Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-ndw9j Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-02T20:01:07.377Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

5 - Grooming syntax as a sensitive measure of the effects of subchronic PCP treatment in rats

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  04 August 2010

Allan V. Kalueff
Affiliation:
National Institute of Mental Health, Washington DC
Justin L. La Porte
Affiliation:
National Institute of Mental Health, Washington DC
Carisa L. Bergner
Affiliation:
National Institute of Mental Health, Washington DC
Get access

Summary

Summary

Grooming is increasingly recognized as a reliable marker of stress-related disturbances in animal models of neuropsychiatric disorders. We previously reported that subchronic exposure to 10 mg/kg of phencyclidine (PCP) for 15 days in rats increased grooming expression under both stressful and appetitive conditions, but impaired grooming syntax only when the behavior was elicited with stressful water sprays directed at the face. For the purpose of this chapter, new indexes from the same rats subjected to the water spray condition were analyzed. Results showed that the PCP group aborted less chains after face washing and spent a lower proportion of time in anterior grooming than control animals. Phencyclidine treatment also increased incorrect chain initiations and enhanced the duration of Phase IV within completed syntactic chains. Finally, PCP-injected rats were less engaged in nongrooming activities, and were more inactive. In a context where grooming was needed rostrally after facial contacts with water sprays, these results indicate that subchronic PCP treatment compromised hygiene efficiency and engendered an unfocused and perseverative grooming, most likely combined with an abnormal stress response. These observations suggest that the two leading approaches in the study of grooming patterning may provide pivotal sets of qualitative observations that help identify hygienic and stress-related irregularities in animal models.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aldridge, JW and Berridge, KC (1998): Coding of serial order by neostriatal neurons: a “natural action” approach to movement sequence. J Neurosci 18:2777–87.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Audet, MC, Goulet, S and Doré, FY (2006): Repeated subchronic exposure to phencyclidine elicits excessive atypical grooming in rats. Behav Brain Res 167:103–10.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Audet, MC, Goulet, S and Doré, FY (2007a): Enhanced anxiety follows withdrawal from subchronic exposure to phencyclidine in rats. Behav Brain Res 176:358–61.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Audet, MC, Goulet, S and Doré, FY (2007b): Transient hypolocomotion in rats repeatedly exposed to phencyclidine: an appraisal of motor function and motivation. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 31:142–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bakker, CB and Amini, FB (1961): Observations on the psychotomimetic effects of Sernyl. Compr Psychiatry 2:269–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berridge, K (1990): Comparative fine structure of action: rules of form and sequence in the grooming patterns of six rodent species. Behaviour 113:21–56.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berridge, KC and Aldridge, JW (2000a): Super-stereotypy I: enhancement of a complex movement sequence by systemic dopamine D1 agonists. Synapse 37:194–204.3.0.CO;2-A>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berridge, KC and Aldridge, JW (2000b): Super-stereotypy II: enhancement of a complex movement sequence by intraventricular dopamine D1 agonists. Synapse 37:205–15.3.0.CO;2-A>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berridge, KC and Whishaw, IQ (1992): Cortex, striatum and cerebellum: control of serial order in a grooming sequence. Exp Brain Res 90:275–90.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berridge, KC, Fentress, JC and Parr, H (1987): Natural syntax rules control action sequence of rats. Behav Brain Res 23:59–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Collip, D, Myin-Germeys, I and Os, J (2008): Does the concept of “sensitization” provide a plausible mechanism for the putative link between the environment and schizophrenia?Schizophr Bull 34:220–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cromwell, HC and Berridge, KC (1996): Implementation of action sequences by a neostriatal site: a lesion mapping study of grooming syntax. J Neurosci 16:3444–58.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cromwell, HC, Berridge, KC, Drago, J and Levine, MS (1998): Action sequencing is impaired in D1A-deficient mutant mice. Eur J Neurosci 10:2426–32.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
D'Aquila, PS, Peana, AT, Carboni, V and Serra, G (2000): Exploratory behaviour and grooming after repeated restraint and chronic mild stress: effect of desipramine. Eur J Pharmacol 399:43–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eilam, D, Talangbayan, H, Canaran, G and Szechtman, H (1992): Dopaminergic control of locomotion, mouthing, snout contact, and grooming: opposing roles of D1 and D2 receptors. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 106:447–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enginar, N, Hatipoglu, I and Firtina, M (2008): Evaluation of the acute effects of amitriptyline and fluoxetine on anxiety using grooming analysis algorithm in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 89:450–5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garner, JP and Mason, GJ (2002): Evidence for a relationship between cage stereotypies and behavioural disinhibition in laboratory rodents. Behav Brain Res 136:83–92.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gispen, WH and Isaacson, RL (1981): ACTH-induced excessive grooming in the rat. Pharmacol Ther 12:209–46.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hanania, T, Hillman, GR and Johnson, KM (1999): Augmentation of locomotor activity by chronic phencyclidine is associated with an increase in striatal NMDA receptor function and an upregulation of the NR1 receptor subunit. Synapse 31:229–39.3.0.CO;2-3>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hartley, JE and Montgomery, AM (2008): 8-OH-DPAT inhibits both prandial and waterspray-induced grooming. J Psychopharmacol 22:746–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Howell, DC (1997): Statistical Methods for Psychology, 4th edn. Belmont, CA: Duxbury Press.Google Scholar
Javitt, DC and Zukin, SR (1991): Recent advances in the phencyclidine model of schizophrenia. Am J Psychiatry 148:1301–8.Google ScholarPubMed
Jentsch, JD and Roth, RH (1999): The neuropsychopharmacology of phencyclidine: from NMDA receptor hypofunction to the dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 20:201–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jolles, J, Rompa-Barendregt, J and Gispen, WH (1979): ACTH-induced excessive grooming in the rat: the influence of environmental and motivational factors. Horm Behav 12:60–72.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kalueff, AV and Tuohimaa, P (2004): Grooming analysis algorithm for neurobehavioural stress research. Brain Res Brain Res Protoc 13:151–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kalueff, AV and Tuohimaa, P (2005): The grooming analysis algorithm discriminates between different levels of anxiety in rats: potential utility for neurobehavioural stress research. J Neurosci Methods 143:169–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kalueff, AV, Aldridge, JW, LaPorte, JL, Murphy, DL and Tuohimaa, P (2007): Analyzing grooming microstructure in neurobehavioral experiments. Nat Protoc 2:2538–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kappas, A (1995): Coder2. Québec (Canada): École de psychologie, Université Laval.Google Scholar
Komorowska, J and Pellis, SM (2004): Regulatory mechanisms underlying novelty-induced grooming in the laboratory rat. Behav Processes 67:287–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Krebs, H, Macht, M, Meyers, P, Weijers, HG and Janke, W (1996): Effects of stressful noise on eating and non-eating behavior in rats. Appetite 26:192–202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lehrmann, E, Colantuoni, C, Deep-Soboslay, Aet al. (2006): Transcriptional changes common to human cocaine, cannabis and phencyclidine abuse. PLoS ONE 1:e114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luby, ED, Cohen, BD, Rosenbaum, G, Gottlieb, JS and Kelley, R (1959): Study of a new schizophrenomimetic drug; sernyl. AMA Arch Neurol Psychiatry 81:363–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, S and Buuse, M (2008). Phencyclidine-induced locomotor hyperactivity is enhanced in mice after stereotaxic brain serotonin depletion. Behav Brain Res 191:289–93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matell, MS, Berridge, KC and Aldridge, JW (2006): Dopamine D1 activation shortens the duration of phases in stereotyped grooming sequences. Behav Processes 71:241–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGregor, IS, Callaghan, PD and Hunt, GE (2008): From ultrasocial to antisocial: a role for oxytocin in the acute reinforcing effects and long-term adverse consequences of drug use?Br J Pharmacol 154:358–68.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Millan, MJ, Loiseau, F, Dekeyne, Aet al. (2008): S33138 (N-[4-[2-[(3aS,9bR)-8-cyano-1,3a,4,9b-tetrahydro[1] benzopyrano[3,4-c]pyrrol-2(3H)-yl)-ethyl]phenyl-acetamide), a preferential dopamine D3 versus D2 receptor antagonist and potential antipsychotic agent: III. Actions in models of therapeutic activity and induction of side effects. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 324:1212–26.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morrens, M, Hulstijn, W, Lewi, PJ, Hert, M and Sabbe, BG (2006): Stereotypy in schizophrenia. Schizophr Res 84:397–404.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, BJ, Cochran, SM and Pratt, JA (2005): PCP: from pharmacology to modelling schizophrenia. Curr Opin Pharmacol 5:101–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mouri, A, Noda, Y, Enomoto, T and Nabeshima, T (2007): Phencyclidine animal models of schizophrenia: approaches from abnormality of glutamatergic neurotransmission and neurodevelopment. Neurochem Int 51:173–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noda, Y, Yamada, K, Furukawa, H and Nabeshima, T (1995): Enhancement of immobility in a forced swimming test by subacute or repeated treatment with phencyclidine: a new model of schizophrenia. Br J Pharmacol 116:2531–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Noda, Y, Mamiya, T, Furukawa, H and Nabeshima, T (1997): Effects of antidepressants on phencyclidine-induced enhancement of immobility in a forced swimming test in mice. Eur J Pharmacol 324:135–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pechnick, RN, George, R and Poland, RE (1989): Naloxone does not antagonize PCP-induced stimulation of the pituitary-adrenal axis in the rat. Life Sci 44:143–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pechnick, RN, Chun, BM, George, R, Hanada, K and Poland, RE (1990): Determination of the loci of action of phencyclidine on the CNS–pituitary–adrenal axis. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 254:344–9.Google ScholarPubMed
Pechnick, RN, Bresee, CJ and Poland, RE (2006): The role of antagonism of NMDA receptor-mediated neurotransmission and inhibition of the dopamine reuptake in the neuroendocrine effects of phencyclidine. Life Sci 78:2006–11.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pijlman, FT and Ree, JM (2002): Physical but not emotional stress induces a delay in behavioural coping responses in rats. Behav Brain Res 136:365–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ridley, RM (1994): The psychology of perseverative and stereotyped behaviour. Prog Neurobiol 44:221–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robertson, BJ, Boon, F, Cain, DP and Vanderwolf, CH (1999): Behavioral effects of anti-muscarinic, anti-serotonergic, and anti-NMDA treatments: hippocampal and neocortical slow wave electrophysiology predict the effects on grooming in the rat. Brain Res 838:234–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roeling, TA, Erp, AM, Meelis, W, Kruk, MR and Veening, JG (1991): Behavioural effects of NMDA injected into the hypothalamic paraventricular nucleus of the rat. Brain Res 550:220–4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sams-Dodd, F (1998a): Effects of continuous D-amphetamine and phencyclidine administration on social behaviour, stereotyped behaviour, and locomotor activity in rats. Neuropsychopharmacology 19:18–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sams-Dood, F (1998b): A test of the predictive validity of animal models of schizophrenia based on phencyclidine and D-amphetamine. Neuropsychopharmacology 18:293–304.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spruijt, BM, Hooff, JA and Gispen, WH (1992): Ethology and neurobiology of grooming behavior. Physiol Rev 72:825–52.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turgeon, SM, Lin, T and Subramanian, M (2007). Subchronic phencyclidine exposure potentiates the behavioral and c-Fos response to stressful stimuli in rats. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 88:73–81.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Erp, AM, Kruk, MR, Meelis, W and Willekens-Bramer, DC (1994): Effect of environmental stressors on time course, variability and form of self-grooming in the rat: handling, social contact, defeat, novelty, restraint and fur moistening. Behav Brain Res 65:47–55.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wimersma Greidanus, TB, Maigret, C, Torn, Met al. (1989): Dopamine D-1 and D-2 receptor agonists and antagonists and neuropeptide-induced excessive grooming. Eur J Pharmacol 173:227–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wachtel, SR, Brooderson, RJ and White, FJ (1992): Parametric and pharmacological analyses of the enhanced grooming response elicited by the D1 dopamine receptor agonist SKF 38393 in the rat. Psychopharmacology (Berl) 109:41–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weisman, AG, Nuechterlein, KH, Goldstein, MJ and Snyder, KS (1998): Expressed emotion, attributions, and schizophrenia symptom dimensions. J Abnorm Psychol 107:355–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xu, X and Domino, EF (1994): Phencyclidine-induced behavioral sensitization. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 47:603–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xu, X and Domino, EF (1999): A further study on asymmetric cross-sensitization between MK-801 and phencyclidine-induced ambulatory activity. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 63:413–16.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zhao, Y, Valdez, GR, Fekete, EMet al. (2007): Subtype-selective corticotropin-releasing factor receptor agonists exert contrasting, but not opposite, effects on anxiety-related behavior in rats. J Pharmacol Exp Ther 323:846–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×