Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-g7gxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-04T18:29:26.518Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

12 - Intergroup Contact and the Central Role of Affect in Intergroup Prejudice

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 April 2011

Linda R. Tropp
Affiliation:
Boston College
Thomas F. Pettigrew
Affiliation:
University of California, Santa Cruz
Larissa Z. Tiedens
Affiliation:
Stanford University, California
Colin Wayne Leach
Affiliation:
University of California, Santa Cruz
Get access

Summary

Decades of research have studied the role of intergroup contact in reducing intergroup prejudice (see Allport, 1954; Hewstone & Brown, 1986; Pettigrew & Tropp, 2000, 2003), yet little consensus has emerged regarding its effects. Past reviews of this extensive literature have reached sharply conflicting conclusions. Some indicate that intergroup contact leads to positive changes in intergroup prejudice, especially when the contact occurs under optimal conditions (e.g., Jackson, 1993; Pettigrew, 1971, 1998; Riordan, 1978). Others take a more critical stance. They hold that intergroup contact has relatively little or no effect on broad-scale changes in intergroup prejudice (e.g., Amir, 1976; Ford, 1986; Rothbart & John, 1985).

At first blush, these perspectives appear to be fundamentally inconsistent, reflecting the long-standing debates that have engulfed this research literature during the last half century. We believe this divergence in perspectives has grown out of an overemphasis on the general question of whether intergroup contact will reduce prejudice, with the phenomenon of intergroup prejudice construed as a single dimension. Close examination of the research contributing to these perspectives suggests that many different components of intergroup prejudice – including both affective and cognitive components – must be considered as potential outcomes of intergroup contact. Thus, it may be that different branches of the research literature have emphasized different aspects of the intergroup relationship. We pursue this possibility in this chapter, and we propose that affective dimensions of intergroup relationships are especially important for understanding the effects of contact on intergroup attitudes.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2004

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adorno, T. W., Frenkel-Brunswik, E., Levinson, D. J., & Sanford, R. N. (1950). The authoritarian personality. New York: Harper
Allport, G. W. (1954). The nature of prejudice. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley
Amir, Y. (1976). The role of intergroup contact in change of prejudice and ethnic relations. In P. A. Katz (Ed.), Toward the elimination of racism (pp. 245–308). Elmsford, NY: Pergamon PressCrossRef
Ashmore, R. D., & Del Boca, F. K. (1981). Conceptual approaches to stereotypes and stereotyping. In D. L. Hamilton (Ed.), Cognitive processes in stereotyping and intergroup behavior (pp. 1–35). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Batson, C. D., Polycarpou, M. P., Harmon-Jones, E., Imhoff, H. J., Mitchener, E. C., Bednar, L. L., Klein, T. R., & Highberger, L. (1997). Empathy and attitudes: Can feelings for a member of a stigmatized group improve feelings toward the group?Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 105–118CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bogardus, E. S. (1928). Immigration and race attitudes. Boston: Heath
Breckler, S. J., & Wiggins, E. C. (1989). Affect versus evaluation in the structure of attitudes. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 25, 253–271CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brewer, M. B., Campbell, D. T., & Levine, R. A. (1971). Cross-cultural test of the relationship between affect and evaluation. Proceedings of the Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, 6, 213–214Google Scholar
Brigham, J. C. (1993). College students' racial attitudes. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 23, 1933–1967CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Britt, T. W., Boniecki, K. A., Vescio, T. K., & Biernat, M. (1996). Intergroup anxiety: A person × situation approach. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 1177–1188CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, R., Condor, S., Mathews, A., Wade, G., & Williams, J. (1986). Explaining intergroup differentiation in an industrial organization. Journal of Occupational Psychology, 59, 273–286CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cook, S. W. (1984). Cooperative interaction in multiethnic contexts. In N. Miller & M. B. Brewer (Eds.), Groups in contact: The psychology of desegregation (pp. 155–185). Orlando: Academic PressCrossRef
Deutsch, M., & Collins, M. E. (1951). Interracial housing: A psychological evaluation of a social experiment. Minneapolis: University of Minnesota Press
Devine, P. G., & Vasquez, K. A. (1998). The rocky road to positive intergroup relations. In J. L. Eberhardt & S. T. Fiske (Eds.), Confronting racism: The problem and the response (pp. 234–262). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage
Dunton, B. C., & Fazio, R. H. (1997). Categorization by race: The impact of automatic and controlled components of racial prejudice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 451–470Google Scholar
Eagly, A. H., & Chaiken, S. (1993). The psychology of attitudes. Fort Worth: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich
Esses, V. M., & Dovidio, J. F. (2002). The role of emotions in determining willingness to engage in intergroup contact. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1202–1214CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Esses, V. M., Haddock, G., & Zanna, M. P. (1993). Values, stereotypes, and emotions as determinants of intergroup attitudes. In D. Mackie & D. Hamilton (Eds.), Affect, cognition, and stereotyping: Interactive processes in group perception (pp. 137–166). San Diego: Academic Press
Ford, W. S. (1986). Favorable intergroup contact may not reduce prejudice: Inconclusive journal evidence, 1960–1984. Sociology and Social Research, 70, 256–258Google Scholar
Hamberger, J., & Hewstone, M. (1997). Inter-ethnic contact as a predictor of blatant and subtle prejudice: Tests of a model in four West European nations. British Journal of Social Psychology, 36, 173–190CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamilton, D. L. (Ed.). (1981a). Cognitive processes in stereotyping and intergroup behavior. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Hamilton, D. L. (1981b). Stereotyping and intergroup behavior: Some thoughts on the cognitive approach. In D. L. Hamilton (Ed.), Cognitive processes in stereotyping and intergroup behavior (pp. 333–353). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
Hamilton, D. L., Stroessner, S. J., & Driscoll, D. M. (1994). Social cognition and the study of stereotyping. In P. G. Devine, D. L. Hamilton, & Ostrom, T. M. (Eds.), Social cognition: Impact on social psychology (pp. 291–321). New York: Academic Press
Herek, G. M., & Capitanio, J. P. (1996). “Some of my best friends”: Intergroup contact, concealable stigma, and heterosexuals' attitudes toward gay men and lesbians. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 22, 412–424CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hewstone, M., & Brown, R. (1986). Contact is not enough: An intergroup perspective on the ‘contact hypothesis.’ In M. Hewstone & R. Brown (Eds.), Contact and conflict in intergroup encounters (pp. 1–44). Oxford, UK: Basil Blackwell
Islam, M. R., & Hewstone, M. (1993). Dimensions of contact as predictors of intergroup anxiety, perceived outgroup variability, and outgroup attitude: An integrative model. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 19, 700–710CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jackson, J. W. (1993). Contact theory of intergroup hostility: A review and evaluation of the theoretical and empirical literature. International Journal of Group Tensions, 23, 43–65Google Scholar
Johnson, B. T., & Eagly, A. H. (2000). Quantitative synthesis of social psychological research. In H. T. Reis & C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social psychology. London: Cambridge University Press
Katz, I., & Hass, R. G. (1988). Racial ambivalence and American value conflict. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55, 893–905CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kinder, D. R., & Sanders, L. M. (1996). Divided by color: Racial politics and democratic ideals. Chicago: University of Chicago Press
Luiz, D., & Krige, P. (1981). The effect of social contact between South African white and colored adolescent girls. Journal of Social Psychology, 113, 153–158CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mackie, D. M., & Hamilton, D. L. (1993a). Affect, cognition, and stereotyping. San Diego: Academic Press
Mackie, D. M., & Hamilton, D. L. (1993b). Affect, cognition, and stereotyping: Concluding comments. In D. M. Mackie & D. L. Hamilton (Eds.), Affect, cognition, and stereotyping: Interactive processes in group perception (pp. 371–383). San Diego: Academic Press
Mackie, D. M., & Smith, E. R. (1998). Intergroup relations: Insights from a theoretically integrative approach. Psychological Review, 105, 499–529CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mackie, D. M., & Smith, E. R. (2002). Intergroup emotions and the social self: Prejudice reconceptualized as differentiated reactions to outgroups. In J. P. Forgas & K. D. Williams (Eds.), The social self: Cognitive, interpersonal, and intergroup perspectives (pp. 309–326). New York: Psychology Press
Mann, J. H. (1959). The relationship between cognitive, affective, and behavioral aspects of racial prejudice. Journal of Social Psychology, 49, 223–228CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McConahay, J. B., Hardee, B. B., & Batts, V. (1981). Has racism declined in America? It depends on who is asking and what is asked. Journal of Conflict Resolution, 25, 563–579CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLaughlin-Volpe, T., Aron, A., Wright, S. C., & Reis, H. T. (2000). Intergroup social interaction and intergroup prejudice: Quantity versus quality. Manuscript submitted for publication
NES Guide to Public Opinion and Electoral Behavior, 1995–2000. Ann Arbor: National Election Studies, Center for Political Studies, University of Michigan. Retrieved from: http://www.umich.edu/∼nes/nesguide/nesguide.htm
Osgood, C. E., Suci, G. J., & Tannenbaum, P. H. (1957). The measurement of meaning. Urbana, IL: University of Illinois Press
Ostrom, T. M. (1969). The relationship between the affective, behavioral, and cognitive components of attitude. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 5, 12–30CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ostrom, T. M., Skowronski, J. J., & Nowak, A. (1994). The cognitive foundation of attitudes: It's a wonderful construct. In P. G. Devine, D. L. Hamilton, & Ostrom, T. M. (Eds.), Social cognition: Impact on social psychology (pp. 195–258). New York: Academic Press
Patchen, M., Davidson, J. D., Hofmann, G., & Brown, W. R. (1977). Determinants of students' interracial behavior and opinion change. Sociology of Education, 50, 55–75CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pettigrew, T. F. (1971). Racially separate or together? New York: McGraw-Hill
Pettigrew, T. F. (1981). Extending the stereotype concept. In D. L. Hamilton (Ed.), Cognitive processes in stereotyping and intergroup behavior (pp. 303–331). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Pettigrew, T. F. (1997a). Generalized intergroup contact effects on prejudice. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 173–185CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pettigrew, T. F. (1997b). The affective component of prejudice: Empirical support of the new view. In S. A. Tuch & J. K. Martin (Eds.), Racial attitudes in the 1990s: Continuity and change (pp. 76–90). Westport, CT: Praeger
Pettigrew, T. F. (1998). Intergroup contact theory. Annual Review of Psychology, 49, 65–85CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pettigrew, T. F., & Tropp, L. R. (2000). Does intergroup contact reduce prejudice? Recent meta-analytic findings. In S. Oskamp (Ed.), Reducing prejudice and discrimination: The Claremont Symposium on Applied Social Psychology (pp. 93–114). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum
Pettigrew, T. F., & Tropp, L. R. (2003). A meta-analytic test of intergroup contact theory. Manuscript submitted for publication
Plant, E. A., & Devine, P. G. (1998). Internal and external motivation to respond without prejudice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 811–832CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riordan, C. (1978). Equal-status interracial contact: A review and revision of the concept. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 2, 161–185CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Riordan, C. (1987). Intergroup contact in small cities. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 11, 143–154CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rojahn, K., & Pettigrew, T. F. (1992). Memory for schema-relevant information: A meta-analysis resolution. British Journal of Social Psychology, 31, 81–109CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosenthal, R. (1991). Meta-analytic procedures for social research. Newbury Park: Sage
Rosenthal, R. (1995). Writing meta-analytic reviews. Psychological Bulletin, 118, 183–192CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothbart, M. (1996). Category-exemplar dynamics and stereotype change. International Journal of Intercultural Relations, 20, 305–321CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothbart, M., & John, O. P. (1985). Social categorization and behavioral episodes: A cognitive analysis of the effects of intergroup contact. Journal of Social Issues, 41, 81–104CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rothbart, M., & John, O. P. (1993). Intergroup relations and stereotype change: A social-cognitive analysis and some longitudinal findings. In P. M. Sniderman, P. E. Tetlock, & E. G. Carmines (Eds.), Prejudice, politics, and the American dilemma (pp. 32–59). Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press
Rothbart, M., & Lewis, S. (1994). Cognitive processes and intergroup relations: A historical perspective. In P. G. Devine, D. L. Hamilton, & Ostrom, T. M. (Eds.), Social cognition: Impact on social psychology (pp. 347–382). New York: Academic Press
Rothbart, M., Sriram, N., & Davis-Stitt, C. (1996). The retrieval of typical and atypical category members. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 32, 309–336CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, E. R. (1993). Social identity and social emotions: Toward new conceptualizations of prejudice. In D. Mackie & D. Hamilton (Eds.), Affect, cognition, and stereotyping: Interactive processes in group perception (pp. 137–166). San Diego: Academic PressCrossRef
Stangor, C., Jonas, K., Stroebe, W., & Hewstone, M. (1996). Influence of student exchange on national stereotypes, attitudes, and perceived group variability. European Journal of Social Psychology, 26, 663–6753.0.CO;2-6>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stangor, C., Sullivan, L. A., & Ford, T. E. (1991). Affective and cognitive determinants of prejudice. Social Cognition, 9, 359–380CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stephan, W. G., Boniecki, K. A., Ybarra, O., Bettencourt, A., Ervin, K. S., Jackson, L. A., McNatt, P. S., & Renfro, C. S. (2002). The role of threats in the racial attitudes of blacks and whites. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1242–1254CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stephan, W. G., & Finlay, K. (1999). The role of empathy in improving intergroup relations. Journal of Social Issues, 55, 729–743CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stephan, W. G., & Stephan, C. W. (1985). Intergroup anxiety. Journal of Social Issues, 41, 157–175CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stephan, W. G., & Stephan, C. W. (1993). Cognition and affect in stereotyping: Parallel interactive networks. In D. M. Mackie & D. L. Hamilton (Eds.), Affect, cognition, and stereotyping: Interactive processes in group perception (pp. 111–136). San Diego: Academic PressCrossRef
Tajfel, H. (1969). Cognitive aspects of prejudice. Journal of Social Issues, 25, 79–97CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tajfel, H. (1981). Human groups and social categories. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press
Taylor, S. (1981). A categorization approach to stereotyping. In D. Hamilton (Ed.), Cognitive processes in stereotyping and intergroup behavior (pp. 83–114). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum
Tropp, L. R. (2003). The psychological impact of prejudice: Implications for intergroup contact. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 6, 131–149CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tropp, L. R., & Pettigrew, T. F. (2004). Examining relationships between intergroup contact and multiple indicators of prejudice. Manuscript submitted for publication
Vorauer, J. D., Main, K. J., & O'Connell, G. B. (1998). How do individuals expect to be viewed by members of lower status groups? Content and implications of meta-stereotypes. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 917–937CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walker, I., & Smith, H. (Eds.). (2001). Relative deprivation: Specification, development and integration. New York: Cambridge University Press
Weber, R., & Crocker, J. (1983). Cognitive processes in the revision of stereotypic beliefs. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 45, 961–977CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilder, D. A. (1984). Intergroup contact: The typical member and the exception to the rule. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 20, 177–194CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilder, D. A. (1986). Cognitive factors affecting the success of intergroup contact. In S. W. Worchel & W. G. Austin (Eds.), Psychology of intergroup relations (pp. 49–66). Chicago: Nelson-Hall
Wright, S. C., Aron, A., & Tropp, L. R. (2002). Including others (and groups) in the self: Self-expansion and intergroup relations. In J. P. Forgas & K. D. Williams (Eds.), The social self: Cognitive, interpersonal, and intergroup perspectives (pp. 343–363). New York: Psychology Press
Wright, S. C., & Van der Zande, C. C. (October, 1999). Bicultural friends: When cross-group friendships cause improved intergroup attitudes. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Society for Experimental Psychology, St. Louis
Zajonc, R. B. (1980). Feeling and thinking: Preferences need no inferences. American Psychologist, 35, 151–175CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zanna, M. P., & Rempel, J. K. (1988). Attitudes: A new look at an old concept. In D. Bar Tal & A. Kruglanski (Eds.), The social psychology of knowledge (pp. 315–334). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×