Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-t5tsf Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-02T19:18:11.771Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

12 - A Foucauldian Analysis of the Learning Sciences: Past, Present, and Future

from PART 3 - FUTURE

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 February 2016

R. Keith Sawyer
Affiliation:
University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill
Michael A. Evans
Affiliation:
North Carolina State University
Martin J. Packer
Affiliation:
Universidad de los Andes, Colombia
R. Keith Sawyer
Affiliation:
University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill
Get access

Summary

We must also question those divisions or groupings with which we have become so familiar .... We are not even sure of ourselves when we use these distinctions in our own world of discourse ... we are analyzing groups of statements which, when first formulated, were distributed, divided, and characterized in a quite different way .... As soon as one questions that unity, it loses its self-evidence; it indicates itself, constructs itself, only on the basis of a complex field of discourse.

(Foucault, 1969/1972, pp. 22–23)

These chapters provide substantial insights into the guiding questions stated in the Introduction: “What is the learning sciences?” and “How did it come to be what it is, and what might it become in the future?” In addition, a more general contribution of this book is to help us answer a much more complex question: “What is a scientific discipline, and how does it form and change over time?” This is the realm of historians of science, who for decades have studied how new scientific disciplines, theories, and methodologies emerge and change over time. Such histories are even more challenging when we are speaking of interdisciplinary sciences – when elements from two or more distinct fields come together to form a new network of relations. This new network attains autonomy and independence, even as the elements remain linked to their foundational disciplines.

In these chapters, we read about the many intellectual currents that joined to form the learning sciences (LS). These currents bubble up, flow quickly or slowly, weave around obstacles, and change direction. They merge with other currents and sometimes synergistically become something very different. I use Foucault's theory (1966/1970, 1969/1972) to explain the interdisciplinary roots and the historical trajectory of LS – the multiple parallel scholarly currents that were active before the interdisciplinary LS emerged and became recognized as its own autonomous scholarly domain. I also use Foucault's theories to analyze the contemporary tensions and contradictions in the field, and discuss how these might drive change in the future. By grounding my analysis in Foucault's theory, I treat this book as a case study in the history of science, one that historians of science may find useful as they analyze other interdisciplinary sciences.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2016

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Beilin, H. (1989). Piagetian theory. Annals of Child Development, 6, 85–131.Google Scholar
Brieske, G. F., Lewis, B. N., Macdonald-Ross, M., & Smith, R. W. (1972). An opening statement from the editors. Instructional Science, 1(1), 1–7.Google Scholar
Foucault, M. (1966/1970). The order of things: An archeology of the human sciences. New York: Random House. Originally published as Les mots et les choses (Paris: Gallimard, 1966).Google Scholar
Foucault, M. (1969/1972). The archeology of knowledge and the discourse on language. New York: Pantheon Books. Originally published as L'Archéologie du Savoir (Paris: Editions Gallimard, 1969).Google Scholar
Gardner, H. (1983). The mind's new science: A history of the cognitive revolution. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Hoadley, C. M. (2004). Learning and design: Why the learning sciences and instructional systems need each other. Educational Technology, 44(3), 6–12.Google Scholar
Hoadley, C. M. (2005). The shape of the elephant: Scope and membership of the CSCL community. Paper presented at the CSCL 2005, Taipai, Taiwan.CrossRef
Kitchener, R. F. (1986). Piaget's theory of knowledge: Genetic epistemology & scientific reason. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kolodner, J. L. (1991). The Journal of the Learning Sciences: Effecting changes in education. The Journal of the Learning Sciences, 1(1), 1–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kolodner, J. L. (2009). Note from the outgoing editor-in-chief. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 18(1), 1–3.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koschmann, T. (1996a). Preface. In Koschmann, T. (Ed.), CSCL: Theory and practice of an emerging paradigm (pp. xi–xii). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Koschmann, T. (1996b). Paradigm shifts and instructional technology: An introduction. In Koschmann, T. (Ed.), CSCL: Theory and practice of an emerging paradigm (pp. 1–23). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Kuhn, T. S. (1962). The structure of scientific revolutions. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kuhn, T. S. (1977). The essential tension: Selected studies in scientific tradition and change. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Kuhn, T. S. (2000). The road since structure: Philosophical essays, 1970–1993, with an autobiographical interview. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lakatos, I., & Musgrave, A. (Eds.). (1970). Criticism and the growth of knowledge. London: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Machamer, P., & Osbeck, L. (2000). The new science of learning: Mechanisms, models, and muddles. Themes in Education (Greece), 1(1), 39–54.Google Scholar
Mayer, R. E. (1992). Cognition and instruction: Their historic meeting within educational psychology. Journal of Educational Psychology, 84(4), 405–412.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N., Kuhl, P. K., Movellan, J., & Seinowski, T. J. (2009). Foundations for a new science of learning. Science, 325(July), 284–288.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Messerly, J. G. (1996). Piaget's conception of evolution: Beyond Darwin and Lamarck. Lanham, MD: Rowman & Littlefield.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1950). Introduction a’ l’épistémologie génétique. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1968/1970). Structuralism. New York: Basic Books. (Originally published as Le Structuralisme, Paris: Presses Universitaires de France, 1968).Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1970/1972). The principles of genetic epistemology. New York: Basic Books. (Originally published as L'Epistémologie génétique, Paris: Presses Universitaires de France, 1970).Google Scholar
Piaget, J., & Garcia, R. (1983/1989). Psychogenesis and the history of science (Feider, H., Trans.). New York: Columbia University Press.Google Scholar
Resnick, L. (1984). Editor's note. Cognition and Instruction, 1(1), 1–4.CrossRef
Rogoff, B. (1990). Apprenticeship in thinking: Cognitive development in social context. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Royer, J. M. (Ed.). (2005). The cognitive revolution in educational psychology: Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
Sawyer, R. K. (2002). Unresolved tensions in sociocultural theory: Analogies with contemporary sociological debates. Culture & Psychology, 8(3), 283–305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sawyer, R. K. (Ed.). (2014). The Cambridge handbook of the learning sciences, 2nd ed. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schunk, D. H. (2012). Learning theories: An educational perspective. Boston: Pearson.Google Scholar
Toulmin, S. (1972). Human understanding. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Tsou, J. Y. (2006). Genetic epistemology and Piaget's philosophy of science: Piaget vs. Kuhn on scientific progress. Theory & Psychology, 16, 203–224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×