Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-g7gxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-14T23:24:47.982Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

5 - Schizophrenia and semantic memory

from Part III - Applications of Models to Understanding Cognitive Dysfunction

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  14 September 2009

Michal Assaf
Affiliation:
Institute of Living; Yale University
Paul Rivkin
Affiliation:
Johns Hopkins University
Michael A. Kraut
Affiliation:
Johns Hopkins University
Vince Calhoun
Affiliation:
Institute of Living; Yale University; Johns Hopkins University
John Hart Jr.
Affiliation:
University of Texas at Dallas
Godfrey Pearlson
Affiliation:
Institute of Living; Yale University; Johns Hopkins University
John Hart
Affiliation:
University of Texas, Dallas
Michael A. Kraut
Affiliation:
The Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine
Get access

Summary

Schizophrenia is a severe, chronic, and disabling psychiatric illness that affects 1 percent of the population. It is often characterized by disorganized speech, delusions, hallucinations, disorganized or catatonic behavior, and negative symptoms such as flat affect and avolition (Diagnostic and statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edn. – DSM-IV). The symptoms related to disorganized and incoherent speech are also known as formal thought disorder (FTD). Although symptoms may vary from patient to patient and between different episodes of the illness in the same patient, 90 percent of the patients show FTD symptoms at some point during the course of their illness (Andreasen, 1979b). Even though these symptoms have been considered to be fundamental to schizophrenia since first described by Bleuler (Bleuler, 1911) and Kraepelin (Kraepelin et al., 1919), the cognitive impairments associated with the neurobiology of FTD are still a matter of debate. One of the most influential theories to date suggests that FTD is strongly associated with impaired semantic memory processing (Spitzer, 1997; Goldberg et al., 1998; Kerns & Berenbaum, 2002). In this chapter we will review the clinical and cognitive symptoms related to FTD, the evidence available that supports different aspects of semantic impairments in FTD, and recent data from our lab suggesting that a far-spreading activation theory within the semantic system is the core, underlying deficit resulting in FTD.

Formal thought disorder

As mentioned above, the symptoms of FTD have been considered by some investigators as pathognomonic to schizophrenia.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Aloia, M. S., Gourovitch, M. L., Missar, D., Pickar, D., Weinberger, D. R., and Goldberg, T. E. (1998). Cognitive substrates of thought disorder, II: specifying a candidate cognitive mechanism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155(12): 1677–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association (1994). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders: DSM-IV, 4th edn. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Association.Google Scholar
Andreasen, N. C. (1979a). Thought, language, and communication disorders. I. Clinical assessment, definition of terms, and evaluation of their reliability. Archives of General Psychiatry, 36(12): 1315–21.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andreasen, N. C. (1979b). Thought, language, and communication disorders. II. Diagnostic significance. Archives of General Psychiatry, 36(12): 1325–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andreasen, N. C. (1984a). The Scale for the Assessment of Negative Symptoms (SANS). Iowa City: The University of Iowa.Google Scholar
Andreasen, N. C. (1984b). The Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms (SAPS). Iowa City: The University of Iowa.Google Scholar
Andreasen, N. C. (1986). Scale for the assessment of thought, language, and communication (TLC). Schizophrenia Bulletin, 12(3): 473–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Assaf, M., Rivkin, P. R., Kuzu, C. H., Calhoun, V. D., Kraut, M. A., Groth, K. M., Yassa, M. A., Hart, J., and Pearlson, G. D. (2006a). Abnormal object recall and anterior cingulate overactivation correlate with formal thought disorder in schizophrenia. Biological Psychiatry, 59(5): 452–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Assaf, M., Calhoun, V. D., Kuzu, C. H., Kraut, M. A., Rivkin, P. R., Hart, J., and Pearlson, G. D. (2006). Neural correlates of the object recall process in semantic memory. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 147(2–3): 115–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barch, D. M. and Berenbaum, H. (1996). Language production and thought disorder in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105(1): 81–8.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bleuler, E. (1911/1950). Dementia Praecox of the Group of Schizophrenias, ed. Zinkin, J. T.. New York: International Universities Press.Google Scholar
First, M. B., Spitzer, R. L.. (2002). Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis I Disorders, Research Version, Patient Edition. (SCID-I/P). New York: Biometrics Research, New York State Psychiatric Institute.Google Scholar
Goldberg, T. E., Aloia, M. S., Gourovitch, M. L., Missar, D., Pickar, D., and Weinberger, D. R. (1998). Cognitive substrates of thought disorder, I: the semantic system. American Journal of Psychiatry, 155(12): 1671–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaplan, R. D., Szechtman, H., Franco, S., Szechtman, B., Nahmias, C., Garnett, E. S., List, S., and Cleghorn, J. M. (1993). Three clinical syndromes of schizophrenia in untreated subjects: relation to brain glucose activity measured by positron emission tomography (PET). Schizophrenia Research, 11(1): 47–54.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kay, S. R., Fiszbein, A., and Opler, L. A. (1987). The positive and negative syndrome scale (PANSS) for schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 13(2): 261–76.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kerns, J. G. and Berenbaum, H. (2002). Cognitive impairments associated with formal thought disorder in people with schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 111(2): 211–24.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kircher, T. T., Liddle, P. F., Brammer, M. J., Williams, S. C., Murray, R. M., and McGuire, P. K. (2001). Neural correlates of formal thought disorder in schizophrenia: preliminary findings from a functional magnetic resonance imaging study. Archives of General Psychiatry, 58(8): 769–74.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kircher, T. T., Liddle, P. F., Brammer, M. J., Williams, S. C., Murray, R. M., and McGuire, P. K. (2002). Reversed lateralization of temporal activation during speech production in thought disordered patients with schizophrenia. Psychological Medicine, 32(3): 439–49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kraepelin, E., Barclay, R. M., and Robertson, G. M. (1919). Dementia Praecox and Paraphrenia. Edinburgh, Scotland: E & S Livingstone.Google Scholar
Kraut, M. A., Kremen, S., Segal, J. B., Calhoun, V., Moo, L. R., and Hart, J. Jr. (2002). Object activation from features in the semantic system. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 14(1): 24–36.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kraut, M. A., Calhoun, V., Pitcock, J. A., Cusick, C., and Hart, J. Jr. (2003). Neural hybrid model of semantic object memory: implications from event-related timing using fMRI. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 9(7): 1031–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Liddle, P. F., Friston, K. J., Frith, C. D., Hirsch, S. R., Jones, T., and Frackowiak, R. S. (1992). Patterns of cerebral blood flow in schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 160: 179–86.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manns, J. R., Hopkins, R. O., and Squire, L. R. (2003). Semantic memory and the human hippocampus. Neuron, 38(1): 127–33.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Manschreck, T. C., Maher, B. A., Milavetz, J. J., Ames, D., Weisstein, C. C., and Schneyer, M. L. (1988). Semantic priming in thought disordered schizophrenic patients. Schizophrenia Research, 1(1): 61–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, A., and Chao, L. L. (2001). Semantic memory and the brain: structure and processes. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 11(2): 194–201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGuire, P. K., Quested, D. J., Spence, S. A., Murray, R. M., Frith, C. D., and Liddle, P. F. (1998). Pathophysiology of “positive” thought disorder in schizophrenia. British Journal of Psychiatry, 173: 231–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salokangas, R. K., Honkonen, T., Stengard, E., and Koivisto, A. M. (2002). Symptom dimensions and their association with outcome and treatment setting in long-term schizophrenia. Results of the DSP project. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 56(5): 319–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sax, K. W., Strakowski, S. M., McElroy, S. L., Keck, P. E. Jr., and West, S. A. (1995). Attention and formal thought disorder in mixed and pure mania, Biological Psychiatry, 37: 420–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slotnick, S. D., Moo, L. R., Kraut, M. A., Lesser, R. P., and Hart, J. Jr. (2002). Interactions between thalamic and cortical rhythms during semantic memory recall in human. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 99(9): 6440–3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solovay, M. R., Shenton, M. E., Gasperetti, C., Coleman, M., Kestnbaum, E., Carpenter, J. T., and Holzman, P. S. (1986). Scoring manual for the Thought Disorder Index. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 12(3): 483–96.Google ScholarPubMed
Spitzer, M., Braun, U., Hermle, L., and Maier, S. (1993). Associative semantic network dysfunction in thought-disordered schizophrenic patients: direct evidence from indirect semantic priming. Biological Psychiatry, 34(12): 864–77.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spitzer, M., Weisker, I., Winter, M., Maier, S., Hermle, L., and Maher, B. A. (1994). Semantic and phonological priming in schizophrenia. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 103(3): 485–94.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spitzer, M. and Mundt, C. (1994). Interchanges between psychology and psychiatry: the continental tradition. Current Opinion in Psychiatry, 7: 417–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spitzer, M. (1997). A cognitive neuroscience view of schizophrenic thought disorder. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 23(1): 29–50.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thompson-Schill, S. L., D'Esposito, M., Aguirre, G. K., and Farah, M. J. (1997). Role of left inferior prefrontal cortex in retrieval of semantic knowledge: a reevaluation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 94(26): 14792–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×