our measurements (Berry & McArthur Reference Berry and McArthur1986; McKelvie Reference McKelvie1993; Hecht & Horowitz Reference Hecht and Horowitz2015).

Materials and methods

Measures of relative muzzle length

Two continuous measures of relative muzzle length based on existing, published datasets were used here (Georgevsky et al. Reference Georgevsky, Carrasco, Valenzuela and McGreevy2014; Packer et al. Reference Packer, Hendricks, Tivers and Burn2015b). Although these measures are fairly similar, we chose to make use of both. This was because some measurement differences exist between the two, and it is possible that these might have an impact on any relationships among muzzle-shortening and fronto-facial features. Most notably, the CFR incorporates forehead-doming into its measure, while the CI does not (Georgevsky et al. Reference Georgevsky, Carrasco, Valenzuela and McGreevy2014). The CI is calculated by dividing the anterior-posterior length of the skull by its width at its widest part (×100), producing a measure in which higher values represent shorter-muzzled, more brachycephalic, cranial morphologies (McGreevy et al. Reference McGreevy, Grassi and Harman2004, Reference McGreevy, Georgevsky, Carrasco, Valenzuela, Duffy and Serpell2013; Roberts et al. Reference Roberts, McGreevy and Valenzuela2010; Georgevsky et al. Reference Georgevsky, Carrasco, Valenzuela and McGreevy2014). Georgevsky and colleagues (Georgevsky et al. Reference Georgevsky, Carrasco, Valenzuela and McGreevy2014) calculated the CI of 960 dogs across 80 breeds (six adult males and six adult females per breed), taking the measurements from standardised images of live dogs photographed from above (transverse plane of head).

The CFR is calculated by using soft tape measurements of live dogs’ head dimensions, without photographs; Dividing the muzzle length by the cranial length produces a measure in which lower values represent shorter-muzzled, more brachycephalic morphologies (Sutter et al. Reference Sutter, Mosher, Gray and Ostrander2008). Packer et al. (Reference Packer, Hendricks and Burn2015a) calculated the CFR for 700 dogs of 97 breeds (varying numbers of exemplars per breed). For the current study, CFR means were calculated using data from dogs of six months of age and above (a majority subset of that used in Packer et al. Reference Packer, Hendricks, Tivers and Burn2015b). We make the assumption here that at six months of age, most dogs have largely adult head-shapes, although this remains to be confirmed empirically.

Measures of facial ‘look’: Fronto-facial ‘kindchenschema’ features

Twenty face-forward photographs of dogs from each breed considered here were obtained from internet searches using the search term “breed name face.” Two different search engines were used to avoid any possible bias associated with search algorithms; ten images per breed were harvested from Google and ten from Bing (globally, the two most popular search engines at the time of access, in January 2020). The images originated from a range of sources, including private postings, stock photos and commercial websites. Criteria for selecting images were: the whole dog face must be visible and of good quality (i.e. photograph not dark or blurred); the dog must be facing directly at the camera; the face could be tilted to the left or right, but not up/down; the top of the skull, the bottom of the nose and the top of the mouth were all visible; no distorted or manipulated images (e.g. with distortion lenses or obvious photo-shop modifications); no eyes closed; no extreme facial expressions (e.g. barking/growling/attacking); no puppies; no repeated individuals (i.e. none from same website or with identical coat patterns); no extreme or comical grooming styles.

A researcher, blind to the hypotheses of the study, measured the face and eye dimensions of each of the 20 images per breed, using Image J software (https://imagej.nih.gov/ij/). The measures that were taken are illustrated in Figure 2. From these, six variables (means per breed) were calculated: relative eye size (single eye aperture area/total face area), relative nose size (nose area/total face area), relative forehead size (forehead area/total face area), eye height/width ratio (i.e. indicating degree of eye-roundedness); nose height/width ratio (i.e. indicating degree of nose-roundedness); relative eye separation (distance between eyes/total face width). Areas were calculated assuming that eyes, faces and noses were ellipse-shaped (area = π (H/2) (W/2)). Details of the means of these measurements for each breed are given in Appendix 1. The full dataset is available from the corresponding author upon reasonable request. It was noted that, despite the wide range of source photographs, considerable within-breed homogeneity in relative feature sizes was found, with high correlations among exemplars of each breed, indicating considerable breed-level conformity among these facial features.

Figure 2. Measurements made on all dog face images, to calculate mean relative feature sizes for each breed: (a) face height; (b) face width; (c) forehead height; (d) eye height; (e) eye width; (f) nose height; (g) nose width; (h) distance between the eyes (to calculate relative distance between the eyes [h]/[b]).

Measures of breed size

To control for the possible confounding effects of overall breed size on the relationships among sagittal and coronal facial features, measures of estimated breed height were also included in our analyses. UK Kennel Club breed standard descriptions of breed height (including height ranges for male and female exemplars) were averaged to provide estimations of the size of each of the 42 breeds. This was supplemented by American Kennel Club breed standard information where UK estimates were not available (American Kennel Club 2021; The Kennel Club 2021).

Dog breeds included in the study

Both CI and CFR data are available for 57 breeds in total (Georgevsky et al. Reference Georgevsky, Carrasco, Valenzuela and McGreevy2014; Packer et al. Reference Packer, Hendricks, Tivers and Burn2015b). Of these, 15 were excluded from the present study because reasonable measurement of fronto-facial feature size from photographic images was not possible, due to excess hair or fur around the face (breeds excluded: Akita, Bearded collie, Bichon frise, Cairn terrier, Chinese crested, Griffon Bruxellois, Japanese spitz, Leonberger, Maltese, Norwegian elkhound, Old English sheepdog, Pomeranian, Shih tzu, Tibetan terrier, West Highland white terrier). Mean CI and CFR data for each of the remaining 42 breeds studied here are shown in Table 1.

Table 1. Relative muzzle length (CFR, CI) of the 42 breeds studied

Statistical analysis

The assumptions of parametric statistics were checked using normality and QQ plots. Pearson product moment correlational analyses were used to investigate associations among the dog breeds’ CI scores, CFR scores and estimated height, and among the fronto-facial measures calculated here (relative eye size, nose size and forehead size; eye and nose height/width ratio; relative eye separation). Following this, multiple linear regressions, using muzzle-shortening score (CFR or CI) and estimated breed size as potential predictor variables, were conducted for all six fronto-facial measures.

Results

Preliminary analyses showed that CI and CFR were highly correlated (this presented as an inverse correlation as low CFR values and high CI values are indicative of relatively shortened muzzles: r = –0.764, n = 42; P < 0.001).

Both CFR and CI also showed modest associations with estimated breed height, with smaller breeds in the present sample also having somewhat shorter muzzles. This association reached significance for CFR but not quite for CI, and the P < 0.05 level (CFR: r = 0.360, n = 42; P = 0.019; CI: r = –0.304, n = 42; P = 0.051).

The fronto-facial variables measured here showed a number of associations. Two key ‘kindchenschema’ measures, relative forehead size and relative eye size, were positively correlated (Table 2). Relative eye size (i.e. aperture area/face area) was also positively correlated with eye height/width ratio. Mean eye height/width ratio across the entire sample was 0.774 (indicating wider-than-tall, almond-shaped eyes for most dogs). These findings show that those breeds with relatively larger eyes also tend to have rounder, less almond-shaped eyes.

Table 2. Associations (Pearson product moment correlations) among fronto-facial ‘kindchenschema’ measures

Following Benjamini adjustments (Benjamini & Hochberg Reference Benjamini and Hochberg1995; Benjamini & Liu Reference Benjamini and Liu1999) for 15 tests, and using a False Discovery Rate (FDR) of 0.1;

* Significant correlations;

(*) correlations only significant without adjustments.

Similar to eye shape, the average nose height/width ratio was 0.760, indicating wider-than-tall, oval-shaped noses for most dogs. But, unlike relative eye size, relative nose size was not associated with nose shape, as described by this ratio. However, relative eye separation was inversely correlated with nose height/width ratio; those breeds with more widely separated eyes also had smaller nose height/width ratios, meaning that their noses were flatter and wider (see Table 2).

The results of multiple linear regressions, using relative muzzle length (Model 1 - CFR; Model 2 - CI) and estimated breed height (at withers) as predictor variables, are summarised in Table 3. Shorter-muzzled breeds had relatively larger forehead sizes and relatively larger eyes, but not significantly smaller noses (Figure 3). Smaller breeds (i.e. with lower estimated height at the withers) had relatively larger eyes and relatively smaller noses, but they did not have significantly larger foreheads (Figure 4). However, estimated breed height was significantly associated with eye height/width ratios, with smaller dogs having relatively taller, rounder eyes (Figure 5). Shorter-muzzled breeds did not have rounder eyes or rounder noses. Relative eye separation was not associated with estimated breed height, and not significantly associated with relative muzzle length following Benjamini adjustments for multiple testing (Benjamini & Hochberg Reference Benjamini and Hochberg1995; Benjamini & Liu Reference Benjamini and Liu1999).

Table 3. Multiple linear regression results for mean breed fronto-facial ‘kindchenschema’ measures, using relative muzzle length and estimated body height as predictors

Model 1 uses CFR as a predictor, and Model 2 uses CI as a predictor (note: smaller CFR values, and larger CI values, indicate relatively shorter muzzle size, so effects for these are oppositely signed). Following Benjamini adjustments (Benjamini & Hochberg Reference Benjamini and Hochberg1995; Benjamini & Liu Reference Benjamini and Liu1999) for 24 comparisons, and using a False Discovery Rate (FDR) of 0.1;

* Significant correlations;

(*) correlations only significant without adjustments.

Figure 3. (a) Mean breed cranio-facial ratio (CFR; Packer et al. Reference Packer, Hendricks and Burn2015a) plotted against relative forehead size, (b) mean breed cranio-facial ratio plotted against relative eye size (aperture area) and (c) mean breed cranio-facial ratio plotted against relative nose size. Smaller CFR values indicate a relatively shorter-muzzled, more brachycephalic cranial morphology.

Figure 4. (a) Estimated breed height at withers (The Kennel Club 2021) plotted against relative forehead size, (b) estimated breed height plotted against relative eye size (aperture area) and (c) estimated breed height plotted against relative nose size.

Figure 5. (a) Mean breed cranio-facial ratio (CFR; Packer et al. Reference Packer, Hendricks and Burn2015a) plotted against eye height/width ratio and (b) estimated breed height at withers (The Kennel Club 2021) plotted against eye height/width ratio. Higher eye height/width ratio values indicate taller, more rounded eye shapes (the mean eye shape across the whole sample is a wider-than-tall, almond shape) and smaller CFR values indicate a relatively shorter-muzzled, more brachycephalic cranial morphology.

Discussion

It is widely agreed that breed-related brachycephalia in dogs is a serious animal welfare problem (e.g. see Asher et al. Reference Asher, Diesel, Summers, McGreevy and Collins2009; Rooney & Sargan Reference Rooney and Sargan2009; Bateson Reference Bateson2016; Brachycephalic Working Group 2017, 2018; British Veterinary Association 2020a,b; O’Neill et al. Reference O’Neill, Sahota, Brodbelt, Church, Packer and Pegram2022), yet ownership of such breeds has increased dramatically across many countries in recent years (e.g. American Kennel Club 2020; Australian National Kennel Council 2020; The Kennel Club 2020). Many contemporary researchers and commentators have suggested that the current popularity of brachycephalic breeds of dog can be explained by the appealing, cute, ‘kindchenchema’ ‘look’ of these animals (Young & Bannasch Reference Young, Bannasch, Ostrander, Giger and Lindblad-Toh2006; Packer et al. Reference Packer, O’Neill, Fletcher and Farnworth2019, Reference Packer, O’Neill, Fletcher and Farnworth2020). But this possibility has never previously been investigated. Smaller mid-face regions and larger, more prominent foreheads in (front-of-face) images of humans and animals tend to be regarded as cute (and have been found to be preferred, and perceived as more baby-like, by adult observers: Gardner & Wallach Reference Gardner and Wallach1965; Alley Reference Alley1981; Berry & McArthur Reference Berry and McArthur1985; see also Figure 1 above). But whether or not breed-related sagittal plane muzzle shortening (i.e. the shorter-muzzled, brachycephalic conformation, in profile) in dogs is also associated with infantile ‘kindchenschema’ features in the coronal plane (i.e. the front of the face), has remained an open question.

We hypothesised that relative muzzle shortening across a range of popular dog breeds would be associated with a more infantile look when viewed face-on: a relatively large (taller) forehead, large eyes, small nose, round (taller) eyes and nose, and widely spaced eyes (Sternglanz et al. Reference Sternglanz, Gray and Murakami1977; Hildebrandt & Fitzgerald Reference Hildebrandt and Fitzgerald1979; Alley Reference Alley1981; Berry & McArthur Reference Berry and McArthur1985; McKelvie Reference McKelvie1993; Glocker et al. Reference Glocker, Langleben, Ruparel, Loughead, Gur and Sachser2009; Hecht & Horowitz Reference Hecht and Horowitz2015). We found that, based on visible facial features depicted in a large sample of internet-sourced photographs, our hypothesis was confirmed in two respects. The shorter-muzzled breeds included in this study had relatively larger (taller) foreheads and relatively larger eyes (i.e. larger eye aperture area, in relation to overall face area), but not smaller noses (Figure 3). Trends towards rounder, less almond-shaped eyes and more widely separated eyes among shorter-muzzled breeds did not reach statistical significance. In sum, the present study was able to confirm that muzzle shortening is associated with some, but not all, aspects of a coronal-plane ‘kindchenschem’ ‘look’ among contemporary dog breeds.

Inclusion of estimated breed height in our models enabled us to conclude that associations found between muzzle shortening and forehead and eye aperture area could not be simply explained by the small size of some short-muzzled breeds. Indeed, although many popular, brachycephalic breeds are also small dogs (e.g. Pug, Chihuahua), this type of facial conformation can also be found in larger breeds (e.g. Boxer, Bull mastiff). Furthermore, the sample considered here showed no (inverse) correlation between breed height and CI (Georgevsky et al. Reference Georgevsky, Carrasco, Valenzuela and McGreevy2014), and only a small positive correlation between height and CFR (Packer et al. Reference Packer, Hendricks, Tivers and Burn2015b).

Why is muzzle-shortening associated with fronto-facial features?

The dataset used here describes only the ‘look’ of different breeds, from photographs, not their underlying, three-dimensional structure (i.e. skeletal and soft-tissue composition), so it is not possible to provide a definitive anatomical explanation as to why muzzle shortening in the sagittal plane, and the coronal-plane features of forehead size and eye size, are associated across the breeds studied here. Additional investigations of live dogs (or cadavers) will be needed to document any morphological relationships among different planes of the canine head, and to potentially link these with key genetic mutations (Young & Bannasch Reference Young, Bannasch, Ostrander, Giger and Lindblad-Toh2006; Drake & Klingenberg Reference Drake and Klingenberg2010; Parr et al. Reference Parr, Wilson, Wroe, Colman, Crowther and Letnic2016; Curth et al. Reference Curth, Fischer and Kupczik2017). Nevertheless, it is possible to speculate on the human factors associated with why the ‘cute’, ‘kindchenschema’ features of sagittal-plane muzzle shortening, and the relatively larger eye and forehead sizes seen in the coronal plane, appear to co-occur across breeds.

First, it is possible that brachycephalic dogs have been selected by humans to look cute and appealing, perhaps unconsciously, to fulfil or enhance a desire for nurturance among owners. If so, then this domestic selection pressure may have acted across multiple morphological features, including eye size, forehead size and muzzle length. Such a scenario could be expected if certain types of dog have been used specifically to function as infant- or child-like companions; to look cute and to stimulate nurturant feelings and behaviour in their owners (Serpell Reference Serpell2002; Waller et al. Reference Waller, Peirce, Caeiro, Scheider, Burrows, McCune and Kaminski2013; Borgi & Cirulli Reference Borgi and Cirulli2016; Serpell Reference Serpell2019; see also Serpell Reference Serpell1996). If this is the case, then it is possible that genetic control of each of these features may be wholly or partially independent, having been modified together simply as a result of having been selected together (for discussions of modularity in cranial features of the dog, see: Parr et al. Reference Parr, Wilson, Wroe, Colman, Crowther and Letnic2016; Curth et al. Reference Curth, Fischer and Kupczik2017). There may, therefore, be future possibilities for breeding dogs in whom some appealing, infantile features could be maintained, while clinically problematic muzzle shortening in the sagittal plane of the skull is selected against. Of course, this raises the wider question of exactly how much facial features can be genetically modified to achieve some degree of ‘kindchenschema’ effect, whilst also maintaining (or preferably, maximising) conformational health. In other words, how cute is too cute (Packer Reference Packer2013)? More specifically, for example, how short can a dog’s muzzle be before brachycephalic obstructive airway syndrome (BOAS) occurs (Packer et al. Reference Packer, Hendricks, Tivers and Burn2015b), how large can eyes be before clinical pathologies such as corneal disease develops (Packer et al. Reference Packer, Hendricks and Burn2015a), and how high or domed can a forehead be before neurological disorders, such as syringomyelia, emerge (Mitchell et al. Reference Mitchell, Knowler, van den Berg, Sykes and Rusbridge2014)?

A second possibility is that only a single facial feature, or subset of features, has been selected for by breeders, and the others have also changed as a result of morphological linkage, i.e. by a process of co-selection. For example, a number of contemporary brachycephalic breeds are thought to have originated as fighting dogs (e.g. Bulldogs; Ellis et al. Reference Ellis, Thomason, Kebteab, Zubair and France2009; American Kennel Club 2017). Those individuals with shorter muzzles may have initially been favoured for breeding because they conferred an advantage in the pit or ring (Young & Bannasch Reference Young, Bannasch, Ostrander, Giger and Lindblad-Toh2006). So, if dogs with short muzzles were selectively bred for this (or other) reasons, it is possible that as their muzzles became shorter, this change also gave rise to a wider suite of ‘kindchenschema’-like features in the coronal plane of their faces. One version of this possibility has been expressed as the neotenisation hypothesis; the proposal that selection for one particular morphology or ‘look’ may have affected development of the whole cranium. For example, some of the genetic mutations harnessed to produce brachycephalic dogs may influence head and face morphology via the entire developmental progression of cranial growth (Pittenger & Shaw Reference Pittenger and Shaw1975; Todd et al. Reference Todd, Mark, Shaw and Pittenger1980; Frank & Frank Reference Frank and Frank1982; Coppinger & Smith Reference Coppinger and Smith1983; Jones Reference Jones1995; although see also Drake Reference Drake2011). Currently, and while remaining a popular view, this hypothesis remains unproven. However, if such a co-selection account is correct, then any amount of future re-breeding may fail to separate the cute, coronal-plane ‘kindchenschema’ appearance from clinically problematic muzzle shortening.

Sources of variation in skull morphology across contemporary dog breeds appears to rely on the contributions of mutations from many different quantitative trait loci (Schoenebeck et al. Reference Schoenebeck, Hutchinson, Byers, Beale, Carrington, Faden, Rimbault, Decker, Kidd, Sood, Boyko, Fondon, Wayne, Bustamante, Ciruna and Ostrander2012; Bannasch et al. Reference Bannasch, Baes and Leeb2020). For example, Bannasch et al. (Reference Bannasch, Baes and Leeb2020) identified strong genome-wide associations for brachycephalic head type on the Cfa1 gene, using comparisons of strongly brachycephalic, compared with non-brachycephalic dogs. Others have found additional mutations to be associated with brachycephalic breeds, including in the gene BMP3, which has been shown to function in cranial development in a wide range of species (Schoenebeck et al. Reference Schoenebeck, Hutchinson, Byers, Beale, Carrington, Faden, Rimbault, Decker, Kidd, Sood, Boyko, Fondon, Wayne, Bustamante, Ciruna and Ostrander2012), and a mutation in the DISHEVELLED2 (DVL2) gene, apparently associated with vertebral and skull malformations in Bulldogs, French bulldogs and Boston terriers (Mansour et al. Reference Mansour, Lucot, Konopelski, Dickinson, Sturges, Vernau, Choi, Stern, Thomasy, Doring, Verstraete, Johnson, York, Rebhun, Ho, Brown and Bannasch2018; Niskanen et al. Reference Niskanen, Reunanen, Salonen, Bannasch, Lappalainen, Lohi and Hytonen2021). Reduced muzzle length is a breed-defining characteristic in most of these dogs, as can be seen from kennel club breed standards (Asher et al. Reference Asher, Diesel, Summers, McGreevy and Collins2009; Packer et al. Reference Packer, Hendricks, Tivers and Burn2015b), and it appears that the degree of muzzle shortening in these breeds has increased across the past 50 to 100 years (Young & Bannasch Reference Young, Bannasch, Ostrander, Giger and Lindblad-Toh2006; see also Knowler et al. Reference Knowler, Gillstedt, Mitchell, Jovanovik, Volk and Rusbridge2019; Skipper Reference Skipper, Packer and O’Neill2021). But the relative contribution of all of these different genes to the coronal-plane ‘kindchenschema’ facial features are, as yet, unexplored. It is possible that further investigations of sagittal- and coronal-plane feature associations will find variance according to breed clades and their associated mutations.

Differences in relative muzzle length, as measured by CFR and by CI

Differences between breeds in the precise forms that muzzle shortening can take is highlighted by differences between the CFR and CI measures. It has previously been noted that muzzle shortening across dog breeds does not represent a single morphological process, with the sagittal shapes of different short-muzzled breeds varying considerably (e.g. Georgevsky et al. Reference Georgevsky, Carrasco, Valenzuela and McGreevy2014; Geiger et al. Reference Geiger, Schoenenbeck, Schneider, Schmidt, Fischer and Sanchez-Villagra2021). Perhaps the most dramatic illustration of this can be seen when two brachycephalic dogs, the Chihuahua and the Pug, are compared. The Chihuahua (especially the ‘apple-headed’ sub-type) has a small but domed and protruding forehead, while the Pug’s forehead profile is an almost continuous rearward slant from nose to ears. Measuring relative muzzle lengths across breeds using the CFR and CI methods yields highly correlated datasets but the two methods do differentiate some morphological variation, including this contrast between the Pug and the Chihuahua. Specifically, the soft-tape method of the CFR captures the roundedness of the Chihuahua forehead, effectively reducing the ‘relative muzzle length’ measure as a result (i.e. using the CFR method exaggerates the estimation of muzzle shortness of Chihuahuas, in comparison with the CI). In the current analyses, the shorter-muzzled breeds were found to have relatively large eye aperture areas, but this significant finding related only to muzzle length measured by CFR (Packer et al. Reference Packer, Hendricks, Tivers and Burn2015b). It is possible, therefore, that the forehead doming in some breeds, such as the Chihuahua, plays an additional role in determining this association. For future research (e.g. both further ‘kindchenschema’ investigations, and morphological studies designed to seek associations with clinical disease), additional measures of muzzle shape (e.g. cranio-facial angle; Regodon et al. Reference Regodon, Vivo, Franco, Guillen and Robina1993) and more specific measures of forehead doming, in addition to relative muzzle length, may prove informative (see also Brehm et al. Reference Brehm, Loeffler and Komeyli1985).

Breed height and ‘kindchenschema’ features

Estimated breed height was positively correlated with relative nose size (smaller breeds had smaller noses, relative to overall face size), and inversely correlated with relative eye aperture area (smaller breeds had larger eyes, relative to overall face size) (Figure 4). Smaller breeds also had somewhat rounder, less almond-shaped eyes than larger breeds (Figure 5). However, no associations were found between breed height and relative forehead size. In sum, short-statured dog breeds showed some ‘kindchenschema’, ‘cute’ features in the coronal plane of the face, but these differed, to a degree, from those seen among short-muzzled dogs.

As with muzzle shortening, it is possible to speculate about the origins of these facial-feature linkages in the selective breeding of relatively small and large breeds of dog. Of all domesticated animals, dogs are notable for their wide variability of stature, with selection for smaller and shorter breeds being associated with a number of breed clades and (original) breed functions (Bannasch et al. Reference Bannasch, Baes and Leeb2020; Geiger et al. Reference Geiger, Schoenenbeck, Schneider, Schmidt, Fischer and Sanchez-Villagra2021). But while the small size of some short-limbed dogs can be explained by the effects of a single-gene mutation (e.g. FGF4 in Dachshunds; Parker et al. Reference Parker, VonHoldt, Quignon, Margulies, Shao, Mosher, Spadey, Elkahloun, Cargill, Jones, Maslen, Acland, Sutter, Kuroki, Bustamante, Wayne and Ostrander2009), breed size across much of the contemporary dog population is a smoothly continuous variable that is likely to have multiple genetic sources (Bannasch et al. Reference Bannasch, Baes and Leeb2020). We can hypothesise that selecting for small, ‘cute’ lapdogs may, like breeding for shortened muzzles, have had direct (independent) effects on a range of features, and/or linked effects across genetically associated features. Relatively large eyes, for example, which were associated in the present sample with both muzzle shortening and short stature, may represent concurrent selection for a known ‘kindchenschema’ feature (i.e. one that elicits preference and cuteness attributions in observers; Kringelbach et al. Reference Kringelbach, Stark, Alexander, Bornstein and Stein2016). However, it is also possible that smaller dogs have relatively large (and rounder) eyes because of a mismatch between the miniaturisation of the skeleton (and in particular the eye socket) and of the soft tissue of the eyes and themselves, resulting in larger, more protruding eyeballs (Geiger et al. Reference Geiger, Schoenenbeck, Schneider, Schmidt, Fischer and Sanchez-Villagra2021). This may be particularly distinctive of domestic breeding (i.e. as compared with miniaturisation resulting from natural selection), resulting from the rapid selection and crossing that some breeds have undergone (e.g. see Parker et al. Reference Parker, Dreger, Rimbault, Davis, Mullen, Carpintero-Ramirez and Ostrander2017), and the relative lack of functional selection pressures (Drake & Klingenberg Reference Drake and Klingenberg2010). For example, among wild species where body size reductions have occurred (e.g. as a result of island miniaturisation; Lomolino Reference Lomolino2005), similar mismatches between skeletal size and eye size do not appear to occur, although extensive research in this area is lacking. Among contemporary dogs, whether certain breed clades show more or less skeletal/soft tissue disproportionality than others, and whether this is associated with greater/lesser vulnerability to particular clinical disorders and consequent welfare problems, remain important questions for future morphological research (e.g. Parker et al. Reference Parker, Dreger, Rimbault, Davis, Mullen, Carpintero-Ramirez and Ostrander2017).

Previous research (McGreevy et al. Reference McGreevy, Grassi and Harman2004) has shown that the volume of the canine eye varies with dog size more than was first thought. The authors reported a correlation between skull and eye size, refuting previous suggestions that eye radius in dogs is a fairly constant 11 mm (Coppinger & Schneider Reference Coppinger, Schneider, Coppinger and Schneider1995), but that instead it ranged from 9.6 to 11.6 mm. This supports the suggestion that large dogs have large eyes (Peichl Reference Peichl1992). The same study (McGreevy et al. Reference McGreevy, Grassi and Harman2004) confirmed that eyelid apertures vary considerably in their orientation within the skull, with the angle of orientation, sub-tended by their midlines, varying from 10° (very frontally placed eyelid apertures) to 50° (more laterally placed eye apertures). These angles were found to correlate with the skull length and cephalic index and, to a lesser degree, skull width. However, a regression analysis revealed that the predictor for eyelid apertures’ orientation was cephalic index and not skull width and length.

Relative eye separation

An additional facial feature considered here was relative eye separation. Although the appearance of widely separated eyes (hypertelorism) has not been extensively studied as a ‘kindchenschema’ feature, it is consistent with the infantile profile of a relatively enlarged cranial vault. In addition, a recent experimental study on the effects of ‘kindchenschema’ features on people’s judgements of animal photographs found that eye separation can increase preferences for manipulated images of dogs’ faces (Hecht & Horowitz Reference Hecht and Horowitz2015). However, in the current study, breed-level variation in relative eye separation was not significantly associated with shorter relative muzzle lengths (or smaller breed height; Table 3). This was somewhat surprising, given that wide eye-placement has been found to be linked with a key mutation present in a number of brachycephalic dog breeds. Mansour et al. (Reference Mansour, Lucot, Konopelski, Dickinson, Sturges, Vernau, Choi, Stern, Thomasy, Doring, Verstraete, Johnson, York, Rebhun, Ho, Brown and Bannasch2018) conducted whole genome sequencing across 21 breeds, identifying a shift mutation in the WNT pathway gene DVL2 in three of those breeds: Bulldogs, French bulldogs and Boston terriers. In our dataset of 42 breeds, the top ten with greatest relative eye separation were: Boston terrier, Great Dane, Bull terrier, English springer spaniel, French bulldog, Bulldog, Boxer, Weimaraner, Gordon setter, Dachshund (miniature). So, although a number of these breeds showing the greatest eye separation are brachycephalic (and the three known DVL2 carrier breeds are in the top ten), they were also intermixed with others with much longer muzzles. While again noting that relative eye separation in the current study was based on measurement of photographs, rather than actual skeletal measurements, this finding suggests a mixed provenance of the eye separation trait, with the possibility that several different genes, and consequent skeletal morphologies, influence variation in eye separation across breeds, and that not all of these are associated with brachycephalia.

The only other facial feature that was associated with relative eye separation in the present study was nose height/width ratio; specifically, breeds with more widely separated eyes had relatively wider, more oval-shaped noses in the sample of images measured here (Table 2). This finding is likely to be indicative of increased eye separation being associated with a more general widening of the midface region of the skull. This is not unexpected; in humans diagnosed with orbital hypertelorism, for example, associated conditions include median facial clefting, resulting in a wide, flattened nose area (Benacerraf et al. Reference Benacerraf, Bromley and Jelin2019).

Animal welfare implications

A number of commentators have attributed the current, extreme popularity of brachycephalic dogs to the attractive qualities of their ‘cute’, ‘kindchenschema’, facial features (e.g. Tuan Reference Tuan1984; Serpell Reference Serpell1996; McGreevy et al. Reference McGreevy, Georgevsky, Carrasco, Valenzuela, Duffy and Serpell2013; Packer et al. Reference Packer, Murphy and Farnworth2017; Sandøe et al. Reference Sandøe, Kondrup, Bennett, Forkman, Meyer, Proschowsky, Serpell and Lund2017; O’Neill et al. Reference O’Neill, Sahota, Brodbelt, Church, Packer and Pegram2022), and the present findings are not inconsistent with that view. The hypothesis that selection for nuturance is an important driving force in the development of companion dog ownership (and pet-keeping more broadly) is certainly one that must be taken seriously (Serpell Reference Serpell1996); if companion dogs are selected across time to have increasingly cute looks (both within breeds, and between breeds in the sense of increased breed-share of the whole owned-dog population), then in the future, welfare problems with brachycephalia and other associated morphologies are destined to grow. Nevertheless, many details of this nurturance hypothesis are still to be fleshed out, and many other factors may be at play in determining the popularity of brachycephalic breeds (e.g. Bognár et al. Reference Bognár, Szabó, Deés and Kubinyi2021). Moving forward to consider the future popularity of cute-looking, short-muzzled dogs, it is important to note that the ‘kindchenschema’ effect shows some cultural dependence, with owners in different countries varying in their preference for brachycephalic pets (see Farnworth et al. Reference Farnworth, Packer, Sordo, Chen, Caney and Gunn-Moore2018 regarding cat breed preferences; see also Archer & Monton Reference Archer and Monton2011 regarding the effects of pet ownership). These findings, combined with the fact that many non-brachycephalic dogs also remain highly popular as pets (Brincat et al. Reference Brincat, McGreevy, Bowell and Packer2022), indicate that people’s preferences do vary, and ‘kindchenschema’ features are far from being the only determinant of breed preference. Cultural fashions come and go, so something that seems like a permanent change or progression at one point in history may be only a short-lived trend (Herzog & Elias Reference Herzog and Elias2004; Herzog et al. Reference Herzog, Bently and Hahn2004; Herzog Reference Herzog2006; Ghirlanda et al. Reference Ghirlanda, Acerbi, Herzog and Serpell2013, Reference Ghirlanda, Acerbi and Herzog2014; Skipper Reference Skipper, Packer and O’Neill2021). Further social and psychological research is needed to establish how important facial ‘kindchenschema’ features are to prospective dog owners, and how an understanding of the clinical and welfare consequences of extremely short muzzles may be used to modify pet ownership and breeding practices in the future.

Supplementary Materials

To view supplementary material for this article, please visit http://doi.org/10.1017/awf.2022.6.

Acknowledgements

The authors would like to thank Professor Mike Mendl for some helpful discussions regarding the content of this paper.

Competing interests

None.

Footnotes

Author contributions: Conceptualisation: VN, EP; Data curation: EM, EC, PMcG, RMAP, EP; Formal analysis: EP, VN; Methodology: EP, VN, PMcG, RMAP; Visualisation: EM; Writing – original draft: EP; Writing – review and editing: PMcG, RMAP, VN.

References

Alley, TR 1981 Head shape and the perception of cuteness. Developmental Psychology 17(5): 650654. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.17.5.650Google Scholar
Almanza-Sepdulveda, ML, Dudin, A, Wonch, KE, Steiner, M, Feinberg, DR, Fleming, AS and Hall, GB 2018 Exploring the morphological and emotional correlates of infant cuteness. Infant Behavior and Development 53: 90100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.infbeh.2018.08.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
American Kennel Club 2017 The New Complete Dog Book, 22nd Edition. Fox Chapel Publishing: Mount Joy, PA, USAGoogle Scholar
American Kennel Club 2020 Top 10 Most popular Breeds of 2018. https://www.akc.org/expert-advice/news/most-popular-dog-breeds-of-2018/Google Scholar
American Kennel Club 2021 https://www.akc.org/Google Scholar
Archer, J and Monton, S 2011 Preferences for infant facial features in pet dogs and cats. Ethology 117(3): 217226. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2010.01863.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Asher, L, Diesel, G, Summers, JF, McGreevy, PD and Collins, LM 2009 Inherited defects in pedigree dogs. Part 1: Disorders related to breed standards. Veterinary Journal 182(3): 402411. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tvjl.2009.08.033CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Australian National Kennel Council 2020 National Registration Analysis 2010-2019. http://ankc.org.au/media/9303/rego-stats-list_2010-2019v4.pdfGoogle Scholar
Bannasch, DL, Baes, C and Leeb, T 2020 Genetic variants affecting skeletal morphology in domestic dogs. Trends in Genetics 36(8): 598609. https://doi.org/10.1016/tig.2020.05.005Google ScholarPubMed
Bateson, P 2016 Independent Inquiry into Dog Breeding. University of Cambridge, UK. https://www.ourdogs.co.uk/special/final-dog-inquiry-120110.pdfGoogle Scholar
Benacerraf, BR, Bromley, BS, Jelin, AC and Society for Maternal-Fetal Medicine 2019 Hypertelorism: SMFM Consult Series. American Journal of Obstetrics and Gynecology 211(5): B18B19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajog.2019.08.053Google Scholar
Benjamini, Y and Hochberg, Y 1995 Controlling the false discovery rate – a practical and powerful approach to multiple testing. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society B: StatisticalMethodology 57(1): 289300. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2517-6161.1995.tb02031.xGoogle Scholar
Benjamini, Y and Liu, W 1999 A step-down multiple hypotheses testing procedure that controls the false discovery rate under independence. Journal of Statistical Planning and Inference 82(1-2): 163170. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0378-3758(99)00040-3Google Scholar
Berry, DS and McArthur, LZ 1985 Some components and consequences of a baby face. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 48(2): 312323. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.48.2.312CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berry, DS and McArthur, LZ 1986 Perceiving character in faces – the impact of age-related craniofacial changes on social-perception. Psychological Bulletin 100(1): 318. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.100.1.3Google ScholarPubMed
Bognár, Z, Szabó, D, Deés, and Kubinyi, E 2021 Shorter headed dogs, visually cooperative breeds, younger and playful dogs form eye contact faster with an unfamiliar human. Scientific Reports 11: 929310.1038/s41598-021-88702-wCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borgi, M and Cirulli, F 2016 Pet face: Mechanisms underlying human-animal relationships. Frontiers in Psychology 7: e298. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00298CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Borgi, M, Cogliati-Dezza, I, Brelsford, V, Meints, K and Cirulli, F 2014 Baby schema in human and animal faces induces cuteness perception and gaze allocation in children. Frontiers in Psychology 5: e411. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00411CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brachycephalic Working Group 2017 Framework for a partnership approach to improving brachycephalic dog health and welfare. file:///Users/lvesp/Downloads/UK%20Brachycephalic%20Working%20Group%20-%20Framework%20for%20a%20partnership%20approach%20N….pdfGoogle Scholar
Brachycephalic Working Group 2018 Open letter from the Brachycephalic Working Group A request to companies to avoid using ‘flat faced’ (brachycephalic) dogs in marketing and advertising. http://www.ukbwg.org.uk/wp-content/uploads/2018/05/Brachycephalic-Working-Group-Request-to-companies-to-avoid-using-%E2%80%98flat-faced%E2%80%99-brachycephalic-dogs-in-advertising.pdfGoogle Scholar
Brehm, H, Loeffler, K and Komeyli, H 1985 The shapes of the canine skull. Anatomy, Histology and Embryology 14: 324331CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brincat, BL, McGreevy, PD, Bowell, VA and Packer, RMA 2022 Who’s getting a head start? Mesocephalic dogs in still images are attributed more positively valenced emotions than dogs of other cephalic index groups. Animals 12: 49. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani12010049CrossRefGoogle Scholar
British Veterinary Association 2020a Animals should be bred for health over looks. https://www.bva.co.uk/take-action/breed-to-breathe-campaign/Google Scholar
British Veterinary Association 2020b Our policies: Brachycephalic dogs. https://www.bva.co.uk/take-action/our-policies/brachycephalic-dogs/Google Scholar
Brosch, T, Sander, D and Scherer, KR 2007 That baby caught my eye… Attention capture by infant faces. Emotion 7(3): 685689. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.3.685CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chersini, N, Hall, NJ and Wynne, CDL 2018 Dog pups’ attractiveness to humans peaks a weaning age. Anthrozoos 31(3): 309318. https://doi.org/10.1080/08927936.2018.1455454CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coppinger, RP and Smith, CK 1983 The domestication of evolution. Environmental Conservation 10(4): 283292. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0376892900012984CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coppinger, R and Schneider, R 1995 Evolution of working dogs. In: Coppinger, R and Schneider, (Eds.) The Domestic Dog pp 2150. Cambridge University Press: Cambridge, UKGoogle Scholar
Curth, S, Fischer, MS and Kupczik, K 2017 Patterns of integration in the canine skull: an inside view into the relationship of the skull modules of domestic dogs and wolves. Zoology 125: 19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.zool.2017.06.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drake, AG 2011 Dispelling dog dogma: an investigation of heterochrony in dogs using 3D geometric morphometric analysis of skull shape. Evolution and Development 13(2): 204213. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1525-142X.2011.00470.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Drake, AG and Klingenberg, CP 2010 Large scale diversification of skull shape in domestic dogs: Disparity and modularity. American Naturalist 175(3): 289301. https://doi.org/10.1086/650372CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellis, JL, Thomason, J, Kebteab, E, Zubair, K and France, J 2009 Cranial dimensions and forces of biting in the domestic dog. Journal of Anatomy 214(3): 362373. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7580.2008.01042.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Esposito, G, Nakazawa, J, Ogawa, S, Stival, R, Putnick, DL and Bornstein, MH 2015 Using infrared thermography to assess emotional responses to infants. Early Child Development and Care 185(3): 438447. https://doi.org/10.1080/03004430.2014.932153CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Farnworth, MJ, Packer, RMA, Sordo, L, Chen, RN, Caney, SMA and Gunn-Moore, DA 2018 In the eye of the beholder: Owner preferences for variations in cats’ appearances with specific focus on skill morphology. Animals 8(2): e30. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani8020030CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fawcett, A, Barrs, V, Awad, M, Child, G, Brunel, L, Mooney, E, Martinez-Taboada, F, McDonald, B and McGreevy, P 2019 Consequences and management of canine brachycephaly in veterinary practice: perspectives from Australian veterinarians and veterinary specialists. Animals 9(1): e3. https://doi.org/10.3390/ani9010003Google Scholar
Frank, H and Frank, MG 1982 On the effects of domestication on canine social-development and behaviour. Applied Animal Ethology 8(6): 507525. https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-3762(82)90215-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fullard, W and Reiling, AM 1976 Investigation of Lorenz’s babyness. Child Development 47(4): 11911193. https://doi.org/10.2307/1128462CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, BT and Wallach, L 1965 Shapes of figures identified as a baby’s head. Perceptual and Motor Skills 20(1): 135142. https://doi.org/10.2466/pms.1965.20.1.135CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geiger, M, Schoenenbeck, JJ, Schneider, RA, Schmidt, MJ, Fischer, MS and Sanchez-Villagra, MR 2021 Exceptional changes in skeletal anatomy under domestication: The case of brachycephaly. Integrative Organismal Biology 3: 131. https://doi.org/10.1093/iob/obab023CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Georgevsky, D, Carrasco, JJ, Valenzuela, M and McGreevy, PD 2014 Domestic dog skull diversity across breeds, breed groupings, and genetic clusters. Journal of Veterinary Behavior-Clinical Applications and Research 9(5): 228234. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2014.04.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ghirlanda, S, Acerbi, A, Herzog, H and Serpell, JA 2013 Fashion vs Function in cultural evolution: The case of dog breed popularity. PLOS One 8(9): e74770. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0074770Google ScholarPubMed
Ghirlanda, S, Acerbi, A and Herzog, H 2014 Dog movie stars and dog breed popularity: A case study in media influence on choice. PLOS One 9(9): e106565. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0106565CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glocker, ML, Langleben, DD, Ruparel, K, Loughead, JW, Gur, RC and Sachser, N 2009 Baby schema in infant faces induces cuteness perception and motivation for caretaking in adults. Ethology 115(3): 257263. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1439-0310.2008.01603.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Golle, J, Lisibach, S, Mast, FW and Lobmaier, JS 2013 Sweet puppies and cute babies: perceptual adaptation to babyfacedness transfers across species. PLOS One 8(3): e58248. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0058248CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hecht, J and Horowitz, A 2015 Seeing dogs: Human preferences for dog physical attributes. Anthrozoos 28(1): 153163. https://doi.org/10.2752/089279315x14129350722217CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herzog, HA 2006 Forty two thousand and one dalmations: Fads, social contagion, and dog breed popularity. Society and Animals 14: 383397. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853006778882448CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Herzog, HA and Elias, SM 2004 Effects of winning the Westminster Kennel Club Dog Show on breed popularity. Journal of the American Veterinary Medical Association 225(3): 365367. https://doi.org/10.2460/javma.2004.225.365CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Herzog, HA, Bently, RA and Hahn, MW 2004 Random drift and large shifts in popularity of dog breeds. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B (Suppl) 271: S353S356. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2004.0185Google ScholarPubMed
Hildebrandt, KA and Fitzgerald, HE 1979 Facial feature determinants of perceived infant attractiveness. Infant Behavior and Development 2(4): 329339. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0163-6383(79)80043-0CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, D 1995 Sexual selection, physical attractiveness, and facial neoteny – cross-cultural evidence and implications. Current Anthropology 36(5): 723748. https://doi.org/10.1086/204427CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knowler, SP, Gillstedt, L, Mitchell, TJ, Jovanovik, J, Volk, HA and Rusbridge, C 2019 Pilot Study of head conformation changes over time in the Cavalier King Charles spaniel breed. Veterinary Record 184(4). https://doi.org/10.1136/vr105135CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kringelbach, ML, Stark, EA, Alexander, C, Bornstein, MH and Stein, A 2016 On cuteness: Unlocking the parental brain and beyond. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 20(7): 545558. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.05.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lacruz, RS, Stringer, CB, Kimbel, WH, Wood, B, Harvati, K, O’Higgins, P, Bromage, TG and Arsuaga, JL 2019 The evolutionary history of the human face. Nature Ecology and Evolution 3(5): 726736. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41559-019-0865-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lehmann, V, Huis in‘t Veld, EMJ and Vingerhoets, A 2013 The human and animal baby schema effect: Correlates of individual differences. Behavioural Processes 94: 99108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.beproc.2013.01.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liu, NC, Troconis, EL, Kalmar, L, Price, DJ, Wright, HE, Adams, VJ, Sargan, DR and Ladlow, JF 2017 Conformational risk factors of brachycephalic obstructive airway syndrome (BOAS) in pugs, French bulldogs, and bulldogs. PLoS One 12(8): https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0181928Google ScholarPubMed
Lomolino, MV 2005 Body size evolution in insular vertebrates: generality of the island rule. Journal of Biogeography 32(10): 16831699. https://doi.org/10.111/j.1365-2699.2005.01314.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lorenz, K 1943 Die angeborenen Formen moglicher Erfahrung. Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie (5): 235409. [Title translation: The innate forms of possible experience]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lorenz, K 1971 Studies in Animal and Human Behaviour, Volume II. Harvard University Press: Cambridge, MA, USAGoogle Scholar
Mansour, TA, Lucot, K, Konopelski, SE, Dickinson, PJ, Sturges, BK, Vernau, KL, Choi, S, Stern, JA, Thomasy, SM, Doring, S, Verstraete, FJM, Johnson, EG, York, D, Rebhun, RB, Ho, H-YH, Brown, CT and Bannasch, DL 2018 Whole genome variant association across 100 dogs identifies a frame shift mutation in DISHEVELLED 2 which contributes to Robinow-like syndrome in Bulldogs and related screw tail dog breeds. PLoS Genetics 14(12): e1007850. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1007850CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGreevy, PD, Georgevsky, D, Carrasco, J, Valenzuela, M, Duffy, DL and Serpell, JA 2013 Dog behavior co-varies with height, bodyweight and skull shape. PLoS One 8(12): e80529. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0080529CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGreevy, P, Grassi, TD and Harman, AM 2004 A strong correlation exists between the distribution of retinal ganglion cells and nose length in the dog. Brain Behavior and Evolution 63(1): 1322. https://doi.org/10.1159/000073756Google Scholar
McKelvie, SJ 1993 Effects of feature variations on attributions for schematic faces. Psychological Reports 73(1): 275288. https://doi.org/10.2466/pr0.1993.73.1.275CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitchell, TJ, Knowler, SP, van den Berg, H, Sykes, J and Rusbridge, C 2014 Syringomyelia: determining risk and protective factors in the conformation of the Cavalier King Charles Spaniel dog. Canine Genetics and Epidemiology 1 : 9. https://doi.org/10.1186/2052-6687-1-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Niskanen, JE, Reunanen, V, Salonen, M, Bannasch, D, Lappalainen, AK, Lohi, H and Hytonen, MK 2021 Canine DVL2 variant contributes to brachycephalic phenotype and caudal vertebral anomalies. Human Genetics 140(11): 15351545. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00439-021-02261-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Neill, DG, Brodbelt, DC, Church, DB and Sanchez, RF 2017 Corneal ulcerative disease in dogs under primary veterinary care in England: epidemiology and management. Canine Genetics and Epidemiology 4(1): 5. https://doi.org/ 10.1136/vr.104108CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Neill, DG, Jackson, C, Guy, JH, Church, DB, McGreevy, PD, Thompson, PC and Brodbelt, D 2015 Epidemiological associations between brachycephaly and upper respiratory tract disorders in dogs attending veterinary practices in England. Canine Genetics and Epidemiology 2: 10. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40575-015-0023-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Neill, DG, Packer, RMA, Lobb, M, Church, DB, Brodbelt, DC and Pegram, C 2020 Demography and commonly recorded clinical conditions of Chihuahuas under primary veterinary care in the UK in 2016. BMC Veterinary Research 16(1): 42. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12917-020-2258-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Neill, DG, Sahota, J, Brodbelt, DC, Church, DB, Packer, RMA and Pegram, C 2022 Health of pug dogs in the UK: disorder predispositions and protections. Canine Medicine and Genetics 9: 4. https://doi.org/10.1186/s40575-022-00117-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Neill, DG, Skipper, AM, Kadhim, J, Church, DB, Brodbelt, DC and Packer, RMA 2019 Disorders of Bulldogs under primary veterinary care in the UK in 2013. PLoS One 14(6): e0217928. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0217928CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Packer, RMA 2013 Quantitative investigation of healthy conformational limits in domestic dog breeds. PhD Thesis, Royal Veterinary College, University of London, UKGoogle Scholar
Packer, RMA, Hendricks, A and Burn, CC 2012 Do dog owners perceive the clinical signs related to conformational inherited disorders as ‘normal’ for the breed? A potential constraint to improving canine welfare. Animal Welfare 21(S1): 8193. https://doi.org/10.7120/09627812X13345905673809CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Packer, RMA, Hendricks, A and Burn, CC 2015a Impact of facial conformation on canine health: Corneal ulceration. PLoS One 10(5): e0123827. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.012382Google ScholarPubMed
Packer, RMA, Hendricks, A, Tivers, MS and Burn, CC 2015b Impact of facial conformation on canine health: Brachycephalic Obstructive Airway Syndrome. PLoS One 10(10): e0137496. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0137496Google ScholarPubMed
Packer, RMA, Murphy, D and Farnworth, MJ 2017 Purchasing popular purebreds: investigating the influence of breed-type on the pre-purchase motivations and behaviour of dog owners. Animal Welfare 26(2): 191201. https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.26.2.191CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Packer, RMA, O’Neill, DG, Fletcher, F and Farnworth, MJ 2019 Great expectations, inconvenient truths, and the paradoxes of the dog-owner relationship for owners of brachycephalic dogs. PLoS One 14(7): e0219918. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0219918CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Packer, RMA, O’Neill, DG, Fletcher, F and Farnworth, MJ 2020 Come for the looks, stay for the personality? A mixed-methods investigation of reacquisition and owner recommendation of Bulldogs, French Bulldogs and Pugs. PLoS One 15(8): e0237276. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0237276CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Packer, RMA and Tivers, MS 2015 Strategies for the management and prevention of conformation-related respiratory disorders in brachycephalic dogs. Veterinary Medicine-Research and Reports 6: 219232. https://doi.org/10.2147/vmrr.s60475CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parker, HG, Dreger, DL, Rimbault, M, Davis, BW, Mullen, AB, Carpintero-Ramirez, G and Ostrander, EA 2017 Genomic analyses reveal the influence of geographic origin, migration, and hybridization on modern dog breed development. Cell Reports 19: 697708. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.celrep.2017.03.079CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Parker, HG, VonHoldt, BM, Quignon, P, Margulies, EH, Shao, S, Mosher, DS, Spadey, TC, Elkahloun, A, Cargill, M, Jones, PG, Maslen, CL, Acland, GM, Sutter, NB, Kuroki, K, Bustamante, CD, Wayne, RK and Ostrander, EA 2009 An expressed FGF4 retrogene is associated with breed defining chondroplasia in domestic dogs. Science 325(5943): 995998. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1173275CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parr, WCH, Wilson, LAB, Wroe, S, Colman, NJ, Crowther, MS and Letnic, M 2016 Cranial shape and the modularity of hybridization in dingoes and dogs; Hybridization does not spell the end for native morphology. Evolutionary Biology 43: 171187. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11692-016-9371-xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parsons, CE, Young, KS, Kumari, N, Stein, A and Kringelbach, ML 2011 The Motivational salience of infant faces is similar for men and women. PLoS One 6(5): e20632. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0020632CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peichl, L 1992 Topography of ganglion cells in the dog and wolf retina. Journal of Comparative Neurology 324(4): 603620. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.903240412CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pittenger, JB and Shaw, RE 1975 Aging faces as viscal-elastic events – implications for a theory of nonrigid shape perception. Journal of Experimental Psychology-Human Perception and Performance 1(4): 374382. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.1.4.374CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Regodon, S, Vivo, JM, Franco, A, Guillen, MT and Robina, A 1993 Craniofacial angle in dolichocephalic, mesocephalic and brachycephalic dogs – radiological determination and application. Annals of Anatomy – Anatomischer Anzeiger 175(4): 361363. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0940-9602(11)80043-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, T, McGreevy, P and Valenzuela, M 2010 Human-induced rotation and reorganization of the brain of domestic dogs. PLoS One 5(7): e11946. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0011946CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rooney, N and Sargan, D 2009 Pedigree dog breeding in the UK: A major welfare concern? An independent scientific report commissioned by the RSPCA. RSPCA: Horsham, UK. file:///Users/lvesp/Downloads/Pedigree-dogs-report-locked.pdfGoogle Scholar
Sandøe, P, Kondrup, SV, Bennett, PC, Forkman, B, Meyer, I, Proschowsky, HF, Serpell, JA and Lund, TB 2017 Why do people buy dogs with potential welfare problems related to extreme conformation and inherited disease? A representative study of Danish owners of four small dog breeds. PLoS One 12(2): e0172091. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0172091CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schoenebeck, JJ, Hutchinson, SA, Byers, A, Beale, HC, Carrington, B, Faden, DL, Rimbault, M, Decker, B, Kidd, JM, Sood, R, Boyko, AR, Fondon, JW, Wayne, RK, Bustamante, CD, Ciruna, B and Ostrander, EA 2012 Variation of BMP3 Contributes to Dog Breed Skull Diversity. PLoS Genetics 8(8): e1002849. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1002849CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Senese, VP, De Falco, S, Bornstein, MH, Caria, A, Buffolino, S and Venuti, P 2013 Human infant faces provoke implicit positive affective responses in parents and non-parents alike. PLoS One 8(11): https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0080379CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seppanen, RTK, Kaimio, M, Schildt, KJM, Lilja-Maula, L, Hyytiainen, HK, Molsa, S, Morelius, M, Rajamaki, MM, Lappalainen, AK and Rantala, M 2019 Skin and ear health in a group of English bulldogs in Finland – a descriptive study with special reference to owner perceptions. Veterinary Dermatology 30(4): 307313. https://doi.org/10.1111/vde.12752Google Scholar
Serpell, JA 1996 In the Company of Animals: A Study of Human-Animal Relationships, Second Edition. CUP: Cambridge, UK.Google Scholar
Serpell, JA 2002 Anthropomorphism and anthropomorphic selection - Beyond the ‘cute response.’ Society and Animals 10(4): 437454. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853002320936926CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Serpell, JA 2019 How happy is your pet? The problem o f subjectivity i n the assessment companion animal welfare. Animal Welfare 28(1): 5766. https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.28.1.057CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Serpell, JA and Paul, ES 2011 The unique family relationship between people and their pets. In: Salmon, C and Shackelford, T (eds) The Oxford Handbook of Evolutionary Family Psychology pp 297309. OUP: Oxford, UK.Google Scholar
Skipper, A 2021 A historical perspective on brachycephalic breed health and the role of the veterinary profession. In: Packer, R and O’Neill, D (eds) Health and Welfare of Brachycephalic (Flat-faced) Companion Animals pp 724. CRC Press: Boca Raton, FL, USA.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternglanz, SH, Gray, JL and Murakami, M 1977 Adult preferences for infantile facial features – ethological approach. Animal Behaviour 25 : 108115. https://doi.org/10.1016/0003-3472(77)90072-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutter, NB, Mosher, DS, Gray, MM and Ostrander, EA 2008 Morphometrics within dog breeds are highly reproducible and dispute Rensch’s rule. Mammalian Genome 19(10-12): 713723. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00335-008-9153-6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Todd, JT, Mark, LS, Shaw, RE and Pittenger, JB 1980 Perception of human growth. Scientific American 242(2): 132145. https://doi.org/10.1038/scientificamerican0280-132CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tuan, Y-F 1984 Dominance and Affection: The Making of Pets. Yale University Press: New Haven and London, UK.Google Scholar
Waller, BM, Peirce, K, Caeiro, CC, Scheider, L, Burrows, AM, McCune, S and Kaminski, J 2013 Paedomorphic facial expressions give dogs a selective advantage. PLoS One 8(12): e82686. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0082686CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, A and Bannasch, D 2006 The dog and its genome. In: Ostrander, EA, Giger, U and Lindblad-Toh, K (eds) The Dog and its Genome. Cold Spring Harbour Press: New York, NY, USA.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 1. Sagittal and coronal plane line-drawings of (from left to right): adult human, infant human, adult French bulldog and adult Siberian husky.

Figure 1

Figure 2. Measurements made on all dog face images, to calculate mean relative feature sizes for each breed: (a) face height; (b) face width; (c) forehead height; (d) eye height; (e) eye width; (f) nose height; (g) nose width; (h) distance between the eyes (to calculate relative distance between the eyes [h]/[b]).

Figure 2

Table 1. Relative muzzle length (CFR, CI) of the 42 breeds studied

Figure 3

Table 2. Associations (Pearson product moment correlations) among fronto-facial ‘kindchenschema’ measures

Figure 4

Table 3. Multiple linear regression results for mean breed fronto-facial ‘kindchenschema’ measures, using relative muzzle length and estimated body height as predictors

Figure 5

Figure 3. (a) Mean breed cranio-facial ratio (CFR; Packer et al. 2015a) plotted against relative forehead size, (b) mean breed cranio-facial ratio plotted against relative eye size (aperture area) and (c) mean breed cranio-facial ratio plotted against relative nose size. Smaller CFR values indicate a relatively shorter-muzzled, more brachycephalic cranial morphology.

Figure 6

Figure 4. (a) Estimated breed height at withers (The Kennel Club 2021) plotted against relative forehead size, (b) estimated breed height plotted against relative eye size (aperture area) and (c) estimated breed height plotted against relative nose size.

Figure 7

Figure 5. (a) Mean breed cranio-facial ratio (CFR; Packer et al. 2015a) plotted against eye height/width ratio and (b) estimated breed height at withers (The Kennel Club 2021) plotted against eye height/width ratio. Higher eye height/width ratio values indicate taller, more rounded eye shapes (the mean eye shape across the whole sample is a wider-than-tall, almond shape) and smaller CFR values indicate a relatively shorter-muzzled, more brachycephalic cranial morphology.

Supplementary material: PDF

Paul et al. supplementary material

Paul et al. supplementary material

Download Paul et al. supplementary material(PDF)
PDF 1.6 MB