Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-27T10:11:18.540Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 10 - The Wisdom in Emotions

from Part II - Foundations of Wisdom in the Individual and in the World

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 May 2022

Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Cornell University, New York
Judith Glück
Affiliation:
Universität Klagenfurt, Austria
Get access

Summary

As the two definitions of the wise personality, introduced by Ardelt and Webster, highlight, researchers in this tradition agree upon the idea that the wise personality possesses a range of distinct highly desirable traits. At the same time, the two different approaches have focused on slightly different components of the wise personality, making it difficult to draw firm conclusions about its exact nature and structure. Important for present purposes, according to both definitions, wisdom contains cognitive, reflective, and emotional elements. As to the emotional elements, there is consensus that wise individuals are empathic toward others and experience compassion and sympathy in advice giving. In addition, although somewhat implicit in Ardelt’s wisdom model, there seems to be agreement that the wise individual has a comprehensive understanding of emotions and can use emotions in constructive ways. According to Webster, humor is particularly worth noting because it can facilitate bonding, distance from the self, and creativity. Critically, one could note that the theoretical ideas about the emotional traits of wise individuals have remained relatively abstract and vague. For example, from what the authors in this tradition write, it is difficult to understand how the wise personality reacts to concrete emotionally laden events, regulates their own and other people’s emotions, or uses knowledge about emotions in judgment and decision processes or when giving advice to others. In part, this vagueness is due to the trait approach of defining the wise personality and the associated global nature of self-report questionnaires. For example, items such as “If I see people in need, I try to help them one way or another” or “I am good at identifying subtle emotions within myself” assess participants’ beliefs about the degree to which general traits and competencies are typical of them, that is, apply to them in most situations most of the time. Such items do not refer to situation-specific behaviors or states and, thus, do not speak to how people specifically deal with emotionally challenging situations, express and regulate their emotions or use them for advice-giving and decision making processes (see Chapter 6).

Type
Chapter
Information
The Psychology of Wisdom
An Introduction
, pp. 157 - 172
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2022

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ardelt, M. (2003). Empirical assessment of a three-dimensional wisdom scale. Research on Aging, 25(3), 275324.Google Scholar
Ardelt, M. (2004). Wisdom as expert knowledge system: a critical review of a contemporary operationalization of an ancient concept. Human Development, 47(5), 257–85.Google Scholar
Baltes, P. B. and Kunzmann, U. (2004). The two faces of wisdom: wisdom as a general theory of knowledge and judgment about excellence in mind and virtue vs. wisdom as everyday realization in people and products. Human Development, 47(5), 290–99.Google Scholar
Baltes, P. B. and Smith, J. (1990). The psychology of wisdom and its ontogenesis. In Sternberg, R. J., ed., Wisdom: Its Nature, Origins, and Development. Cambridge University Press, pp. 87120.Google Scholar
Baltes, P. B. and Staudinger, U. M. (2000). Wisdom: a metaheuristic (pragmatic) to orchestrate mind and virtue toward excellence. American Psychologist, 55(1), 122–36.Google Scholar
Carstensen, L. L., Isaacowitz, D. M., and Charles, S. T. (1999). Taking time seriously: a theory of socioemotional selectivity. American Psychologist, 54(3), 165–81.Google Scholar
Castro, V.L., Cheng, Y., Halberstadt, A.G., and Grühn, D. (2016). EUReKA! A conceptual model of emotion understanding. Emotion Review, 8(3), 258–68.Google Scholar
Clayton, V. P. and Birren, J. E. (1980). The development of wisdom across the life span: a re-examination of an ancient topic. In Baltes, P. B. and Brim, O. G., Jr., eds., Life-Span Development and Behavior, Vol. 3. Academic Press, pp. 103–35.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1999). Basic emotions. In Dalgleish, T. and Power, M. J., eds., Handbook of Cognition and Emotion. John Wiley & Sons Ltd, pp. 4560.Google Scholar
Erikson, E. H. (1959). Identity and the Life Cycle. International University Press.Google Scholar
Fredrickson, B. L. (1998). What good are positive emotions? Review of General Psychology, 2(3), 300–19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fredrickson, B. L. (2001). The role of positive emotions in positive psychology: the broaden-and build theory of positive emotions. American Psychologist, 56(3), 218–26.Google Scholar
Fredrickson, B. L. and Branigan, C. A. (2001). Positive emotions. In Mayne, T. J. and Bonnano, G. A., eds., Emotion: Current Issues and Future Developments. Guilford Press, pp. 123–51.Google Scholar
Frijda, N. H., Kuipers, P., and ter Schure, E. (1989). Relations among emotion, appraisal, and emotional action readiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(2), 212–28.Google Scholar
Glück, J. (2015). Wisdom, psychology of. In Wright, J., ed., International Encyclopedia of Social and Behavioral Sciences, 2nd ed. Vol. 25. Elsevier, pp. 590–97.Google Scholar
Glück, J. and Bluck, S. (2013). The MORE Life Experience Model: a theory of the development of personal wisdom. In Ferrari, M. and Weststrate, N., eds., The Scientific Study of Personal Wisdom. Springer, pp. 7598.Google Scholar
Glück, J., Bluck, S., and Weststrate, N. M. (2018). More on the MORE Life Experience Model: what we have learned (so far). Journal of Value Inquiry, 53(6), 349–70.Google Scholar
Gross, J. J. (2015). Emotion regulation: current status and future prospects. Psychological Inquiry, 26(1), 126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grossmann, I. (2017). Wisdom in context. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(2), 233–57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Grossmann, I., Gerlach, T. M., and Denissen, J. J. (2016). Wise reasoning in the face of everyday life challenges. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7(7), 611–22.Google Scholar
Isen, A. M. (1987). Positive affect, cognitive processes, and social behavior. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 20, 203–53.Google Scholar
Isen, A. M. (1999). Positive affect. In Dalgleish, T. and Power, M., eds., Handbook of Emotion and Cognition. Erlbaum, pp. 521–39.Google Scholar
Kramer, G. (2007). Insight Dialogue: The Interpersonal Path to Freedom. Shambhala Publications.Google Scholar
Kunzmann, U. and Baltes, P. B. (2003). Wisdom-related knowledge: affective, motivational, and interpersonal correlates. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(9), 1104–19.Google Scholar
Kunzmann, U. and Wrosch, C. (2017). Emotional development in old age. In Pachana, N., ed., Encyclopedia of Geropsychology. Springer, pp. 752–62.Google Scholar
Kunzmann, U. and Wrosch, C. (2018). Comment: The emotion–health link: perspectives from a lifespan theory of discrete emotions. Emotion Review, 10(1), 5961.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kunzmann, U., Stange, A., and Jordan, J. (2005). Positive affectivity and lifestyle in adulthood: do you do what you feel? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31(4), 574–88.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kunzmann, U., Wieck, C., and Dietzel, C. (2018). Empathic accuracy: age differences from adolescence into middle adulthood. Cognition and Emotion, 32(8), 1611–24.Google Scholar
Kunzmann, U., Schilling, O., Wrosch, C. et al. (2019). Negative emotions and chronic physical illness: a lifespan developmental perspective. Health Psychology, 38(11), 949–59.Google Scholar
Labouvie-Vief, G. (1990). Wisdom as integrated thought: historical and developmental perspectives. In Sternberg, R. J., ed., Wisdom: Its Nature, Origins, and Development. Cambridge University Press, pp. 5283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labouvie-Vief, G. (2003). Dynamic integration: affect, cognition, and the self in adulthood. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 12(6), 201–6.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and Adaptation. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Levenson, R. W. (2000). Expressive, physiological, and subjective changes in emotion across adulthood. In Qualls, S. H. and Abeles, N., eds., Psychology and the Aging Revolution: How We Adapt to Longer Life. American Psychological Association, pp. 123–40.Google Scholar
Mickler, C. and Staudinger, U. M. (2008). Personal wisdom: validation and age-related differences of a performance measure. Psychology and Aging, 23(4), 787–99.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salovey, P., Kokkonen, M., Lopes, P. N., and Mayer, J.D. (2004). Emotional intelligence: what do we know? In Manstead, A. S. R., Frijda, N. H., and Fischer, A. H., eds., Feelings and Emotions: The Amsterdam Symposium. Cambridge University Press, pp. 319–38.Google Scholar
Sheppes, G., Scheibe, S., Suri, G. et al. (2014). Emotion regulation choice: a conceptual framework and supporting evidence. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(1), 163–81.Google Scholar
Staudinger, U. M. and Glück, J. (2011). Psychological wisdom research: commonalities and differences in a growing field. Annual Review of Psychology, 62(1), 215–41.Google Scholar
Staudinger, U. M., Lopez, D. F., and Baltes, P. B. (1997). The psychometric location of wisdom-related performance: intelligence, personality, and more? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23(11), 1200–14.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., ed. (1990). Wisdom: Its Nature, Origins, and Development. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1998). A balance theory of wisdom. Review of General Psychology, 2(4), 347–65.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (2001). Why schools should teach for wisdom: the balance theory of wisdom in educational settings. Educational Psychologist, 36(4), 227–45.Google Scholar
Webster, J. D. (2003). An exploratory analysis of a self-assessed wisdom scale. Journal of Adult Development, 10(1), 1322.Google Scholar
Webster, J.D. (2007). Measuring the character strength of wisdom. The International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 65(2), 163–83.Google Scholar
Wink, P. and Helson, R. (1997). Practical and transcendent wisdom: their nature and some longitudinal findings. Journal of Adult Development, 4(1), 115.Google Scholar
Wink, P. and Staudinger, U. M. (2016). Wisdom and psychosocial functioning in later life. Journal of Personality, 84(3), 306–18.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×