Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-23T10:31:18.816Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 6 - Trust and Emotion

The Effects of Incidental and Integral Affect

from Part II - Neuropsychological Level of Trust

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  09 December 2021

Frank Krueger
Affiliation:
George Mason University, Virginia
Get access

Summary

We review research investigating the influences of affective states on trust. To delineate the behavioral and neural effects of emotions on trust decisions, we consider research from Economics, Psychology and Neuroeconomics. We focus on behavioral and neural research that examined the impact of moods and emotions experienced at the moment of choice, and critically examine evidence concerning both positive and negative incidental and integral emotions. Overall, a pattern emerges from previous findings that strongly suggests that both incidental and integral emotions can influence decisions to trust. Specifically, positive incidental emotions, such as happiness, can enhance trust while negative incidental emotions, such as anxiety, reduce trust. At the same time, neuroimaging findings suggest that this behavioral effect is paralleled by emotions having specific effects on decision-relevant neural circuitry. Emotions alter activity during trust decisions in the temporoparietal junction and medial prefrontal cortex, which have been implicated in theory of mind, as well as the anterior insula, which is commonly implicated in anticipatory negative affect. We conclude by pointing at important avenues of research regarding the role of emotions in learning to trust from past experiences, as well as the chronic distortions of affect and social behavior commonly observed in psychopathology.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2021

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aimone, J., Ball, S., & King-Casas, B. (2015). The betrayal aversion elicitation task: An individual level betrayal aversion measure. PLoS ONE, 10(9), Article e0137491. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0137491Google Scholar
Aimone, J. A., & Houser, D. (2011). Beneficial betrayal aversion. PLoS ONE, 6(3), Article e17725. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0017725Google Scholar
Aimone, J. A., & Houser, D. (2012). What you don’t know won’t hurt you: A laboratory analysis of betrayal aversion. Experimental Economics, 15(4), 571588. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.1589146Google Scholar
Aimone, J. A., Houser, D., & Weber, B. (2014). Neural signatures of betrayal aversion: An fMRI study of trust. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 281(1782), 20132127. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2013.2127Google Scholar
Alguacil, S., Tudela, P., & Ruz, M. (2015). Ignoring facial emotion expressions does not eliminate their influence on cooperation decisions. Psicológica, 36(2), 309335. www.redalyc.org/articulo.oa?id=16941182006Google Scholar
Alós-Ferrer, C., & Farolfi, F. (2019). Trust games and beyond. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 13(887), 114. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2019.00887Google Scholar
Andrade, E. B., & Ho, T.-H. (2007). How is the boss’s mood today? I want a raise. Psychological Science, 18(8), 668671. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01956.xGoogle Scholar
Andreoni, J. (1995). Warm-glow versus cold-prickle: The effects of positive and negative framing on cooperation in experiments. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 110(1), 121. https://doi.org/10.2307/2118508Google Scholar
Bechara, A., & Martin, E. M. (2004). Impaired decision making related to working memory deficits in individuals with substance addictions. Neuropsychology, 18(1), 152162. https://doi.org/10.1037/0894-4105.18.1.152Google Scholar
Bellucci, G., Chernyak, S. V., Goodyear, K., Eickhoff, S. B., & Krueger, F. (2017). Neural signatures of trust in reciprocity: A coordinate-based meta-analysis. Human Brain Mapping, 38(3), 12331248. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23451Google Scholar
Bellucci, G., Feng, C., Camilleri, J., Eickhoff, S. B., & Krueger, F. (2018). The role of the anterior insula in social norm compliance and enforcement: Evidence from coordinate-based and functional connectivity meta-analyses. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 92, 378389. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2018.06.024Google Scholar
Bellucci, G., Molter, F., & Park, S. Q. (2019). Neural representations of honesty predict future trust behavior. Nature Communications, 10(1), 112. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467–019-13261-8Google Scholar
Berg, J., Dickhaut, J., & McCabe, K. (1995). Trust, reciprocity, and social history. Games and Economic Behavior, 10(1), 122142. https://doi.org/10.1006/game.1995.1027Google Scholar
Bohnet, I., Greig, F., Herrmann, B., & Zeckhauser, R. (2008). Betrayal aversion: Evidence from Brazil, China, Oman, Switzerland, Turkey, and the United States. American Economic Review, 98(1), 294310. https://doi.org/10.1257/aer.98.1.294CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bohnet, I., & Zeckhauser, R. (2004). Trust, risk and betrayal. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 55(4), 467484. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.478424Google Scholar
Bonnefon, J.-F., Hopfensitz, A., & De Neys, W. (2017). Can we detect cooperators by looking at their face? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 26(3), 276281. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721417693352CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Camerer, C. F. (2003). Behavioral game theory: Experiments in strategic interaction. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Campellone, T. R., & Kring, A. M. (2013). Who do you trust? The impact of facial emotion and behaviour on decision making. Cognition & Emotion, 27(4), 603620. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699931.2012.726608Google Scholar
Capra, M. C. (2004). Mood-driven behavior in strategic interactions. American Economic Review, 94(2), 367372. https://doi.org/10.1257/0002828041301885CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caria, A., Sitaram, R., Veit, R., Begliomini, C., & Birbaumer, N. (2010). Volitional control of anterior insula activity modulates the response to aversive stimuli: A real-time functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Biological Psychiatry, 68(5), 425432. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.04.020Google Scholar
Charness, G., Du, N., & Yang, C.-L. (2011). Trust and trustworthiness reputations in an investment game. Games and Economic Behavior, 72(2), 361375. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.geb.2010.09.002Google Scholar
Chen, M., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). Consequences of automatic evaluation: Immediate behavioral predispositions to approach or avoid the stimulus. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 25(2), 215224. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167299025002007Google Scholar
Cohn, A., Engelmann, J., Fehr, E., & Maréchal, M. A. (2015). Evidence for countercyclical risk aversion: An experiment with financial professionals. American Economic Review, 105(2), 860885. https://doi.org/10.1257/aer.20131314Google Scholar
Craig, A. (2009). Emotional moments across time: A possible neural basis for time perception in the anterior insula. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1525), 19331942. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0008Google Scholar
Cutler, J., & Campbell-Meiklejohn, D. (2019). A comparative fMRI meta-analysis of altruistic and strategic decisions to give. NeuroImage, 184, 227241. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.09.009Google Scholar
De Hooge, I. E., Nelissen, R., Breugelmans, S. M., & Zeelenberg, M. (2011). What is moral about guilt? Acting “prosocially” at the disadvantage of others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(3), 462473. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021459Google Scholar
De Neys, W., Hopfensitz, A., & Bonnefon, J.-F. (2017). Split-second trustworthiness detection from faces in an economic game. Experimental Psychology, 64(4), 231239. https://doi.org/10.1027/1618-3169/a000367CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeBruine, L. M. (2002). Facial resemblance enhances trust. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 269(1498), 13071312. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2002.2034Google Scholar
Delgado, M. R., Frank, R. H., & Phelps, E. A. (2005). Perceptions of moral character modulate the neural systems of reward during the trust game. Nature Neuroscience, 8(11), 16111618. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1575Google Scholar
Desmet, P. T., De Cremer, D., & Van Dijk, E. (2011). In money we trust? The use of financial compensations to repair trust in the aftermath of distributive harm. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 114(2), 7586. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2010.10.006Google Scholar
Dunn, J. R., & Schweitzer, M. E. (2005). Feeling and believing: The influence of emotion on trust. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(5), 736748. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.88.5.736Google Scholar
Dunning, D., Fetchenhauer, D., & Schlösser, T. (2012). Trust as a social and emotional act: Noneconomic considerations in trust behavior. Journal of Economic Psychology, 33(3), 686694. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2011.09.005Google Scholar
Dunning, D., Fetchenhauer, D., & Schlösser, T. (2017). The varying roles played by emotion in economic decision making. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 15, 3338. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2017.05.006Google Scholar
Eckel, C. C., & Wilson, R. K. (2003). The human face of game theory. In Ostrom, E. & Walker, J. M. (Eds.), Trust and reciprocity: Interdisciplinary lessons from experimental research (pp. 245274). Russell Sage Foundation.Google Scholar
Efferson, C., & Vogt, S. (2013). Viewing men’s faces does not lead to accurate predictions of trustworthiness. Scientific Reports, 3, Article 1047. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep01047Google Scholar
Eimontaite, I., Nicolle, A., Schindler, I. C., & Goel, V. (2013). The effect of partner-directed emotion in social exchange decision making. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(469), 111. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00469Google Scholar
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. V. (2003). Unmasking the face: A guide to recognizing emotions from facial clues. Malor Books.Google Scholar
Engell, A. D., Haxby, J. V., & Todorov, A. (2007). Implicit trustworthiness decisions: Automatic coding of face properties in the human amygdala. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(9), 15081519. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2007.19.9.1508Google Scholar
Engelmann, J. B. (2010). Measuring trust in social neuroeconomics: A tutorial. Hermeneutische Blätter, 1(2), 225242.Google Scholar
Engelmann, J. B., & Fehr, E. (2017). The neurobiology of trust and social decision: The important role of emotions. In Van Lange, P. A. M., Rockenbach, B., & Yamagishi, T. (Eds.), Trust in social dilemmas (pp. 3356). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Engelmann, J. B., & Hare, T. A. (2018). Emotions can bias decision making processes by promoting specific behavior. In Fox, A. S., Lapate, R. C., Shackman, A. J., & Davidson, R. J. (Eds.), The nature of emotion: Fundamental questions (2nd ed., pp. 355370). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Engelmann, J. B., Meyer, F., Fehr, E., & Ruff, C. C. (2015). Anticipatory anxiety disrupts neural valuation during risky choice. Journal of Neuroscience, 35(7), 30853099. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.2880-14.2015Google Scholar
Engelmann, J. B., Meyer, F., Ruff, C. C., & Fehr, E. (2019). The neural circuitry of affect-induced distortions of trust. Science Advances, 5(3), Article eaau3413. https://doi.org/10.1126/sciadv.aau3413Google Scholar
Engelmann, J. B., Schmid, B., De Dreu, C. K., Chumbley, J., & Fehr, E. (2019). On the psychology and economics of antisocial personality. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(26), 1278112786. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1820133116Google Scholar
Fehr, E. (2009). On the economics and biology of trust. Journal of the European Economic Association, 7(2–3), 235266. https://doi.org/10.1162/JEEA.2009.7.2-3.235CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, E., Fischbacher, U., & Kosfeld, M. (2005). Neuroeconomic foundations of trust and social preferences: Initial evidence. American Economic Review, 95(2), 346351. https://doi.org/10.1257/000282805774669736Google Scholar
Fehr, E., & Gächter, S. (2000). Cooperation and punishment in public goods experiments. American Economic Review, 90(4), 980994. http://doi.org/10.1257.aer.90.4.980Google Scholar
FeldmanHall, O., Dunsmoor, J. E., Tompary, A., Hunter, L. E., Todorov, A., & Phelps, E. A. (2018). Stimulus generalization as a mechanism for learning to trust. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(7), E1690E1697. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1715227115CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferrer, R. A., Grenen, E. G., & Taber, J. M. (2015). Effectiveness of internet-based affect induction procedures: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Emotion, 15(6), 752762. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000035CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fertuck, E. A., Fischer, S., & Beeney, J. (2018). Social cognition and borderline personality disorder: Splitting and trust impairment findings. Psychiatric Clinics, 41(4), 613632. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psc.2018.07.003Google ScholarPubMed
Fetchenhauer, D., & Dunning, D. (2012). Betrayal aversion versus principled trustfulness: How to explain risk avoidance and risky choices in trust games. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 81(2), 534541. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2011.07.017Google Scholar
Forgas, J. P. (1995). Mood and judgment: The affect infusion model (AIM). Psychological Bulletin, 117(1), 3966. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.117.1.39Google Scholar
Franzen, N., Hagenhoff, M., Baer, N., et al. (2011). Superior “theory of mind” in borderline personality disorder: An analysis of interaction behavior in a virtual trust game. Psychiatry Research, 187(1–2), 224233. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psychres.2010.11.012Google Scholar
Fredrickson, B. L., Tugade, M. M., Waugh, C. E., & Larkin, G. R. (2003). What good are positive emotions in crisis? A prospective study of resilience and emotions following the terrorist attacks on the United States on September 11th, 2001. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(2), 365376. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.84.2.365Google Scholar
Gerrards-Hesse, A., Spies, K., & Hesse, F. W. (1994). Experimental inductions of emotional states and their effectiveness: A review. British Journal of Psychology, 85(1), 5578. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-8295.1994.tb02508.xGoogle Scholar
Göritz, A. S., & Moser, K. (2006). Web-based mood induction. Cognition and Emotion, 20(6), 887896. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930500405386Google Scholar
Gradin, V., Pérez, A., MacFarlane, J., et al. (2015). Abnormal brain responses to social fairness in depression: An fMRI study using the Ultimatum Game. Psychological Medicine, 45(6), 12411251. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0033291714002347Google Scholar
Gromann, P. M., Heslenfeld, D. J., Fett, A.-K., Joyce, D. W., Shergill, S. S., & Krabbendam, L. (2013). Trust versus paranoia: Abnormal response to social reward in psychotic illness. Brain, 136(6), 19681975. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awt076Google Scholar
Güth, W., Schmittberger, R., & Schwarze, B. (1982). An experimental analysis of ultimatum bargaining. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 3(4), 367388. https://doi.org/10.1016/0167-2681(82)90011-7Google Scholar
Hallquist, M. N., & Pilkonis, P. A. (2012). Refining the phenotype of borderline personality disorder: Diagnostic criteria and beyond. Personality Disorders: Theory, Research, and Treatment, 3(3), 228246. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027953Google Scholar
Hare, T. A., Malmaud, J., & Rangel, A. (2011). Focusing attention on the health aspects of foods changes value signals in vmPFC and improves dietary choice. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(30), 1107711087. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.6383-10.2011Google Scholar
Harlé, K. M., Chang, L. J., van’t Wout, M., & Sanfey, A. G. (2012). The neural mechanisms of affect infusion in social economic decision making: A mediating role of the anterior insula. NeuroImage, 61(1), 3240. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.02.027Google Scholar
Hellhammer, D. H., Wüst, S., & Kudielka, B. M. (2009). Salivary cortisol as a biomarker in stress research. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 34(2), 163171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.psyneuen.2008.10.026Google Scholar
Hertel, G., Neuhof, J., Theuer, T., & Kerr, N. L. (2000). Mood effects on cooperation in small groups: Does positive mood simply lead to more cooperation? Cognition & Emotion, 14(4), 441472. https://doi.org/10.1080/026999300402754Google Scholar
Hinson, J. M., Jameson, T. L., & Whitney, P. (2003). Impulsive decision making and working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 29(2), 298306. https://doi.org/10.1037/0278-7393.29.2.298Google ScholarPubMed
Hirschfeld, R., Montgomery, S. A., Keller, M. B., et al. (2000). Social functioning in depression: A review. The Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 61(4), 268275. https://doi.org/10.4088/JCP.v61n0405Google Scholar
Houser, D., Schunk, D., & Winter, J. (2010). Distinguishing trust from risk: An anatomy of the investment game. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 74(1–2), 7281. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2010.01.002Google Scholar
Isaac, R. M., & Walker, J. M. (1988). Group size effects in public goods provision: The voluntary contributions mechanism. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 103(1), 179199. https://doi.org/10.2307/1882648Google Scholar
Jaeger, B., Evans, A. M., Stel, M., & Van Beest, I. (2019). Explaining the persistent influence of facial cues in social decision making. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148(6), 10081021. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000591Google Scholar
Jaeger, B., Oud, B., Williams, T., Krumhuber, E. G., Fehr, E., & Engelmann, J. B. (2020). Trustworthiness detection from faces: Does reliance on facial impressions pay off? PsyArXiv preprint. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/ayqehGoogle Scholar
Johnson, N. D., & Mislin, A. A. (2011). Trust games: A meta-analysis. Journal of Economic Psychology, 32(5), 865889. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2011.05.007Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., Knetsch, J. L., & Thaler, R. H. (1986). Fairness and the assumptions of economics. Journal of Business, 59(4), S285S300. https://doi.org/10.1086/296367Google Scholar
Kang, Y., Williams, L. E., Clark, M. S., Gray, J. R., & Bargh, J. A. (2011). Physical temperature effects on trust behavior: The role of insula. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 6(4), 507515. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsq077Google Scholar
Kausel, E. E., & Connolly, T. (2014). Do people have accurate beliefs about the behavioral consequences of incidental emotions? Evidence from trust games. Journal of Economic Psychology, 42, 96111. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2014.02.002Google Scholar
Kenealy, P. (1988). Validation of a music mood induction procedure: Some preliminary findings. Cognition & Emotion, 2(1), 4148. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699938808415228Google Scholar
King-Casas, B., Sharp, C., Lomax-Bream, L., Lohrenz, T., Fonagy, P., & Montague, P. R. (2008). The rupture and repair of cooperation in borderline personality disorder. Science, 321(5890), 806810. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1156902Google Scholar
Kirchsteiger, G., Rigotti, L., & Rustichini, A. (2006). Your morals might be your moods. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 59(2), 155172. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2004.07.004Google Scholar
Klapper, A., Dotsch, R., Van Rooij, I., & Wigboldus, D. H. (2016). Do we spontaneously form stable trustworthiness impressions from facial appearance? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 111(5), 655664. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000062Google Scholar
Krueger, F., Bellucci, G., Xu, P., & Feng, C. (2020). The critical role of the right dorsal and ventral anterior insula in reciprocity: Evidence from the trust and ultimatum games. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 14(176), 16. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2020.00176Google Scholar
Krueger, F., Grafman, J., & McCabe, K. (2008). Neural correlates of economic game playing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1511), 38593874. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0165Google Scholar
Krumhuber, E., Manstead, A. S., Cosker, D., Marshall, D., Rosin, P. L., & Kappas, A. (2007). Facial dynamics as indicators of trustworthiness and cooperative behavior. Emotion, 7(4), 730735. https://doi.org/10.1037/1528-3542.7.4.730Google Scholar
Kugler, T., Ye, B., Motro, D., & Noussair, C. N. (2020). On trust and disgust: Evidence from face reading and virtual reality. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 11(3), 317325. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550619856302Google Scholar
Lamm, C., & Singer, T. (2010). The role of anterior insular cortex in social emotions. Brain Structure and Function, 214(5–6), 579591. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429–010-0251-3Google Scholar
Lench, H. C., Flores, S. A., & Bench, S. W. (2011). Discrete emotions predict changes in cognition, judgment, experience, behavior, and physiology: A meta-analysis of experimental emotion elicitations. Psychological Bulletin, 137(5), 834855. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024244Google Scholar
Lerner, J. S., & Keltner, D. (2000). Beyond valence: Toward a model of emotion-specific influences on judgement and choice. Cognition & Emotion, 14(4), 473493. https://doi.org/10.1080/026999300402763Google Scholar
Lerner, J. S., Li, Y., Valdesolo, P., & Kassam, K. S. (2015). Emotion and decision making. Annual Review of Psychology, 66(1), 799823. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115043CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lim, S.-L., O’Doherty, J. P., & Rangel, A. (2011). The decision value computations in the vmPFC and striatum use a relative value code that is guided by visual attention. Journal of Neuroscience, 31(37), 1321413223. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.1246-11.2011Google Scholar
Lindquist, K. A., Wager, T. D., Kober, H., Bliss-Moreau, E., & Barrett, L. F. (2012). The brain basis of emotion: A meta-analytic review. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 35(3), 121143. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x11000446Google Scholar
Loewenstein, G., & Lerner, J. S. (2003). The role of affect in decision making. In Davidson, R. J., Scherer, K. R., & Goldsmith, H. H. (Eds.), Handbook of affective science (pp. 619642). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Loewenstein, G. F., Weber, E. U., Hsee, C. K., & Welch, N. (2001). Risk as feelings. Psychological Bulletin, 127(2), 267286. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.127.2.267Google Scholar
Loomes, G., & Sugden, R. (1982). Regret theory: An alternative theory of rational choice under uncertainty. The Economic Journal, 92(368), 805824. https://doi.org/10.2307/2232669Google Scholar
Loomes, G., & Sugden, R. (1986). Disappointment and dynamic consistency in choice under uncertainty. The Review of Economic Studies, 53(2), 271282. https://doi.org/10.2307/2297651Google Scholar
Lyubomirsky, S., King, L., & Diener, E. (2005). The benefits of frequent positive affect: Does happiness lead to success? Psychological Bulletin, 131(6), 803855. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.803Google Scholar
Maccallum, F., McConkey, K. M., Bryant, R. A., & Barnier, A. J. (2000). Specific autobiographical memory following hypnotically induced mood state. International Journal of Clinical and Experimental Hypnosis, 48(4), 361373. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207140008410366Google Scholar
Martinez, L. F., & Zeelenberg, M. (2015). Trust me (or not): Regret and disappointment in experimental economic games. Decision, 2(2), 118126. https://doi.org/10.1037/dec0000025Google Scholar
McCabe, K., Houser, D., Ryan, L., Smith, V., & Trouard, T. (2001). A functional imaging study of cooperation in two-person reciprocal exchange. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 98(20), 1183211835. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.211415698Google Scholar
Mellers, B. A., Haselhuhn, M. P., Tetlock, P. E., Silva, J. C., & Isen, A. M. (2010). Predicting behavior in economic games by looking through the eyes of the players. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 139(4), 743755. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020280Google Scholar
Mellers, B. A., Schwartz, A., Ho, K., & Ritov, I. (1997). Decision affect theory: Emotional reactions to the outcomes of risky options. Psychological Science, 8(6), 423429. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1997.tb00455.xGoogle Scholar
Mislin, A., Williams, L. V., & Shaughnessy, B. A. (2015). Motivating trust: Can mood and incentives increase interpersonal trust? Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Economics, 58, 1119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socec.2015.06.001Google Scholar
Mulckhuyse, M., Crombez, G., & Van der Stigchel, S. (2013). Conditioned fear modulates visual selection. Emotion, 13(3), 529536. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031076Google Scholar
Mulckhuyse, M., Engelmann, J. B., Schutter, D. J., & Roelofs, K. (2017). Right posterior parietal cortex is involved in disengaging from threat: A 1-Hz rTMS study. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(11), 18141822. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsx111Google Scholar
Myers, C. D., & Tingley, D. (2016). The influence of emotion on trust. Political Analysis, 24(4), 492500. https://doi.org/10.1093/pan/mpw026Google Scholar
Nelissen, R. M., Dijker, A. J., & deVries, N. K. (2007). How to turn a hawk into a dove and vice versa: Interactions between emotions and goals in a give-some dilemma game. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43(2), 280286. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2006.01.009Google Scholar
Niv, Y., Edlund, J. A., Dayan, P., & O’Doherty, J. P. (2012). Neural prediction errors reveal a risk-sensitive reinforcement-learning process in the human brain. Journal of Neuroscience, 32(2), 551562. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.5498-10.2012Google Scholar
Nolte, T., Bolling, D. Z., Hudac, C., Fonagy, P., Mayes, L. C., & Pelphrey, K. A. (2013). Brain mechanisms underlying the impact of attachment-related stress on social cognition. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7(816), 112. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2013.00816Google Scholar
Pessoa, L. (2008). On the relationship between emotion and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(2), 148158. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2317Google Scholar
Pham, M. T. (2007). Emotion and rationality: A critical review and interpretation of empirical evidence. Review of General Psychology, 11(2), 155178. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2680.11.2.155Google Scholar
Phelps, E. A., Lempert, K. M., & Sokol-Hessner, P. (2014). Emotion and decision making: Multiple modulatory neural circuits. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 37(1), 263287. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-neuro-071013-014119Google Scholar
Prochazkova, E., Prochazkova, L., Giffin, M. R., Scholte, H. S., De Dreu, C. K., & Kret, M. E. (2018). Pupil mimicry promotes trust through the theory-of-mind network. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(31), E7265E7274. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1803916115Google Scholar
Quercia, S. (2016). Eliciting and measuring betrayal aversion using the BDM mechanism. Journal of the Economic Science Association, 2(1), 4859. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40881–015-0021-3Google Scholar
Rabin, M. (1993). Incorporating fairness into game theory and economics. The American Economic Review, 83(5), 12811302. www.jstor.org/stable/2117561?seq=1#metadata_info_tab_contentsGoogle Scholar
Rangel, A., Camerer, C., & Montague, P. R. (2008). A framework for studying the neurobiology of value-based decision making. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(7), 545556. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2357CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rangel, A., & Hare, T. (2010). Neural computations associated with goal-directed choice. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 20(2), 262270. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2010.03.001Google Scholar
Rick, S., & Loewenstein, G. (2008). The role of emotion in economic behavior. In Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones, J. M., & Barrett, L. F. (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd ed.; pp. 138158). The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Rilling, J. K., Sanfey, A. G., Aronson, J. A., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D. (2004). The neural correlates of theory of mind within interpersonal interactions. NeuroImage, 22(4), 16941703. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.04.015Google Scholar
Roelofs, K. (2017). Freeze for action: Neurobiological mechanisms in animal and human freezing. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 372(1718), 20160206. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0206Google Scholar
Roelofs, K., Putman, P., Schouten, S., Lange, W.-G., Volman, I., & Rinck, M. (2010). Gaze direction differentially affects avoidance tendencies to happy and angry faces in socially anxious individuals. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 48(4), 290294. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brat.2009.11.008Google Scholar
Ruff, C. C., & Fehr, E. (2014). The neurobiology of rewards and values in social decision making. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 15(8), 549562. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3776Google Scholar
Saadaoui, H., El Harbi, S., & Ibanez, L. (2019). Do people trust more when they are happy or when they are sad? Evidence from an experiment. Managerial and Decision Economics, 40(4), 374383. https://doi.org/10.1002/mde.3008Google Scholar
Sanfey, A. G. (2009). Expectations and social decision making: Biasing effects of prior knowledge on Ultimatum responses. Mind & Society, 8(1), 93107. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11299–009-0053-6Google Scholar
Sanfey, A. G., Rilling, J. K., Aronson, J. A., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D. (2003). The neural basis of economic decision making in the Ultimatum Game. Science, 300(5626), 17551758. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1082976Google Scholar
Scharlemann, J. P., Eckel, C. C., Kacelnik, A., & Wilson, R. K. (2001). The value of a smile: Game theory with a human face. Journal of Economic Psychology, 22(5), 617640. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0167–4870(01)00059-9Google Scholar
Schlösser, T., Fetchenhauer, D., & Dunning, D. (2016). Trust against all odds? Emotional dynamics in trust behavior. Decision, 3(3), 216230. https://doi.org/10.1037/dec0000048Google Scholar
Schmitz, A., & Grillon, C. (2012). Assessing fear and anxiety in humans using the threat of predictable and unpredictable aversive events (the NPU-threat test). Nature Protocols, 7(3), 527532. https://doi.org/10.1038/nprot.2012.001Google Scholar
Schönberg, T., Daw, N. D., Joel, D., & O’Doherty, J. P. (2007). Reinforcement learning signals in the human striatum distinguish learners from nonlearners during reward-based decision making. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(47), 1286012867. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.2496-07.2007Google Scholar
Schultz, W. (2002). Getting formal with dopamine and reward. Neuron, 36(2), 241263. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0896–6273(02)00967-4Google Scholar
Schurz, M., Radua, J., Aichhorn, M., Richlan, F., & Perner, J. (2014). Fractionating theory of mind: A meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 42, 934. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.01.009Google Scholar
Schwarz, N. (1990). Feelings as information: Informational and motivational functions of affective states. In Higgins, T. E. & Sorrentino, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition: Foundations of social behavior (Vol. 1; pp. 527561). The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Sripada, C. S., Angstadt, M., Banks, S., Nathan, P. J., Liberzon, I., & Phan, K. L. (2009). Functional neuroimaging of mentalizing during the trust game in social anxiety disorder. Neuroreport, 20(11), 984989. https://doi.org/10.1097/wnr.0b013e32832d0a67Google Scholar
Stanley, D. A., Sokol-Hessner, P., Fareri, D. S., et al. (2012). Race and reputation: Perceived racial group trustworthiness influences the neural correlates of trust decisions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1589), 744753. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0300Google Scholar
Tabibnia, G., Satpute, A. B., & Lieberman, M. D. (2008). The sunny side of fairness: Preference for fairness activates reward circuitry (and disregarding unfairness activates self-control circuitry). Psychological Science, 19(4), 339347. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02091.xGoogle Scholar
Thielmann, I., & Hilbig, B. E. (2015). Trust: An integrative review from a person–situation perspective. Review of General Psychology, 19(3), 249277. https://doi.org/10.1037/gpr0000046Google Scholar
Thielmann, I., Spadaro, G., & Balliet, D. (2020). Personality and prosocial behavior: A theoretical framework and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 146(1), 3039. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000217Google Scholar
Tognetti, A., Berticat, C., Raymond, M., & Faurie, C. (2013). Is cooperativeness readable in static facial features? An inter-cultural approach. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(6), 427432. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.08.002Google Scholar
Tortosa, M. I., Strizhko, T., Capizzi, M., & Ruz, M. (2013). Interpersonal effects of emotion in a multi-round trust game. Psicologica: International Journal of Methodology and Experimental Psychology, 34(2), 179198.Google Scholar
Velten, J. E. (1968). A laboratory task for induction of mood states. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 6(4), 473482. https://doi.org/10.1016/0005-7967(68)90028-4Google Scholar
Verplaetse, J., Vanneste, S., & Braeckman, J. (2007). You can judge a book by its cover: The sequel. A kernel of truth in predictive cheating detection. Evolution and Human Behavior, 28(4), 260271. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2007.04.006Google Scholar
Wager, T. D., & Barrett, L. F. (2017). From affect to control: Functional specialization of the insula in motivation and regulation. BioRxiv, Article 102368. https://doi.org/10.1101/102368Google Scholar
Waytz, A., & Mitchell, J. P. (2011). Two mechanisms for simulating other minds: Dissociations between mirroring and self-projection. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(3), 197200. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721411409007Google Scholar
Wehebrink, K. S., Koelkebeck, K., Piest, S., de Dreu, C. K., & Kret, M. E. (2018). Pupil mimicry and trust–implication for depression. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 97, 7076. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpsychires.2017.11.007Google Scholar
Willis, J., & Todorov, A. (2006). First impressions: Making up your mind after a 100-ms exposure to a face. Psychological Science, 17(7), 592598. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01750.xGoogle Scholar
Winston, J. S., Strange, B. A., O’Doherty, J., & Dolan, R. J. (2002). Automatic and intentional brain responses during evaluation of trustworthiness of faces. Nature Neuroscience, 5(3), 277283. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn816Google Scholar
Yiend, J. (2010). The effects of emotion on attention: A review of attentional processing of emotional information. Cognition and Emotion, 24(1), 347. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930903205698Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×