Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T02:03:16.080Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

2 - Children's Social Behaviors and Peer Interactions in Diverse Cultures

from Part I - Culture and Peer Relationships: Theoretical and Methodological Issues

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 August 2009

Carolyn Pope Edwards
Affiliation:
University of Nebraska-Lincoln, Lincoln, Nebraska USA
Maria Rosario Tretasco de Guzman
Affiliation:
University of Nebraska-Lincoln, Lincoln, Nebraska USA
Jill Brown
Affiliation:
University of Nebraska-Lincoln, Lincoln, Nebraska USA
Asiye Kumru
Affiliation:
Abant Izzet Baysal Universitesi, Bolu, Turkey
Xinyin Chen
Affiliation:
University of Western Ontario
Doran C. French
Affiliation:
Illinois Wesleyan University
Barry H. Schneider
Affiliation:
University of Ottawa
Get access

Summary

Cultural socialization has long interested behavioral and social scientists, but recent advances in theory and methodology have allowed researchers to construct new and more powerful theoretical frameworks for conceptualizing the complex ways in which children interact with their environments during the course of development. Studies of childhood socialization in the classic tradition of cross-cultural research were static in their approach to analyzing underlying processes because of limitations in the theories and methods available at the time they were conducted. Many studies, for example, involved straightforward associations or comparisons of levels of parental socialization pressure (the antecedent condition) with children's social or cognitive behavior (the consequent condition). In contrast, using new theoretical and methodological tools, researchers today can go beyond testing predictions about how differences in childhood environments may predict group differences in some kind of child characteristic and instead consider dynamic and transactional child–environment relations. For instance, current researchers have employed theoretical frameworks from social–cognitive development, Vygotskian psychology, and cultural psychology to characterize the children and their contexts in reframed ways and to highlight such themes as self-socialization and guided participation in cultural socialization.

In this chapter, we address the topic of peer relations in cultural context to elaborate how classic and recent approaches to research can be brought together to construct a set of guiding principles for thinking about the cultural dimensions of children's socialization by peers. We define peers as nonfamily children who are similar to one another in age and competence level.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abe, J. A., & Izard, C. E. (1999). Compliance, noncompliance strategies, and the correlates of compliance in 5-year-old Japanese and American children. Social Development, 8(1), 1–20.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aukrust, V., Edwards, C. P., Kumru, A., Knoche, L., & Kim, M. (2003). Young children's extended relationships in school: Parental ethnotheories in four communities, in Norway, United States, Turkey, and Korea. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 27(6), 481–494.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Belle, D. (1989). Gender differences in children's social networks and supports. In Belle, D. (Ed.), Children's social networks and social supports (pp. 173–188). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Benenson, J., Apostoleris, N., & Parnass, J. (1998). The organization of children's same-sex peer relationships. In Bukowski, W. M. & Cillessen, A. H. (Eds.), Sociometry then and now: Building on six decades of measuring children's experience with the peer group (pp. 5–23). San Francisco:Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
Berndt, T. J., & Hoyle, S. G. (1985). Stability and change in childhood and adolescent friendships. Developmental Psychology, 21, 1007–1015.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bloch, M. N., & Adler, S. M. (1994). African children's play and the emergence of the sexual division of labor. In Roopnarine, J., Johnson, J., & Hooper, F. (Eds.), Children's play in diverse cultures, (pp. 148–178). Albany:State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.). (1991). Cultural approaches to parenting. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Boyatzis, C. J., Mallis, M., & Leon, I. (1999). Effects of game type on children's gender-based peer preferences: A naturalistic observational study. Sex Roles, 40, 93–105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, B. B., Mory, M. S., & Kinney, D. (1994). Casting adolescent crowds in a relational perspective: Caricature, channel, and context. In Montemayor, R., Adams, G. R., & Gullotta, T. P., (Eds.), Personal relationships during adolescence. (pp. 123–167). London: Sage.Google Scholar
Buhrmester, D., & Furman, W. (1987). The development of companionship and intimacy. Child Development, 58, 1101–1113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ciarunji, C. (2004). The teaching of values old and new. In Edwards, C. P. & Whiting, B. B. (Eds.), Ngecha: A Kenyan village in a time of rapid social change (pp. 153–178). Lincoln:University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Collins, W. A., & Russell, G. (1991). Mother-child and father-child relationships in middle childhood and adolescence: A developmental analysis. Developmental Review, 11, 99–136.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corsaro, W. A., & Eder, D. J. (1990). Children's peer cultures. Annual Review of Sociology, 16, 197–220.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Degirmencioglu, S. M., Urberg, K. A., Tolson, J. M., & Richard, P. (1998). Adolescent friendship networks: Continuity and change over the school year. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 44, 313–337.Google Scholar
de Guzman, M. R. T., Brown, J., Kirkland, T., Edwards, C. P., & Carlo, G. (2003, April). “Do as you're told”: Examining the contexts and consequences of Gikuyu mothers' prosocial demands on their children. Poster presented at the annual meeting of the Nebraska Symposium on Motivation, Lincoln, NE.
Guzman, M. R. T., & Carlo, G. (2004). Family, peer, and acculturative correlates of prosocial development among Latino youth in Nebraska. Great Plains Research, 14, 182–202.Google Scholar
Guzman, M. R. T., Carlo, G., Ontai, L. L., Koller, S. H., & Knight, G. P. (2004). Gender and age differences in Brazilian children's friendship nominations and peer sociometric ratings. Sex Roles, 51, 217–255.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guzman, M. R. T., Edwards, C. P., & Carlo, G. (2005). Prosocial behaviors in context: A study of the Gikuyu children of Ngecha, Kenya. Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 26 (2005), 542–558).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edwards, C. P. (1978). Social experience and moral judgment in East African young adults. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 133, 19–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edwards, C. P. (1982). Moral development in comparative cultural perspective. In Wagner, D. & Stevenson, H. W. (Eds.), Cultural perspectives on child development (pp. 258–279). San Francisco: Freeman.Google Scholar
Edwards, C. P. (1992). Cross-cultural perspectives on family-peer relations. In Parke, R. D. & Ladd, G. W. (Eds.), Family-peer relationships: Modes of linkage (pp. 285–316). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Edwards, C. P. (2000). Children's play in cross-cultural perspective: A new look at the Six Culture Study. Cross Cultural Research, 34(4), 318–338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edwards, C. (2004). The university as gateway to a complex world. In Edwards, C. P. & Whiting, B. B. (Eds.), Ngecha: A Kenyan village in a time of rapid social change (pp. 215–244). Lincoln:University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Edwards, C. P., Gandini, L., & Giovannini, D. (1996). The contrasting developmental expectations of parents and early childhood teachers in two cultural communities. In Harkness, S. & Super, C. (Eds.), Parents' cultural belief systems (pp. 270–288). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Edwards, C. P., & Whiting, B. B. (1993). “Mother, older sibling, and me”: The overlapping roles of caregivers and companions in the social world of two- to three-year-olds in Ngecha, Kenya. In MacDonald, K. (Ed.), Parent-child play: Descriptions and implications (pp. 305–328). Albany:State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Edwards, C. P., & Whiting, B. B. (Eds.) (2004). Ngecha: A Kenyan village in a time of rapid social change. Lincoln:University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Emirbayer, M., & Goodwin, J. (1994). Network analysis, culture, and the problem of agency. American Journal of Sociology, 99, 1411–1454.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feiring, C., & Lewis, M. (1987). The child's social network: Sex differences from three to six years. Sex Roles, 17, 621–636.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feiring, C., & Lewis, M. (1989). The social networks of girls and boys from early through middle childhood. In Belle, D. (Ed.), Children's social networks and social supports (pp. 119–172). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Feiring, C., & Lewis, M. (1991). The development of social networks from early to middle childhood: Gender differences and the relation to school competence. Sex Roles: A Journal of Research, 25, 237–53.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Field, T. M., Sostek, A. M., Vietze, P., & Leiderman, P. H. (Eds.). (1981). Culture and early interactions. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Frone, M. R. (1999). Developmental consequences of youth employment. In Barling, J. & Kelloway, E. K. (Eds.), Young workers: Varieties of experience. Washington DC: American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Furman, W., & Bierman, K. L. (1983). Developmental changes in young children's conceptions of friendship. Child Development, 54, 549–556.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garcia Coll, C. T., Meyer, E. C., & Brillon, L. (2002). Ethnic and minority parenting. In Bornstein, M. H. (Ed.), Handbook of parenting, Vol. 2: Biology and ecology of parenting (pp. 189–209). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Gardiner, H. W., & Kosmitzki, C. (2004). Lives across cultures: Cross-cultural human development. Boston, MA: Allyn & Bacon.Google Scholar
Giordano, P. C., Cernkovich, S. A., & Holland, D. D. (2003). Changes in friendship relations over the life course: Implications for desistance from crime. Criminology, 41, 293–328.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodnow, J. J., Cashmore, J., Cotton, S., & Knight, R. (1984). Mothers' developmental timetables in two cultural groups. International Journal of Psychology, 19, 193–205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graham, J. A., Cohen, R., Zbikowski, S. M., & Secrist, M. E. (1998). A longitudinal investigation of race and sex as factors in children's classroom friendship choices. Child Study Journal, 28, 245–27.Google Scholar
Greenberger, E., & Steinberg, L. D. (1986). When teenagers work: The psychological and social costs of adolescent employment. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M. (2004). Weaving generations together: Evolving creativity in the Maya of Chiapas. Santa Fe, NM: SAR Press.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M., Maynard, A. E., & Childs, C. P. (2003). Historical change, cultural learning, and cognitive representation in Zinacantec Maya children. Cognitive Development, 18, 455–487.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harkness, S., & Super, C. M. (1985). The cultural context of gender segregation in children's peer groups. Child Development, 56, 219–224.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harkness, S., & Super, C. (Eds.). (1996). Parents' cultural belief systems (pp. 270–288). New York: Guilford.Google Scholar
Harrison, A. O., Wilson, M. N., Pine, C. J., Chan, S. Q., & Buriel, R. (1990). Family Ecologies of ethnic minority children. Child Development, 61, 347–362.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartup, W. W. (1992). Social relationships and their developmental significance. American Psychologist, 44, 120–126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hess, R. D., Kashiwagi, K., Azuma, H., Price, G. G., & Dickson, W. P. (1980). Maternal expectations for mastery of developmental tasks in Japan and the United States. International Journal of Psychology, 15, 259–271.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hess, R. D., Price, G. G., Dickson, W. P., & Conroy, M. (1981). Different roles for mothers and teachers: Contrasting styles of child care. In Kilmer, S. (Ed.), Advances in early education and day care (Vol. 2, pp. 1–28). Greenwich, CT: JAI Press.Google Scholar
Hobbs, S., & Cornwell, D. (1986). Child labor: An underdeveloped topic in psychology, International Journal of Psychology, 21, 225–234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Howes, C. & Aikins, J. W. (2002). Peer relations in the transition to adolescence. Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 29, 195–230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacklin, C. N., & Maccoby, E. E. (1978). Social behavior at thirty-three months in same-sex and mixed-sex dyads. Child Development, 49, 557–569.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Julian, T. W., McKenry, P. C., & McKelvey, M. W. (1994). Cultural variations in parenting: Perceptions of Caucasian, African-American, Hispanic, and Asian-American parents. Family Relations: Interdisciplinary Journal of Applied Family Studies, 43, 30–37.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kielburger, C., & Major, K. (2000). Free the children: A young man fights against child labor and proves that children can change the world. New York: Harper Collins.Google Scholar
Freniere, P., Strayer, F. F., & Gauthier, R. (1984). The emergence of same-sex affiliative preferences among preschool peers: A developmental/ethological perspective. Child Development, 55, 1958–1965.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Larson, R., & Verma, S. (1999). How children and adolescents around the world spend time: Work, play, and developmental opportunities. Psychological Bulletin, 125, 701–736.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
LeVine, R. A., Dixon, S., LeVine, S., Richman, A., Leiderman, P. H., Keefer, C. H., & Brazelton, T. B. (1994). Child care and culture: Lessons from Africa. New York: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lockheed, M. E. (1986). Reshaping the social order: The case of gender segregation. Sex Roles, 14, 617–628.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Long, S. (1997). Friends as teachers: The impact of peer interaction on the acquisition of a new language. In Gregory, E. (Ed.), One child, many worlds: Early learning in multicultural communities (pp. 123–136). New York: Teachers College Press.Google Scholar
Maccoby, E. E. (1988). Gender as a social category. Developmental Psychology, 24, 755–765.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Maccoby, E. E. (1990). Gender and relationships. American Psychologist, 45, 513–520.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maccoby, E. E. (1998). The two sexes: Growing up apart, coming together. Cambridge, MA: Bellknap Press.Google Scholar
Maynard, A. E. (2004, February). Baby boys can make tortillas, but men can't: Gender and development in Zinacantec Maya sibling interactions. Paper session presented at the 33rd Annual Meeting of the Society for Cross Cultural Research, San Jose, CA.Google Scholar
McDermott, D. (2001). Parenting and ethnicity. In Fine, M. J. & Lee, S. W. (Eds.), Handbook of diversity in parent education: The changing faces of parenting and parent education. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Morelli, G., & Tronick, E. Z. (1992). Male care among Efe foragers and Lese farmers. In Hewlett, B. S. (Ed.), Father-child relations: Cultural and biosocial contexts (pp. 231–261). Hawthorne, NY: Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Munroe, R. L. (2004). Social structure and sex-role choices. Cross-Cultural Research, 38(4), 387–406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Munroe, R. L., & Munroe, R. H. (1975). Cross-cultural human development. Prospect Heights, IL: Waveland Press.Google Scholar
Munroe, R. L., & Munroe, R. H. (1992). Fathers in children's environments: A four culture study. In Hewlett, B. S. (Ed.), Father-child relations: Cultural and biosocial contexts (pp. 213–239). Hawthorne, NY: Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Nugent, J. K., Lester, B. M., & Brazelton, T. B. (1989). The cultural context of infancy. Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Padilla, Y. C. (2002). The social ecology of child development in the Mexican American population: Current theoretical and empirical perspectives. Journal of Human Behavior in the Social Environment, 5, 9–29.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinquart, M., & Silbereisen, R. K. (2004). Human development in times of social change: Theoretical considerations and research needs. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 28(4), 289–298.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pitcher, E. G., & Schultz, L. H. (1983). Boys and girls at play: The development of sex roles. New York: Bergin & Garvey.Google Scholar
Porter, K. (1999, November 19). An anthropological defense of child labor. The Chronicle of Higher Education.Google Scholar
Rogoff, B. (2003). The cultural nature of human development. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rubin, K. H., Bukowski, W., & Parker, J. G. (1998). Peer interactions, relationships, and groups. In Eisenberg, N. (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology: Social and personality development (Vol. 3, pp. 619–700). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Sameroff, A. J., & Haith, M. M. (Eds.). (1996). The five to seven year shift: The age of reason and responsibility. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Scarr, S., & McCartney, K. (1983). How people make their own environments: A theory of genotype-environment effects. Child Development, 54, 424–435.Google ScholarPubMed
Tietjen, A. M. (1989). The ecology of children's social support networks. In Belle, D. (Ed.), Children's social networks and social supports (pp. 37–69). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Tobin, J. J., Wu, D. Y. H., & Davidson, D. H. (1989). Preschool in three cultures: Japan, China, and the United States. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Tudge, J., Hayes, S., Doucet, F., Odero, D.Kulakova, N., Tammeveski, P.. (2000). Parents' participation in cultural practices with their preschoolers. Psicologia: Teoria e Pesquisa, 16, 1–10.Google Scholar
Tudge, J. & Hogan, D. (2005). An ecological approach to naturalistic observation. In Greene, S. M. & Hogan, D. M. (Eds.), Researching children's experiences: Approaches and methods (pp. 102–121). London: Sage.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tudge, J. R. H., Hogan, D. M., Lee, S., Meltsas, M., Tammeveski, P., Kulakova, N. N., et al. (1999). Cultural heterogeneity: Parental values and beliefs and their preschoolers' activities in the United States, South Korea, Russia, and Estonia. In Goncu, A. (Ed.), Children's engagement in the world, (pp. 62–96). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Weisner, T. S. (1987). Socialization for parenthood in sibling caretaking societies. In Lancaster, J., Rossi, A., Altmann, J., & Sherrod, L. (Eds.). Parenting across the lifespan (pp. 237–270). New York: Aldine Press.Google Scholar
Weisner, T. S. (1989). Cultural and universal aspects of social support for children: Evidence from the Abaluyia of Kenya. In Belle, D. (Ed.), Children's social networks and social supports (pp. 70–90). New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Weisner, T. S. (1996). The 5 to 7 transition as an ecocultural project. In Sameroff, A. J. & Haith, M. M. (Eds.), The five to seven year shift: The age of reason and responsibility (pp. 295–328). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Weisner, T. S., Bradley, C., & Kilbride, P. L. (Eds.). (1997). African families and the crisis of social change. Westport, CT: Bergin & Garvey.Google Scholar
White, S. H. (1996). The child's entry into the “age of reason.” In Sameroff, A. J. & Haith, M. M. (Eds.), The five to seven year shift: The age of reason and responsibility (pp. 17–32). Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Whiting, B. (2004a). Changing concepts of the good child and good mothering. In Edwards, C. P. & Whiting, B. B. (Eds.), Ngecha: A Kenyan village in a time of rapid social change (pp. 119–152). Lincoln:University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Whiting, B. B. (2004b). Ngecha, Kenya, behavior observations. Collected by Beatrice Whiting and research collaborators in 1968–1972 [CD ROM]. Edwards, C. P. (Ed.) & M. R. T. de Guzman (compiled). New Haven, CT: Human Relations Area Files Press.Google Scholar
Whiting, B. B. & Edwards, C. P. (1988). Children of different worlds: The formation of social behavior. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Whiting, B., Whiting, J., Herzog, J., & Edwards, C. (2004). The historical stage. In Edwards, C. P. & Whiting, B. B. (Eds.), Ngecha: A Kenyan village in a time of rapid social change (pp. 53–90). Lincoln:University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Whiting, B. B., & Whiting, J. W. M. (1971). Task assignment and personality: A consideration of the effect of herding on boys. In Lambert, W. W. & Weisbrod, R. (Eds.), Comparative perspectives on social psychology (pp. 33–44). Boston, MA: Little, Brown.Google Scholar
Whiting, B. B., & Whiting, J. W. M. (1975). Children of six cultures: A psychocultural analysis. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×