Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T22:41:19.806Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

3 - The Point, the Shrug, and the Question of Clarification

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  10 January 2020

Lucas Payne Butler
Affiliation:
University of Maryland, College Park
Samuel Ronfard
Affiliation:
University of Toronto Mississauga
Kathleen H. Corriveau
Affiliation:
Boston University
Get access

Summary

Early studies of children’s questions focused primarily on the epistemic motive that prompted them. Increasingly, however, investigators have recognized that questions are situated within a dialogue in which the child’s interlocutor and the answers that the interlocutor supplies are likely to determine what children learn from their questions and the broader stance that children take to curiosity–driven conversations. Moreover, children do not ask questions with one single motive. Many of their questions are aimed at gathering information about the world but some are aimed at securing help, or permission or clarification of what has just been said. Even in the second year of life toddlers use nonverbal gestures such as pointing to seek input from a partner. Such gestures appear to be interrogative in the sense that information provided in their wake is especially well remembered. Toddlers also signal their ignorance again nonverbally via shrug gestures but increasingly via explicit admissions of ignorance: “I don’t know.” Finally, despite the alleged lack of metacognitive awareness among young children, they are prompt in seeking to resolve comprehension glitches by asking clarification questions.

Type
Chapter
Information
The Questioning Child
Insights from Psychology and Education
, pp. 29 - 50
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Acredolo, L. P., and Goodwyn, S. W. (1985). Symbolic gesturing in language development: A case study. Human Development, 28, 40–9. http://doi:10.1159/000272934CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Acredolo, L. P., and Goodwyn, S. W. (1988). Symbolic gesturing in normal infants. Child Development, 59, 450–66. http://doi:10.2307/1130324Google Scholar
Aviezer, O. (2003). Bedtime talk of three-year olds: Collaborative repair of miscommunication. First Language, 23, 117–39. http://doi:10.1177/0142723703023001006CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartsch, K., and Wellman, H. M. (1995). Children talk about the mind. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartz, D. T. (2017). Young children’s meta-ignorance. PhD thesis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University.Google Scholar
Begus, K., and Southgate, V. (2012). Infant pointing serves an interrogative function. Developmental Science, 15, 611–77. http://doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01160.xGoogle Scholar
Begus, K., Gliga, T., and Southgate, V. (2014). Infants learn what they want to learn: Responding to infant pointing leads to superior learning. PLOS ONE, 9. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0108817CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Begus, K., Gliga, T., and Southgate, V. (2016). Infants’ preferences for native speakers are associated with an expectation of information. PNAS, 113, 12397–402. http://doi:10.1073/pnas.1603261113CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Behne, T., Carpenter, M., and Tomasello, M. (2014). Young children create iconic gestures to inform others. Developmental Psychology, 50, 2049–60. http://doi:10.1037/a0037224Google Scholar
Brown, R. (1973). A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Call, J., and Carpenter, M. (2001). Do apes and children know what they have seen? Animal Cognition, 3, 207–20. http://doi:10.1007/s100710100078CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chouinard, M. (2007). Children’s questions: A mechanism for cognitive development. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, serial no. 286, 72 (no. 1).Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. London: Murray.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dingemanse, M., Roberts, S. G., Baranova, J., et al. (2015). Universal principles in the repair of communication problems. PLOS ONE, 10 (no. 9). http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0136100CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dörrenberg, S., Rakoczy, H., and Liszkowski, U. (2018). How (not) to measure infant theory of mind: Testing the replicability and validity of four non-verbal measures. Cognitive Development. http://doi:10.1016/j.cogdev.2018.01.001Google Scholar
Enfield, N. J. (2017). How we talk. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Flavell, J. H. (1979). Metacognition and cognitive monitoring: A new area of cognitive-developmental inquiry. American Psychologist, 34, 906–11. http://doi:10.1037/0003-066x.34.10.906Google Scholar
Gallagher, T. M. (1981). Contingent query sequences within adult–child discourse. Journal of Child Language, 8, 5162. http://doi:10.1017/s0305000900003007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldin-Meadow, S., Levine, S. C., Hedges, L. V., et al. (2014) New evidence about language and cognitive development based on a longitudinal study: Hypotheses for intervention. American Psychologist, 69, 588–99. http://doi:10.1037/a0036886Google Scholar
Gopnik, A. (1993). How we know our minds: The illusion of first-person knowledge of intentionality. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16, 114. http://doi:10.1017/s0140525x00028636Google Scholar
Goupil, L., Romand-Monnier, M., and Kouider, S. (2016). Infants ask for help when they don’t know. PNAS, 113, 3492–6. http://doi:10.1073/pnas.1515129113Google Scholar
Harris, P. L. (1996). Desires, beliefs and language. In Carruthers, P. and Smith, P. K. (eds.), Theories of theories of mind (pp. 200–20). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. http://doi:10.1017/cbo9780511597985.014Google Scholar
Harris, P. L. (2012). Trusting what you’re told: How children learn from others. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press/Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Harris, P. L. (2018). Revisiting privileged access. In Proust, J. and Fortier, M. (eds.), Metacognitive diversity: An interdisciplinary approach (pp. 8396). Oxford: Oxford University Press. http://doi:10.1093/oso/9780198789710.003.0005Google Scholar
Harris, P. L. (2019). Infants want input. In Grover, V., Uccelli, P., Rowe, M. L., and Lieven, E. (eds.), Learning through language: Towards an educationally informed theory of language learning (pp. 31–9). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Harris, P. L., Kruithof, A., Meerum Terwogt, M., and Visser, T. (1981) Children’s detection and awareness of textual anomaly. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 31, 212–30. http://doi:10.1016/0022-0965(81)90013-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harris, P. L, Ronfard, S., and Bartz, D. T. (2017a) Young children’s developing conception of knowledge and ignorance: Work in progress. European Journal of Developmental Psychology, 14, 221–32. http://doi:10.1080/17405629.2016.1190267Google Scholar
Harris, P.L., Yang, B., and Cui, Y. (2017b). “I don’t know”: Children’s early talk about knowledge. Mind and Language, 32, 283307.Google Scholar
Horner, V., Whiten, A., Flynn, E., and de Waal, F. M. (2006). Faithful replication of foraging techniques along cultural transmission chains by chimpanzees and children. PNAS, 103, 13878–83. http://doi:10.1073/pnas.0606015103Google Scholar
Isaacs, N. (1930). Children’s “why” questions. In Isaacs, S. (ed.), Intellectual growth in young children. London: George Routledge & Sons.Google Scholar
Knudsen, B., and Liszkowski, U. (2012). Eighteen and 24-month-old infants correct others in anticipation of action mistakes. Developmental Science, 15, 113–22. http://doi:10.1111/j.1467-7687.2011.01098.xGoogle Scholar
Kovács, A., Tauzin, T., Téglás, E., Gergely, G., and Csibra, G. (2014). Pointing as epistemic request: 12-month-olds point to receive new information. Infancy, 19, 543–57. http://doi:10.1111/infa.12060Google Scholar
Kulke, L., Reiß, M., Krist, H., and Rakoczy, H. (2018). How robust are anticipatory looking measures of theory of mind? Replication attempts across the life span. Cognitive Development. http://doi: 10.1016/j.cogdev.2017.09.001Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C. (2006). On the human “interaction engine.” In Enfield, N. J. and Levinson, S. C. (eds.), Roots of human sociality (pp. 3969). Oxford: Berg.Google Scholar
Lucca, K., and Wilbourn, M. P. (2016). Communicating to learn: Infant’s pointing gestures result in optimal learning. Child Development, 89, 941–60. http://doi:10.1111/cdev.12707Google Scholar
Markman, E. M. (1977). Realizing that you don’t understand: A preliminary investigation. Child Development, 48, 986–92. http://doi:10.2307/1128350Google Scholar
Markman, E. M. (1979). Realizing that you don’t understand: Elementary school children’s awareness of inconsistencies. Child Development, 50, 643–55. http://doi:10.2307/1128929CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neldner, K., Collier-Baker, E., and Nielsen, M. (2015). Chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and human children (Homo sapiens) know when they are ignorant about the location of food. Animal Cognition, 18, 683–99. http://doi:10.1007/s10071-015-0836-6Google Scholar
Ninio, A., and Snow, C. E. (1996). Pragmatic development. Boulder, CO: Westview Press.Google Scholar
O’Neill, D. K. (1996). Two-year-old children’s sensitivity to a parent’s knowledge state when making requests. Child Development, 67, 659–77. http://doi:10.2307/1131839Google Scholar
Palincsar, A. S., and Brown, A. L. (1984). Reciprocal teaching of comprehension-fostering and comprehension-monitoring activities. Cognition and Instruction, 1, 117–75. http://doi:10.1207/s1532690xci0102_1Google Scholar
Peterson, C. C., and Siegal, M. (2000). Insights into theory of mind from deafness and autism. Mind & Language, 15, 123–45. http://doi:10.1111/1468-0017.00126Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1926). The language and thought of the child. New York: Harcourt Brace.Google Scholar
Revelle, G. L., Wellman, H. M., and Karabenick, J. D. (1985). Comprehension monitoring in preschool children. Child Development, 56, 654–63. http://doi:10.2307/1129755Google Scholar
Shatz, M., Wellman, H. M., and Silber, S. (1983). The acquisition of mental verbs: A systematic investigation of the first reference to mental state. Cognition, 14, 301–21. http://doi:10.1016/0010-0277(83)90008-2Google Scholar
Spilton, D., and Lee, L. C. (1977). Some determinants of effective communication in four-year-olds. Child Development, 48, 968–77. http://doi:10.2307/1128348Google Scholar
Stivers, T., Enfield, N. J., Brown, P., et al. (2009). Universals and turn-taking in conversation. PNAS, 106, 10587–92. http://doi:10.1073/pnas.0903616106Google Scholar
Stivers, T., Sidnell, J., and Bergen, C. (2018). Children’s responses to questions in peer interaction: A window into the ontogenesis of interactional competence. Journal of Pragmatics, 124, 1430. http://doi:10.1016/j.pragma.2017.11.013Google Scholar
Sully, J. (2000) [1896]. Studies of childhood. London: Free Association Books.Google Scholar
Tizard, B., and Hughes, M. (1984). Young children learning. London: Fontana.Google Scholar
Wellman, H. M., Harris, P. L., Banerjee, M., and Sinclair, A. (1995). Early understanding of emotion: Evidence from natural language. Cognition and Emotion, 9, 117–49. http://doi:10.1080/02699939508409005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., and Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory of mind: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72, 655–84. http://doi:10.1111/1467-8624.00304Google Scholar
Wu, Z., and Gros-Louis, J. (2015). Caregivers provide more labeling responses to infants’ pointing than to infants’ object-directed vocalizations. Journal of Child Language, 42, 538–61. http://doi:10.1017/s0305000914000221Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×