Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gvvz8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T02:51:22.240Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - Data-collection methods

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 April 2013

Natalie Schilling
Affiliation:
Georgetown University, Washington DC
Get access

Summary

In this chapter we cover a range of data-collection methods and instruments, from surveys conducted via questionnaires to sociolinguistic interviews to ethnographic participant-observation. In addition to discussing methods for obtaining data on variable language production, we discuss techniques for obtaining data on perception from within a sociolinguistic framework, including issues related to listeners’ identification of features, varieties, and speakers, as well as listener attitudes toward those who use particular features, varieties, and codes. In each case, we discuss both designing and implementing the various techniques, as well as the advantages, disadvantages, and purposes of each. At issue throughout is the question of “authenticity,” and we consider whether it is possible or even desirable to seek to remove researcher effects, in an effort to overcome Labov’s observer’s paradox: “To obtain the data that is most important for linguistic theory, we have to observe how people speak when they are not being observed” (1972a: 113). This chapter concludes with the recommendation that, wherever possible, researchers should seek to use a variety of data-collection methods in each of their studies since, as Labov so aptly put it in the earliest days of variationist sociolinguistics, we most closely approach sociolinguistic truth “by convergence of several kinds of data with complementary sources of error” (Labov 1972a: 97).

Method of administration: Face-to-face or long distance

The earliest dialect studies were conducted via survey questionnaires designed to elicit information on a range of language forms – lexical, phonological, and grammatical. A few early studies employed postal questionnaires, including the first dialect geographic study, conducted in Germany by Wenker, beginning in 1876. However, researchers soon decided that it would be advantageous on a number of levels to send out trained fieldworkers to administer questionnaires in person, and until recent decades, most sociolinguistic and dialectological studies that included a survey component relied on face-to-face rather than long-distance methods. Face-to-face surveys allow more control over who the respondents are, since we cannot know from long-distance surveys whether claimed demographic and other characteristics (e.g. attitudes) are accurate. In addition, administering surveys in person allows us to record responses ourselves (through on-the-spot phonetic transcription or audio recording), to record variable as well as categorical usages (since a single respondent may give more than one response to an elicitation in person but probably not on paper), and to provide and request clarification. In addition, because in-person surveys are usually administered orally, respondents may be more relaxed than when filling out a written form and so produce more naturalistic data.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2013

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Wolfram, Walt and Fasold, Ralph 1974. Field Methods in the Study of Social Dialects. Chapter 4 in The Study of Social Dialects in American English. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall.Google Scholar
Labov, William 1984. Field Methods of the Project on Linguistic Change and Variation. In Baugh, John and Sherzer, Joel (eds.), Language in Use. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 28–53.Google Scholar
Wolfson, Nessa 1976. Speech Events and Natural Speech: Some Implications for Sociolinguistic Methodology. Language in Society 5: 189–209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Briggs, Charles L. 1986. Learning How to Ask: A Sociolinguistic Appraisal of the Role of the Interview in Social Science Research. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell-Kibler, Kathryn 2010. Sociolinguistics and Perception. Language and Linguistics Compass 4(6): 377–389.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duranti, Alessandro 1997. Linguistic Anthropology. Cambridge University Press. See especially Chapter 4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saville-Troike, Muriel 2003. The Ethnography of Communication. Malden, MA: Blackwell. See especially Chapter 4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Data-collection methods
  • Natalie Schilling, Georgetown University, Washington DC
  • Book: Sociolinguistic Fieldwork
  • Online publication: 05 April 2013
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511980541.003
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • Data-collection methods
  • Natalie Schilling, Georgetown University, Washington DC
  • Book: Sociolinguistic Fieldwork
  • Online publication: 05 April 2013
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511980541.003
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • Data-collection methods
  • Natalie Schilling, Georgetown University, Washington DC
  • Book: Sociolinguistic Fieldwork
  • Online publication: 05 April 2013
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511980541.003
Available formats
×