Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-28T03:09:07.121Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

8 - Explaining Wellbeing

A First Exploration

from Part III - How Our Experience Affects Our Wellbeing

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  12 May 2023

Richard Layard
Affiliation:
London School of Economics and Political Science
Jan-Emmanuel De Neve
Affiliation:
University of Oxford

Summary

This concludes our brief initial overview of the main causes of high and low wellbeing – and of the huge variation in wellbeing in the world. All the findings are cross-sectional, with time series and experiments left to later chapters. The findings of this chapter provide the framework for the rest of Part III of the book – starting with personal factors and working outwards to those relating to whole communities.

Within a country (if it is advanced), the main factors explaining the variance of wellbeing (and the prevalence of misery) are in rough order of importance: mental illness; physical illness; having work and the quality of that work; having a partner; family income; and education.

The variation of wellbeing across countries is largely explained (in rough order of importance) by: income; health; social support; personal freedom; trusting social relations; and generosity.

Predicting whether a child will become a happy adult is not easy. But wellbeing in childhood is a better predictor of satisfaction in adult life than the child’s academic success is. And as the next chapter shows, both schools and parents have big effects on children’s wellbeing.

Type
Chapter
Information
Wellbeing
Science and Policy
, pp. 126 - 137
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2023
Creative Commons
Creative Common License - CCCreative Common License - BYCreative Common License - NCCreative Common License - ND
This content is Open Access and distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution licence CC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 https://creativecommons.org/cclicenses/

Happy the man who has learned the cause of things …

Virgil

We can now put these tools to use in explaining the huge inequalities that exist in wellbeing. These inequalities are interesting in themselves. And they also provide naturalistic evidence that helps us to predict how policies of different kinds would change people’s wellbeing.Footnote 1 So this chapter applies the tools presented in Chapter 7 to explain and learn from the differences in Chapter 6.

We proceed as follows:

  • First, we outline a framework for how adult wellbeing is determined within a country.

  • Next, we take adult wellbeing differences within a country and see how these are explained by differences in adult characteristics.

  • Then we go back to childhood and see how well the child predicts the adult.

  • Finally, we look at the role of social norms and institutions and study their effects by looking at differences between countries.

This chapter is very important in setting the framework for the rest of Part III of the book – which looks at each influence in much more detail.

Wellbeing and the Life Cycle

Our adult wellbeing is the product of our whole life to date (see Figure 8.1). Our early development is affected first by our parents (including their genes) and then by our schooling. These are the main factors that determine our outcomes up to the end of childhood. These outcomes then help to predict our adult outcomes, which in turn determine our adult life satisfaction. In what follows, we shall work backwards, looking first at the role of adult outcomes, then at the role of child outcomes and finally (in Chapter 9) at the role of family and schooling.

Figure 8.1 How adult wellbeing is determined

Note: Earlier factors also influence later outcomes directly

Personal Determinants of Adult Wellbeing

There have been thousands of studies of the personal causes of adult wellbeing. But they are difficult to compare because they use different measures of wellbeing and they study the effect of different sets of determinants. We shall therefore focus in this chapter on one study that used a single measure of wellbeing (life satisfaction) and looked at all the main influences simultaneously.Footnote 2 The main influences included were:

  • Health

    • Physical health (number of illnesses)

    • Mental health (has ever been diagnosed with depression or an anxiety disorder)

  • Work

    • Is unemployed (versus employed)

    • Quality of work (index)

  • Family

    • Has partner (versus single)

    • Is separated (versus single)

    • Is widowed (versus single)

  • Income (log of household income per equivalent adult)

  • Education (years)

Many studies of wellbeing do not include mental health as an explanatory factor, since it is itself a feeling. But this is why we do not include self-reported feelings as measures of mental health – instead, we include an objective diagnosis by a third party. The question used is ‘Have you ever been told by a doctor, nurse of other health professional that you had an anxiety disorder and/or depressive disorder?’ Since most such experiences are prior to being surveyed, they are essentially measuring something exogenous. Besides, it would be quite wrong not to include mental illness as an explanation for wellbeing, when this is such an important issue for so many people, independently of the other right-hand variables. As the equations that follow show, low wellbeing is not the same as mental illness and can be caused by many other factors besides mental illness.

So the task is to estimate equation (1) in Chapter 7, with current life satisfaction as the dependent variable and the variables listed above as the independent variables. The equation estimated is cross-sectional – a point to which we shall return. The study covered Britain, Germany, Australia and the United States, with broadly similar results from all four countries. The results we report in Table 8.1 come mainly from a British survey (Understanding Society). But the mental health coefficient is from the United States and Australia (where the measure of mental health is more exogenous and the results are very similar). And the quality of work result comes from a different study using the European Social Survey.Footnote 3

Table 8.1 How different factors affect life satisfaction (0–10) of adults over 25 (Britain) (Pooled cross section) (R2 = 0.19)

Effect on life satisfaction (0–10)
Physical health problems (No. of illnesses)−0.22 (0.01)
Mental health problems (0,1)−0.72 (0.05)
Unemployed (versus employed; 0,1)−0.70 (0.04)
Quality of work (effect of 1SD)+0.40 (0.04)
Partnered (versus single; 0,1)+0.59 (0.03)
Separated (versus single; 0,1)−0.15 (0.04)
Widowed (versus single; 0,1)+0.11 (0.08)
Income (log)+0.17 (0.01)
Education (years)+0.03 (0.00)
Source: A. E. Clark et al. (Reference Clark, Flèche, Layard, Powdthavee and Ward2018) Table 16.2 Mainly Britain (Understanding Society) 1996–2014 but see text

Notes: Control variables include comparators’ income, education, unemployment and partnership, as well as gender, age and age squared and year fixed effects. Standard errors in brackets.

Table 8.1 shows, for example, that people ever diagnosed with mental illness are currently (other things equal) less satisfied with their life by 0.72 points.Footnote 4 Unemployment has a similar effect, as does not having a partner. A unit increase in log income increases wellbeing by 0.17 points, which means that the doubling of income raises wellbeing by 0.12 points.Footnote 5 Note that the standard error (in brackets) of these coefficients are small relative to the coefficients themselves, so the estimates are fairly well defined and significantly different from zero at the 95% level.

From Table 8.1, we learn the effect of each variable upon wellbeing. But this does not tell us how far the inequality of that variable explains the inequality of wellbeing. For that purpose, we have to multiply the effect of each explanatory variable by its own standard deviation – and then divide it by the standard deviation of wellbeing. This gives us the βj measure discussed in Chapter 7 – recall that βj = ajσj/σw.

In Figure 8.2, we give these β values in graphical form. To simplify the display, we have treated every variable as if it had a positive β – by, for example, relabelling Unemployed as Not unemployed.

Figure 8.2 What explains the variation of life satisfaction among adults over 25? (Britain) Partial correlation coefficients (β) (R2 = 0.19).

Notes: For quality of work see their chapter 4. Standard errors in brackets

Source: A. E. Clark et al. (Reference Clark, Flèche, Layard, Powdthavee and Ward2018) Table 16.1; otherwise see Table 8.1; ‘partnered’ means partnered versus any other relationship status

Notice first that the whole equation has an R2 of 0.19 – we are only explaining 19% of the total variance. It is very important not to over-claim explanatory power.Footnote 6 And it is also quite wrong to label the unexplained residual as ‘luck’. It is quite simply the variation in wellbeing that we have not been able to explain or is due to measurement error. Nevertheless, what we do know is that health is extremely important (especially mental health). Work is also important – having it (if you want it) and its quality. Family life also matters.

So does income, but its explanatory power is no higher than many other variables. In Figure 8.2 it explains under 1% of the variance of life satisfaction, since its β2 value is under 0.12. But as we shall see in Chapter 13, the figure can rise to 3% in some countries – but still no higher than some other influences.

Before we accept this conclusion, we have to ask some of the questions we discussed in Chapter 7. First, is there a problem of measurement error? This is unlikely to affect the ranking of income in Figure 8.2 since income is measured more accurately than many of the other variables. Second, are there important omitted variables or problems of endogeneity? To investigate these problems, we can use the panel data to perform a fixed-effects analysis as in Equation (7.4). In panel regressions, all the coefficients are reduced, partly because the problem of measurement error becomes more acute. The coefficients on income fall more than most, though this may be partly because the exact timings are not well caught by the data. For this chapter, we stick to cross-sectional results; but in Chapter 13 we also give the results of fixed effects regressions.

It is important to stress that for all the variables in Table 8.1 and Figure 8.2, the coefficients reflect the effect of the variable, other things constant. If we wanted to investigate the total effect of income on wellbeing, we would also have to include its effects through other ‘mediating’ variables like having a partner. This is quite difficult to compute. But it is easy to compute a maximum value for the total coefficient on income (or any other variable). It is the simple bivariate coefficient, which in this case is about double the coefficient holding other things constant.

Another obvious question is this: Doesn’t income explain more of the prevalence of low wellbeing than is implied by Figure 8.2? To investigate this, we construct a new variable. This is a simple binary variable for misery (M), which is constructed as follows:Footnote 7

M equals 1 if wellbeing is 5 or below

M equals 0 if wellbeing is above 5

Misery (thus defined) affects the bottom 10% or so of the British population – so it is a good measure of deprivation. We can then run the following simple equation:

Mi=a0+ajXij+ei.

If we take averages across all individuals, this equation predicts the proportion of people in misery, which is given by

M¯=a0+ajX¯j.

In an analysis for Britain, Australia and the United States, mental health problems accounted for more of the misery than any other factor.Footnote 8 In Britain and Australia, this was followed by physical illness, and in the United States by low income. Unemployment, though devastating to those affected, came in lower than health and poverty, because of the smaller numbers affected.

A slightly different question is What best explains who is in misery and who is not? In other words, what are the most important elements in the following relationship:

R2=1σe2σM2=βj2+βkβsrksks.

In Figure 8.3, we show the values of the β coefficients where this time Misery is the dependent variable. As can be seen, these coefficients are slightly smaller than when we are explaining the full continuous range of Life–Satisfaction (in Figure 8.2). This is to be expected. But, strikingly, the relative importance of the different factors is the same whether we are explaining low wellbeing or the whole spread across the spectrum.

Figure 8.3 What explains the variation of misery among adults over 25? (Britain) Partial correlation coefficients (β) (R2 = 0.14)

Note: See Figure 8.2

Source: A. E. Clark et al. (Reference Clark, Flèche, Layard, Powdthavee and Ward2018) Table 16.1 Mainly Britain (Understanding Society) but see text

Childhood Predictors of Individual Wellbeing

So much for the adult causes of adult wellbeing. But aren’t many of the adult influences we have looked at caused by how we were in childhood? They are indeed. There are three main dimensions of child development: intellectual, emotional and behavioural. A big question for schools is ‘Which of these is the best predictor of whether a child will have a satisfying adult life?’

The following analysis is based on a follow-up of all British children born in a week in 1970 (the British Cohort Study 1970). The measures of child development that we use are these:

  • Intellectual: highest qualification ever obtained

  • Behavioural: behaviour measured at age 16 (by 17 questions asked to the mother)

  • Emotional: emotional health measured at age 16 (by 22 questions asked to the young person and 8 to the mother).

As Figure 8.4 shows, the best predictor of a satisfying adult life is not your qualifications but a simple measure of your emotional health at 16. This is an important finding for educational policy since, as Chapter 9 shows, schools have such a huge influence on the wellbeing of their children. Qualifications also matter of course and are by far the best predictor of an adult’s income. But, as we have seen, income is less important for adults than their mental health, which is best predicted by their emotional health in childhood.

Figure 8.4 How adult life satisfaction is predicted by child outcomes (Britain) Partial correlation coefficients (β) (R2 = 0.035)

Note: Adult life satisfaction is average at ages 34 and 42. Controls include family variables. Standard errors in brackets

Source: See A. E. Clark et al. (Reference Clark, Flèche, Layard, Powdthavee and Ward2018) Figure 1.2 British Cohort Study (BCS)

As we argue in Chapter 9, child wellbeing is important in itself – childhood is a substantial part of our whole life experience. But wellbeing as a child is also the foundation for wellbeing as an adult.

The Effects of Social Norms and Institutions

We have focused so far on what explains the differences in wellbeing between people within the same country. But what explains the differences between countries? There are important social norms and institutions that everyone in a country shares. While we cannot identify these effects by studying people in one country only, we can do it by comparing one country with another. The Gallup World Poll data enable us to do just that – to study the effect of

  • ethical standards (trustworthiness and generosity),

  • networks of social support and

  • personal freedom.

We measure these by the answers in each country to the following questions:

TrustProportion who say Yes to the first half of ‘In general, do you think that most people can be trusted, or alternatively that you can’t be too careful in dealing with people?’
GenerosityProportion who say Yes to ‘Have you donated money to a charity in the present month?’
Social SupportProportion who say Yes to ‘If you were in trouble, do you have relatives or friends you can count on to help you whenever you need them?’
FreedomProportion who say Yes to ‘Are you satisfied or dissatisfied with your freedom to choose what you do with your life?’.

We also include the effect of average GDP per head and healthy life expectancy (measured in years).

In the cross-sectional analysis in Table 8.2, we show for each of these variables how average life-evaluation changes when the percentage who say Yes rises from 0 to 100. All four factors have substantial effect. So does healthy life-expectancy – for example, an increase of 10 years raises life evaluation by 0.3 points. And the effect of income across countries is similar to its effect within many countries – a doubling of income raises average life-evaluation by 0.23 points (out of 10).

Table 8.2 How national life satisfaction (0–10) is affected by country-level variables (R2 = 0.77)

ChangeEffect on average life satisfaction (0–10)
Trust100% v. 0%1.08 (.45)
Generosity100% v. 0%0.54 (.41)
Social support100% v. 0%2.03 (.61)
Freedom100% v. 0%1.41 (.49)
IncomeDoubling0.23 (.06)
HealthYears of healthy life0.03 (.01)
Source: Gallup World Poll, Cantril ladder; average data for 2009–2015 except for trust (mostly 2009); analysis by John Helliwell; standard errors in brackets

It is also interesting to see how far the different factors contribute to the actual dispersion of life satisfaction across countries. Here income plays a more conspicuous role due to the huge income differences between countries. Health differences also come through as very important. This can be seen in Figure 8.5, which gives the βj coefficients corresponding to the aj coefficients in Table 8.2. The social norms are also very important. For example, what distinguishes the eight countries with the highest life satisfaction in the world is not their income but their high levels of trust, social support, freedom and generosity.Footnote 9 These are the five Nordic countries as well as the Netherlands, Switzerland and New Zealand (see Table 13.1). The countries at the bottom are mainly the war-torn countries of sub-Saharan Africa and the Middle East (Afghanistan, Syria and Yemen), which not only have poor income and healthcare but are low on the social features that wellbeing requires.Footnote 10

Figure 8.5 How differences in national life satisfaction are explained by country-level variables – partial correlation coefficients (β) (R2 = 0.77)

Source: See Table 8.2

Conclusions

This concludes our brief initial overview of the main causes of high and low wellbeing – and of the huge variation in wellbeing in the world. All the findings are cross-sectional, with time series and experiments left to later chapters. The findings of this chapter provide the framework for the rest of Part III of the book – starting with personal factors and working outwards to those relating to whole communities.

  • Within a country (if it is advanced), the main factors explaining the variance of wellbeing (and the prevalence of misery) are in rough order of importance:

    • mental illness,

    • physical illness,

    • having work and the quality of that work,

    • having a partner,

    • family income and

    • education

  • The variation of wellbeing across countries is largely explained (in rough order of importance) by:

    • income,

    • health,

    • social support,

    • personal freedom,

    • trusting social relations and

    • generosity

  • Predicting whether a child will become a happy adult is not easy. But the child’s wellbeing is a better predictor of satisfaction in adult life than the child’s academic success is. And as Chapter 9 shows, both schools and parents have big effects on children’s wellbeing.

Questions for discussion

  1. (1) Are the findings about income in Figure 8.2 credible? Could problems of measurement error have produced incorrect rankings?

  2. (2) Is Figure 8.5 informative?

Footnotes

1 This also requires evidence on the immediate effects of the policy, preferably from experiments.

2 A.E. Clark et al. (Reference Clark, Flèche, Layard, Powdthavee and Ward2018). In Chapter 13, we report results for the full range of countries using the Gallup World Poll.

3 For the quality of work, see A. E. Clark et al. (Reference Clark, Flèche, Layard, Powdthavee and Ward2018) p. 74. On mental health, the British measure (being less exogenous) attracts a higher coefficient than the US measure.

4 In the Gallup World Poll analysis reported in annex 13.1, there is a similar coefficient on the self-reported question ‘Yesterday did you experience a lot of depression’.

5 Loge2=0.7. Note that we always use logarithms to the base e.

6 A well-known device for over-claiming is the odds ratio. See Chapter 7.

7 In the original Understanding Society survey, life satisfaction is measured on a scale of 1–7. We have here transformed it to a scale of 0–10. Note that the multiple regression approach can be used to examine binary outcomes, with easy-to-interpret coefficients. Logit and probit analysis can also be used, but usually deliver equivalent results but in a form that is more difficult to understand.

10 If the Global Peace Index is added to a fixed effects regression of average life satisfaction on the 6 variables, a 1 standard deviation of the Index raises average live-satisfaction by 0.15 points (Helliwell et al. [Reference Helliwell, Huang, Wang, Norton, Helliwell, Layard, Sachs and De Neve2019] p. 40).

References

Further Reading

Clark, A. E., Flèche, S., Layard, R., Powdthavee, N., and Ward, G. (2018). The Origins of Happiness: The Science of Wellbeing over the Life Course: Princeton University Press. Especially chapters 1, 2, 6 and 16.Google Scholar
World Happiness Report (latest version). Helliwell, J. F., Layard, R., Sachs, J., and De Neve, J. E. (Eds.). World Happiness Report. New York: Sustainable Development Solutions Network. Chapter 2.Google Scholar
Figure 0

Figure 8.1 How adult wellbeing is determinedNote: Earlier factors also influence later outcomes directly

Figure 1

Table 8.1 How different factors affect life satisfaction (0–10) of adults over 25 (Britain) (Pooled cross section) (R2 = 0.19)

Source: A. E. Clark et al. (2018) Table 16.2 Mainly Britain (Understanding Society) 1996–2014 but see text
Figure 2

Figure 8.2 What explains the variation of life satisfaction among adults over 25? (Britain) Partial correlation coefficients (β) (R2 = 0.19).Notes: For quality of work see their chapter 4. Standard errors in brackets

Source: A. E. Clark et al. (2018) Table 16.1; otherwise see Table 8.1; ‘partnered’ means partnered versus any other relationship status
Figure 3

Figure 8.3 What explains the variation of misery among adults over 25? (Britain) Partial correlation coefficients (β) (R2 = 0.14)Note: See Figure 8.2

Source: A. E. Clark et al. (2018) Table 16.1 Mainly Britain (Understanding Society) but see text
Figure 4

Figure 8.4 How adult life satisfaction is predicted by child outcomes (Britain) Partial correlation coefficients (β) (R2 = 0.035)Note: Adult life satisfaction is average at ages 34 and 42. Controls include family variables. Standard errors in brackets

Source: See A. E. Clark et al. (2018) Figure 1.2 British Cohort Study (BCS)
Figure 5

Table 8.2 How national life satisfaction (0–10) is affected by country-level variables (R2 = 0.77)

Source: Gallup World Poll, Cantril ladder; average data for 2009–2015 except for trust (mostly 2009); analysis by John Helliwell; standard errors in brackets
Figure 6

Figure 8.5 How differences in national life satisfaction are explained by country-level variables – partial correlation coefficients (β) (R2 = 0.77)

Source: See Table 8.2

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×