Book contents
- Frontmatter
- Contents
- Contributors
- Preface
- 1 Introduction
- 2 On the moraic representation of underlying geminates: evidence from Prosodic Morphology
- 3 Verbal reduplication in three Bantu languages
- 4 Prosodic Morphology and tone: the case of Chichewa
- 5 Exceptional stress-attracting suffixes in Turkish: representations versus the grammar
- 6 Realignment
- 7 Faithfulness and identity in Prosodic Morphology
- 8 Austronesian nasal substitution and other NC effects
- 9 The prosodic base of the Hausa plural
- 10 Prosodic optimality and prefixation in Polish
- 11 Double reduplications in parallel
- Index of subjects
- Index of constraints
- Index of languages
- Index of names
1 - Introduction
Published online by Cambridge University Press: 27 January 2010
- Frontmatter
- Contents
- Contributors
- Preface
- 1 Introduction
- 2 On the moraic representation of underlying geminates: evidence from Prosodic Morphology
- 3 Verbal reduplication in three Bantu languages
- 4 Prosodic Morphology and tone: the case of Chichewa
- 5 Exceptional stress-attracting suffixes in Turkish: representations versus the grammar
- 6 Realignment
- 7 Faithfulness and identity in Prosodic Morphology
- 8 Austronesian nasal substitution and other NC effects
- 9 The prosodic base of the Hausa plural
- 10 Prosodic optimality and prefixation in Polish
- 11 Double reduplications in parallel
- Index of subjects
- Index of constraints
- Index of languages
- Index of names
Summary
Prosodic Morphology before the 1990s
One who consults Chomsky and Halle's (1968) Subject Index for entries beginning with pros is referred exclusively to passages discussing prosodic features. This is just one manifestation of this book's explicit claim that hierarchy has no direct role in the study of sound phenomena. Even though the input to the phonological component (a separate morphological component, including a lexicon, became available only later) includes rich hierarchical structures, in that it is the output of the grammar's syntactic component, these structures are flattened out in a so-called intermediate readjustment component, resulting in completely non-hierarchical, flat strings. These strings are sequences of soundsegments and non-segments. The individual sound-segments represent the “sounds” of a given morpheme or word in an abstract sense, through binary-valued phonological features. These features include the ones traditionally called “prosodic,” such as [stress], [pitch] and [tone] (376–77), but they have no special status vis-à-vis other features, just as for instance “cavity features” ([anterior], [coronal], [back], etc.) have no special status. Non-segments are the boundary symbols and the syntactic brackets, such as those occurring between a stem and an affix, between two words, between two phrases, and so on. Roughly speaking the number of boundary symbols and/or brackets between two morphemes reflects their degree of “coherence” (or, conversely, their comparative degree of embedding in the syntactic tree), but this not a hard-and-fast rule: not only is it the task of the readjustment component to flatten out hierarchical structure, but some of the rules of this component are also allowed to manipulate (segments and) non-segments, resulting in derived representations from which the original syntactic structure cannot any longer be automatically read off.
- Type
- Chapter
- Information
- The Prosody-Morphology Interface , pp. 1 - 38Publisher: Cambridge University PressPrint publication year: 1999
- 38
- Cited by