Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7czq Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-22T20:28:39.375Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

2 - Time, the Body, and the Other in Phenomenology and Psychopathology

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 October 2020

Christian Tewes
Affiliation:
Heidelberg University Hospital
Giovanni Stanghellini
Affiliation:
Chieti University
Get access

Summary

The relations between embodiment and temporality reach from the micro-temporality of conscious experience to the enactment of human existence. First, the basic internal time consciousness is marked by the rhythmicity of vital processes (heartbeat, respiratory rhythm, daily periods, etc.). Moreover, the bodily drives, urges, and needs, which may be subsumed under the term conation, crucially determine the future-directed temporality of primary experience. On the other hand, the body forms an extract of sensorimotor and affective experience which are sedimented in implicit or body memory, thus shaping an individual's capacities and dispositions. Finally, existential temporality is essentially characterized by the vital processes of birth, growth, aging, and dying.

Body, time, and intersubjectivity are equally interconnected. First, interbodily resonance establishes the primary experience of the shared present, beginning in the child's early development. Thus, a basic contemporality emerges, which later continues in social synchronizations and temporal orders as the basis of social life. However, this temporal alignment is also subject to desynchronizations, for example, in backlogs of tasks, in guilt, remorse, or grief – situations that require processes of psychosocial resynchronization.

All these interconnections are subject to various kinds of disturbances which are also found in psychopathology and thus crucially determine the emergence and course of mental disorders.

Type
Chapter
Information
Time and Body
Phenomenological and Psychopathological Approaches
, pp. 12 - 40
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Améry, J. (1977). Jenseits von Schuld und Sühne. Bewältigungsversuche eines Überwältigten [Beyond guilt and expiation. Attempts to overcome of someone overcome]. Munich, Germany: dtv.Google Scholar
Amminger, G., Schäfer, M. R., Papageorgiou, K., Klier, C. M., Schlögelhofer, M., Mossaheb, N., … McGorry, P. D. (2012). Emotion recognition in individuals at clinical high-risk for schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 38(5), 10301039. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbr015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arendt, H. (1958). The human condition. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Bech, P. (1975). Depression: Influence on time estimation and time experience. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavia, 51(1), 4250. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.1975.tb00211.xGoogle Scholar
Blankenburg, W. (1971). Der Verlust der natürlichen Selbstverständlichkeit [The loss of natural self-evidence]. Berlin, Germany: Springer.Google Scholar
Bourke, C., Douglas, K., & Porter, R. (2010). Processing of facial emotion expression in major depression: A review. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 44(8), 681696. doi:10.3109/00048674.2010.496359Google Scholar
Bschor, T., Ising, M., Bauer, M., Lewitzka, U., Skerstupeit, M., Müller-Oerlinghausen, B., & Baethge, C. (2004). Time experience and time judgment in major depression, mania and healthy subjects. A controlled study of 93 subjects. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 109(3), 222229.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casey, E. S. (1984). Habitual body and memory in Merleau-Ponty. In Mohanty, J. N. (Ed.), Phenomenology and the human sciences (pp. 3957). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Condon, W. S. (1986). Communication: Rhythm and structure. In Evans, J. R. & Clynes, M. (Eds.), Rhythm in psychological, linguistic and musical processes (pp. 5577). Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas.Google Scholar
Craig, A. D. (2009). Emotional moments across time: A possible neural basis for time perception in the anterior insula. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London: Biological Sciences, B, 364(1525), 19331942. doi:10.1098/rstb.2009.0008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Czeisler, C. A., Duffy, J. F., Shanahan, T. L., Brown, E. N., Mitchell, J. F., Rimmer, D. W., … Kronauer, R. E. (1999). Stability, precision, and near-24-hour period of the human circadian pacemaker. Science, 284(5423), 21772181. doi:10.1126/science.284.5423.2177CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, M. (Ed.). (1982). Interaction rhythms. Periodicity in communicative behavior. New York, NY: Human Sciences Press.Google Scholar
De Haan, S. (2020). Enactive psychiatry. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
De Haan, S., & Fuchs, T. (2010). The ghost in the machine: Disembodiment in schizophrenia. Two case studies. Psychopathology, 43(5), 327333. doi:10.1159/000319402Google Scholar
De Jaegher, H. (2013). Embodiment and sense-making in autism. Frontiers in Integrative Neuroscience, 7, 119. doi:10.3389/fnint.2013.00015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Jaegher, H. (2019). Loving and knowing: Reflections for an engaged epistemology. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences. Advance online publication. doi:10.1007/s11097-019-09634-5Google Scholar
De Jaegher, H., & Di Paolo, E. (2007). Participatory sense-making: An enactive approach to social cognition. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 6(4), 485507. doi:10.1007/s11097-007-9076-9CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dignon, A., Beardsmore, A., Spain, S., & Kuan, A. (2006). ‘Why I won't eat’: Patient testimony from 15 anorexics concerning the causes of their disorder. Journal of Health Psychology, 11(6), 942956. doi:10.1177/1359105306069097CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edwards, J., Jackson, H. J., & Pattison, P. E. (2002). Emotion recognition via facial expression and affective prosody in schizophrenia: A methodological review. Clinical Psychology Review, 22(6), 789832. doi:10.1016/S0272-7358(02)00130-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freud, S. (1975). Jenseits des Lustprinzips [Beyond the pleasure principle]. In Mitscherlich, A., Richards, A., & Strachey, J. (Eds.), Studienausgabe (Vol. 3). Psychologie des Unbewußten [Psychology of the unconscious] (pp. 213272). Frankfurt am Main, Germany: S. Fischer.Google Scholar
Frith, U. (1989). Autism: Explaining the enigma. Oxford, UK: Basil Blackwell.Google Scholar
Froese, T., & Fuchs, T. (2012). The extended body: A case study in the neurophenomenology of social interaction. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), 205236. doi:10.1007/s11097-012-9254-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuchs, T. (2000). Psychopathologie von Leib und Raum. Phänomenologisch-empirische Untersuchungen zu depressiven und paranoiden Erkrankungen [Psychopathology of body and space. Phenomenological-empirical investigations in depressive and paranoid disorders]. Darmstadt, Germany: Steinkopff.Google Scholar
Fuchs, T. (2001). Melancholia as a desynchronization. Towards a psychopathology of interpersonal time. Psychopathology, 34(4), 179186. doi:10.1159/000049304Google Scholar
Fuchs, T. (2005). Corporealized and disembodied minds. A phenomenological view of the body in melancholia and schizophrenia. Philosophy, Psychiatry & Psychology, 12(2), 95107. Retrieved from https://muse.jhu.edu/article/190379Google Scholar
Fuchs, T. (2007). The temporal structure of intentionality and its disturbance in schizophrenia. Psychopathology, 40(4), 229235. doi:10.1159/000101365CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fuchs, T. (2011). The psychopathology of hyperreflexivity. Journal of Speculative Philosophy, 24(3), 239255. doi:10.1353/jsp.2010.0010Google Scholar
Fuchs, T. (2012). The phenomenology of body memory. In Koch, S., Fuchs, T., Summa, M., & Müller, C. (Eds.), Body memory, metaphor and movement (pp. 922). Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Fuchs, T. (2013a). Existential vulnerability. Toward a psychopathology of limit situations. Psychopathology, 46(5), 301308. doi:10.1159/000351838CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuchs, T. (2013b). Temporality and psychopathology. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 12(1), 75104. doi:10.1007/s11097-010-9189-4Google Scholar
Fuchs, T. (2017). Self across time: The diachronic unity of bodily existence. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 16(2), 291315. doi:10.1007/s11097-015-9449-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuchs, T. (2018a). Ecology of the brain. The phenomenology and biology of the embodied mind. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fuchs, T. (2018b). The cyclical time of the body and its relation to linear time. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 25(7–8), 4765. Retrieved from www.ingentaconnect.com/content/imp/jcs/2018/00000025/f0020007/art00003Google Scholar
Fuchs, T. (2019a). Body memory and the unconscious. In Gipps, R. & Lacewing, M. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of philosophy and psychoanalysis (pp. 457470). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fuchs, T. (2019b). The life-world of persons with mood disorders. In Stanghellini, G., Broome, M., Raballo, A., Fernandez, A. V., Fusar-Poli, P., & Rosfort, R. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of phenomenological psychopathology (pp. 617633). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fuchs, T., & De Jaegher, H. (2009). Enactive intersubjectivity: Participatory sense-making and mutual incorporation. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 8(4), 465486. doi:10.1007/s11097-009-9136-4Google Scholar
Fuchs, T., & Koch, S. (2014). Embodied affectivity: On moving and being moved. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 112. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00508Google Scholar
Fuchs, T., & Schlimme, J. (2009). Embodiment and psychopathology: A phenomenological perspective. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 22(6), 570575. doi:10.1097/YCO.0b013e3283318e5cGoogle Scholar
Gaete, I. M., & Fuchs, T. (2016). From body image to emotional bodily experience in eating disorders. Journal of Phenomenological Psychology, 47(1), 1740. doi:10.1163/15691624-12341303Google Scholar
Gallagher, S. (2004). Understanding interpersonal problems in autism: Interaction theory as an alternative to Theory of Mind. Philosophy, Psychiatry & Psychology, 11(3), 199217. doi:10.1353/ppp.2004.0063CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gallagher, S. (2005). How the body shapes the mind. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gendlin, E. T. (2012). Focusing-oriented psychotherapy: A manual of the experiential method. New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Gepner, B., & Mestre, D. (2002). Rapid visual-motion integration deficit in autism. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(11), 455. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(02)02004-1Google Scholar
Gill, S. P. (2012). Rhythmic synchrony and mediated interaction: Towards a framework of rhythm in embodied interaction. AI & Society, 27(1), 111127. doi:10.1007/s00146-011-0362-2CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hackmann, A., Ehlers, A., Speckens, A., & Clark, D. M. (2004). Characteristics and content of intrusive memories in PTSD and their changes with treatment. Journal of Traumatic Stress, 17(3), 231240. doi:10.1023/B:JOTS.0000029266.88369.fdGoogle Scholar
Happé, F. (1995). Autism: An introduction to psychological theory. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Heidegger, M. (1962). Being and time (Macquarrie, J. & Robinson, E., Trans.). New York, NY: Harper and Row.Google Scholar
Henry, M. (2012). Philosophy and phenomenology of the body (Etzkorn, G., Trans.). Berlin, Germany: Springer. (Original work published 1975)Google Scholar
Hildebrandt, M. K., Koch, S. C., & Fuchs, T. (2016). “We dance and find each other”: Effects of dance/movement therapy on negative symptoms in autism spectrum disorder. Behavioral Sciences, 6(4), 117. doi:10.3390/bs6040024CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hobson, R. P., & Lee, A. (1998). Hello and goodbye: A study of social engagement in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 28(2), 117127. doi:10.1023/A:1026088531558Google Scholar
Hobson, R. P., & Lee, A. (1999). Imitation and identification in autism. The Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 40(4), 649659. doi:10.1111/1469-7610.00481Google Scholar
Husserl, E. (1952). Ideen zu einer reinen Phänomenologie und phänomenologischen Philosophie. II. Phänomenologische Untersuchungen zur Konstitution. [Ideas pertaining to a pure phenomenology and to a phenomenological philosophy. II. Studies in the phenomenology of constitution]. The Hague, Netherlands: Martinus Nijhoff.Google Scholar
Husserl, E. (1991). On the phenomenology of the consciousness of internal time (Brough, J., Trans.). Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer Academic.Google Scholar
Issartel, J., Marin, L., & Cadopi, M. (2007). Unintended interpersonal coordination: “Can we march to the beat of our own drum?” Neuroscience Letters, 411(3), 174179. doi:10.1016/j.neulet.2006.09.086Google Scholar
Jaspers, K. (1919). Psychologie der Weltanschauungen [Psychology of worldviews]. Berlin, Germany: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jonas, H. (1963). The phenomenon of life. Toward a philosophical biology. New York, NY: Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Jones, W., Carr, K., & Klin, A. (2008). Absence of preferential looking to the eyes of approaching adults predicts level of social disability in 2-year-old toddlers with autism spectrum disorder. Archives of General Psychiatry, 65(8), 946954. doi:10.1001/archpsyc.65.8.946CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaplan, L. J. (1984). Adolescence: The farewell to childhood. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Kington, J. M., Jones, L. A., Watt, A. A., Hopkin, E. J., & Williams, J. (2000). Impaired eye expression recognition in schizophrenia. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 34(4–5), 341347. doi:10.1016/S0022-3956(00)00029-7Google Scholar
Kitamura, T., & Kumar, R. (1982). Time passes slowly for patients with depressive state. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 65(6), 415420. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0447.1982.tb00865.xGoogle Scholar
Kiverstein, J. (2010). Making sense of phenomenal unity: An intentionalist account of temporal experience. Royal Institute of Philosophy Supplements, 67, 155181. doi:10.1017/S1358246110000081Google Scholar
Klin, A., Jones, W., Schultz, R., & Volkmar, F. (2003). The enactive mind, or from actions to cognition: Lessons from autism. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 358(1430), 345360. doi:10.1098/rstb.2002.1202Google Scholar
Klin, A., Lin, D. J., Gorrindo, P., Ramsay, G., & Jones, W. (2009). Two-year-olds with autism orient to non-social contingencies rather than biological motion. Nature, 459(7244), 257261. doi:10.1038/nature07868Google Scholar
Koch, S. C., Mehl, L., Sobanski, E., Sieber, M., & Fuchs, T. (2015). Fixing the mirrors: A feasibility study of the effects of dance movement therapy on young adults with autism spectrum disorder. Autism, 19(3), 338350. doi:10.1177/1362361314522353CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kornmeier, J., Ehm, W., Bigalke, H., & Bach, M. (2007). Discontinuous presentation of ambiguous figures: How interstimulus-interval durations affect reversal dynamics and ERPs. Psychophysiology, 44(4), 552560. doi:10.1111/j.1469-8986.2007.00525.xGoogle Scholar
Laroche, J., Berardi, A. M., & Brangier, E. (2014). Embodiment of intersubjective time: Relational dynamics as attractors in the temporal coordination of interpersonal behaviors and experiences. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 117. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01180Google Scholar
Leder, D. (1990). The absent body. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Legrand, D. (2010). Subjective and physical dimensions of bodily self-consciousness, and their dis-integration in anorexia nervosa. Neuropsychologia, 48(3), 726737. doi:10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2009.08.026CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Li, J. Z., Bunney, B. G., Meng, F., Hagenauer, M. H., Walsh, D. M., Vawter, M. P., … Bunney, W. E. (2013). Circadian patterns of gene expression in the human brain and disruption in major depressive disorder. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(24), 99509955. doi:10.1073/pnas.1305814110Google Scholar
Maduell, M., & Wing, A. M. (2007). The dynamics of ensemble: The case for flamenco. Psychology of Music, 35(4), 591627. doi:10.1177/0305735607076446CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malloch, S. N. (1999). Mothers and infants and communicative musicality. Musicae Scientiae, 3(1, Suppl.), 2957. doi:10.1177/10298649000030S104Google Scholar
Malloch, S. N., & Trevarthen, C. (Eds.). (2009). Communicative musicality: Narratives of expressive gesture and being human. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Markowitsch, H., & Welzer, H. (2009). The development of autobiographical memory. London, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Martin, L. M., Koch, S. C., Hirjak, D., & Fuchs, T. (2016). Overcoming disembodiment: The effect of movement therapy on negative symptoms in schizophrenia – A multicenter randomized controlled trial. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 114. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00483.Google Scholar
McFarland, D. H. (2001). Respiratory markers of conversational interaction. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 44(1), 128143. doi:10.1044/1092-4388(2001/012)Google Scholar
McGhie, A., & Chapman, J. (1961). Disorders of attention and perception in early schizophrenia. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 34(2), 103116. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8341.1961.tb00936.xGoogle Scholar
Merleau-Ponty, M. (1962). Phenomenology of perception (Smith, C., Trans.). London, UK: Routledge and Kegan Paul. (Original work published 1945)Google Scholar
Merleau-Ponty, M. (1968). The visible and the invisible (Lingis, A., Trans.). Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press. (Original work published 1964)Google Scholar
Minkowski, E. (1958). Findings in a case of schizophrenic depression. In May, R., Angel, E., & Ellenberger, H. (Eds.), Existence. A new dimension in psychiatry and psychology (pp. 127139). New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Minkowski, E. (1970). Lived time: Phenomenological and psychopathological studies (Metzel, N., Trans.). Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar
Nadel, J., Carchon, I., Kervella, C., Marcelli, D., & Réserbat-Plantey, D. (1999). Expectancies for social contingency in 2-month-olds. Developmental Science, 2(2), 164173. doi:10.1111/1467-7687.00065Google Scholar
Nielsen, K., & Ward, T. (2018). Towards a new conceptual framework for psychopathology: Embodiment, enactivism, and embedment. Theory & Psychology, 28(6), 800822. doi:10.1177/0959354318808394Google Scholar
Plessner, H. (1928). Die Stufen des Organischen und der Mensch [Levels of organic life and the human]. Berlin, Germany: De Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pöppel, E. (1997). A hierarchical model of temporal perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1(2), 5661. doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(97)01008-5Google Scholar
Pöppel, E. (2000). Grenzen des Bewusstseins. Wie kommen wir zur Zeit, und wie entsteht Wirklichkeit? [Limits of consciousness. How do we get to time and how does reality emerge?] Frankfurt am Main, Germany: Insel.Google Scholar
Ramseyer, F., & Tschacher, W. (2011). Nonverbal synchrony in psychotherapy: Coordinated body movement reflects relationship quality and outcome. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 79(3), 284295. doi:10.1037/a0023419CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Röhricht, F., & Priebe, S. (2006). Effect of body-oriented psychological therapy on negative symptoms in schizophrenia: A randomized controlled trial. Psychological Medicine, 36(5), 669678. doi:10.1017/S0033291706007161Google Scholar
Rümke, H. C. (1990). The nuclear symptom of schizophrenia and the praecox feeling (J. Neelman, Trans.). History of Psychiatry, 1(3), 331341. (Original work published 1941) doi:10.1177/0957154X9000100304CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sartre, J.-P. (1969). Being and nothingness: An essay on phenomenological ontology (Barnes, H. E., Trans.). New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Sass, L. A. (1992). Madness and modernism. Insanity in the light of modern art, literature, and thought. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Sass, L. A., & Parnas, J. (2003). Schizophrenia, consciousness, and the self. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 29(3), 427444. doi:10.1093/oxfordjournals.schbul.a007017Google Scholar
Seikkula, J., Alakare, B., & Aaltonen, J. (2011). The comprehensive open-dialogue approach in Western Lapland: II. Long-term stability of acute psychosis outcomes in advanced community care. Psychosis, 3(3), 192204. doi:10.1080/17522439.2011.595819CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sheridan, J., & McAuley, J. D. (1997). Rhythm as a cognitive skill: Temporal processing deficits in autism. Paper presented at the Fourth Australiasian Cognitive Science Conference, Newcastle, NSW. Retrieved from http://citeseerx.ist.psu.edu/viewdoc/download;jsessionid=A0658E4D38992A0792D3F274AB602585?doi=10.1.1.54.4592&rep=rep1&type=pdfGoogle Scholar
Smith, I. M., & Bryson, S. E. (1994). Imitation and action in autism: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 116(2), 259273. doi:10.1037/0033-2909.116.2.259CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stanghellini, G. (2004). Disembodied spirits and deanimated bodies: The psychopathology of common sense. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stanghellini, G., Ballerini, M., Presenza, S., Mancini, M., Raballo, A., Blasi, S., & Cutting, J. (2016). Psychopathology of lived time: Abnormal time experience in persons with schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 42(1), 4555. doi:10.1093/schbul/sbv052Google ScholarPubMed
Stern, D. N. (1985). The interpersonal world of the infant: A view from psychoanalysis and developmental psychology. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Stern, D. N. (2004). The present moment in psychotherapy and everyday life. New York, NY: W. W. Norton & Company.Google Scholar
Stern, D. N. (2010). Forms of vitality: Exploring dynamic experience in psychology, the arts, psychotherapy, and development. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Street, R. L. Jr. (1984). Speech convergence and speech evaluation in fact-finding interviews. Human Communication Research, 11(2), 139169. doi:10.1111/j.1468-2958.1984.tb00043.xGoogle Scholar
Szelag, E., von Steinbüchel, N., Reiser, M., Gilles de Langen, E., & Pöppel, E. (1996). Temporal constraints in processing of nonverbal rhythmic patterns. Acta Neurobiologiae Experimentalis, 56(1), 215225.Google Scholar
Tellenbach, H. (1980). Melancholy. History of the problem, endogeneity, typology, pathogenesis, clinical considerations (Eng, E., Trans.). Pittsburgh, PA: Duquesne University Press.Google Scholar
Thoma, S., & Fuchs, T. (2018). A phenomenology of sensus communis. Outline of a phenomenological approach to social psychiatry. In Englander, M. (Ed.), Phenomenology and the social context of psychiatry: Social relations, psychopathology, and Husserl's philosophy (pp. 137159). London, UK: Bloomsbury.Google Scholar
Trevarthen, C., & Daniel, S. (2005). Disorganized rhythm and synchrony: Early signs of autism and Rett syndrome. Brain and Development, 27(Suppl. 1), S25S34. doi:10.1016/j.braindev.2005.03.016Google Scholar
Van der Kolk, B. (2014). The body keeps the score: Mind, brain and body in the transformation of trauma. London, UK: Penguin.Google Scholar
Waller, G., Sines, J., Meyer, C., Foster, E., & Skelton, A. (2007). Narcissism and narcissistic defenses in the eating disorders. International Journal of Eating Disorders, 40(2), 143148. doi:10.1002/eat.20345Google Scholar
Wittmann, M. (2013). The inner sense of time: How the brain creates a representation of duration. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14(3), 217223. doi:10.1038/nrn3452Google Scholar
Wittmann, M. (2015). Modulations of the experience of self and time. Consciousness and Cognition, 38, 172181. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2015.06.008Google Scholar
Zahavi, D. (1999). Self-awareness and alterity. A phenomenological investigation. Evanston, IL: Northwestern University Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×