Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-8ctnn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T04:09:35.943Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Mind the neuron! The role of the single neuron in a theory of mind

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  24 June 2014

Richard M. Vickery*
Affiliation:
School of Medical Sciences, UNSW, Sydney, New South Wales, Australia
*
Dr Richard M. Vickery, School of Medical Sciences, UNSW, Sydney, NSW 2052, Australia. Tel: 61 2 93851676; Fax: 61 2 93851059; E-mail: [email protected]

Abstract

Objective:

To present the argument that the only secure foundation for a theory of behaviour, and ultimately of mind, rests at the level of single neurons, and to assess progress at this level of explanation.

Methods:

Relevant data were obtained by a search of PubMed, last updated in January 2007, focused on implemented models from single-neuron studies.

Results:

Technical limitations on recording neural activity produce trade-offs between temporal and spatial resolution and the ability to track the massively parallel activity of the nervous system. The properties of the single neuron that would need to be measured and the techniques available to obtain the data are described. The concept of a fixed neuronal identity may be impeding progress and should be replaced with the concept of dynamically assigned neuron identity.

Conclusion:

Modern data collection techniques make it possible to obtain data at the single-neuron level on the complete nervous systems of simple organisms. Present models based on this data do not provide an integrated explanation of behaviour. However, there do not appear to be insurmountable theoretical or practical obstacles to building such models in the future or of scaling the data collection up to more complex organisms.

Type
Research Article
Copyright
Copyright © 2007 Blackwell Munksgaard

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Bennett, MR, Hacker, PMS. Philosophical foundations of neuroscience. Malden: Blackwell, 2003.Google Scholar
Miller, SM. On the correlation/constitution distinction problem (and other hard problems) in the scientific study of consciousness. Acta Neuropsychiatr 2007;19:159176.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grill-Spector, K, Knouf, N, Kanwisher, N. The fusiform face area subserves face perception, not generic within-category identification. Nat Neurosci 2004;7:555562.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, SM, Ngo, TT. Studies of caloric vestibular stimulation: implications for the cognitive neurosciences, the clinical neurosciences and neurophilosophy. Acta Neuropsychiatr 2007;19:183203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mountcastle, VB. Modality and topographic properties of cat’s somatic sensory cortex. J Neurophysiol 1957;20:408434.Google ScholarPubMed
Zhao, F, Wang, P, Hendrich, K, Kim, S-G. Spatial specificity of cerebral blood volume-weighted fMRI responses at columnar resolution. NeuroImage 2005;27:416424.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horton, JC, Adams, DL. The cortical column: a structure without a function. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society, Series B 2005;360:837862.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shadlen, MN, Newsome, WT. Neural basis of a perceptual decision in the parietal cortex (Area LIP) of the rhesus monkey. J Neurophysiol 2001;86:19161936.Google ScholarPubMed
Schafer, WR. Deciphering the neural and molecular mechanisms of C. elegans behavior. Curr Biol 2005;15:R723R729.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rieke, F, Warland, D, De Ruyter van Steveninck, R, Bialek, W. Spikes: exploring the neural code. Cambridge: MIT Press, 1999.Google Scholar
Kirkcaldie, MTK, Kitchener, PD. When brains expand: mind and the evolution of cortex. Acta Neuropsychiatr 2007;19:139148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Felleman, DJ, Van Essen, DC. Distributed hierarchical processing in the primate cerebral cortex. Cereb Cortex 1991;1:147.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, BT, D’Esposito, M. Searching for “the top” in top-down control. Neuron 2005;48:535538.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lin, L, Chen, G, Xie, K, Zaia, KA, Zhang, S, Tsien, JZ. Large-scale neural ensemble recording in the brains of freely behaving mice. J Neurosci Methods 2006;155:2838.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Briggman, KL, Kristan, WB Jr. Imaging dedicated and multifunctional neural circuits generating distinct behaviors. J Neurosci 2006;26:1092510933.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ringach, DL, Hawken, MJ, Shapley, R. Dynamics of orientation tuning in macaque primary visual cortex. Nature 1997;387:281284.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rolls, ET, Kesner, RP. A computational theory of hippocampal function, and empirical tests of the theory. Prog Neurobiol 2006;79:148.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Makarova, VA, Panetsos, F, De Feo, O. A method for determining neural connectivity and inferring the underlying network dynamics using extracellular spike recordings. J Neurosci Methods 2005;144:265279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cudeiro, J, Sillito, AM. Looking back: corticothalamic feedback and early visual processing. Trends Neurosci 2006;29:298306.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, JM, Hill, JJ, Bargmann, CI. A circuit for navigation in Caenorhabditis elegans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2005;102:31843191.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Schutter, E, Ekeberg, O, Kotaleski, JH, Achard, P, Lansner, A. Biophysically detailed modelling of microcircuits and beyond. Trends Neurosci 2005;28:562569.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hannan, AJ. Brain phylogeny, ontogeny and dysfunction: integrating evolutionary, developmental and clinical perspectives in cognitive neuroscience. Acta Neuropsychiatr 2007;19:149158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
White, J G, Southgate, E, Thomson, JN, Brenner, S. The structure of the nervous system of the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans: the mind of a worm. Phil Trans R Soc Lond B 1986;314:1340.CrossRefGoogle Scholar