Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gvvz8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T02:06:59.463Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

12 - Psychological Assessment In and Over Time: Challenges of Assessing Psychological Constructs and Processes in Cultural Dynamics

from Part 3 - Culture and Assessment

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 January 2021

Michael Bender
Affiliation:
Universiteit van Tilburg, The Netherlands
Byron G. Adams
Affiliation:
Universiteit van Tilburg, The Netherlands
Get access

Summary

Cross-cultural methodology has made significant progress. Yet, research methodology is in part a response to the challenges posed by research agendas given the intellectual and technological resources available at the time. Just as the contemporary achievements in cross-cultural research methodology are a response to the necessity of the synchronic cross-cultural research in the globalizing world of the late 20th century, the changing cultural landscape of the early 21st century demands a diachronic research agenda – cross-temporal research of psychological phenomena in human populations. Meaning equivalence and autocorrelation are highlighted as major challenges to both synchronic and diachronic approaches in cross-cultural psychology. Researchers need to meet these challenges in the future.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2021

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1985). Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Brislin, R. W. (1970). Back-Translation for Cross-Cultural Research. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 1, 185216.Google Scholar
Campbell, D. T. (1975). On the conflicts between biological and social evolution and between psychology and moral tradition. American Psychologist, 30, 11031126.Google Scholar
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., & Feldman, M. W. (1981). Cultural transmission and evolution. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Chatfield, C. (2016). The analysis of time series: An introduction. Boca Raton: CRC Press.Google Scholar
Chiu, C.-Y., & Hong, Y.-Y. (2013). Social psychology of culture. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Cooley, C. H. (1902). Human nature and the social order. New York: Charles Scribner and Sons.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Devine, P. G., & Elliot, A. J. (1995). Are racial stereotypes really fading? The Princeton trilogy revisited. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 21, 11391150.Google Scholar
Dow, M. M. (2007). Galton’s problem as multiple network autocorrelation effects: Cultural trait transmission and ecological constraint. Cross-Cultural Research, 41, 336363.Google Scholar
Dow, M. M., & Eff, E. A. (2008). Global, regional, and local network autocorrelation in the standard cross-cultural sample. Cross-Cultural Research, 42, 148171.Google Scholar
Eshel, G. (2011). Spatiotemporal data analysis. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Gelfand, M. J., Raver, J. L., Nishii, L., Leslie, L. M., Lun, J., Lim, B. C., & Yamaguchi, S. (2011). Differences between tight and loose cultures: A 33-nation study. Science, 332, 11001104.Google Scholar
Gilbert, G. M. (1951). Stereotype persistence and change among college students. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 46, 245254.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M. (2013). The changing psychology of culture from 1800 through 2000. Psychological Science, 24, 17221731. 7Google Scholar
Grossmann, I., & Varnum, M. E. (2015). Social structure, infectious diseases, disasters, secularism, and cultural change in America. Psychological Science, 26, 311324Google Scholar
Hamamura, T. (2012). Are cultures becoming individualistic? A cross-temporal comparison of individualism-collectivism in the United States and Japan. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 16, 324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture’s consequences: International differences in work-related values. Beverly Hills: Sage.Google Scholar
Huntington, S. P. (1997). The clash of civilisations. New York: Simon and Schuster.Google Scholar
Jebb, A. T., Tay, L., Wang, W., & Huang, Q. (2015). Time series analysis for psychological research: examining and forecasting change. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 727.Google Scholar
Karlins, M., Coffman, T. L., & Walters, G. (1969). On the fading of social stereotypes: Studies in three generations of college students. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 13, 116.Google Scholar
Kashima, Y. (2005). Is culture a problem for social psychology? Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 8, 1938.Google Scholar
Kashima, Y. (2008). A social psychology of cultural dynamics: Examining how cultures are formed, maintained, and transformed. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 107120.Google Scholar
Kashima, Y. (2014). How can you capture cultural dynamics? Frontiers in Psychology, 5(995), 116.Google Scholar
Kashima, Y. (2016a). Cultural dynamics. Current Opinion in Psychology, 8, 9397.Google Scholar
Kashima, Y. (2016b). Culture and psychology in the 21st century: Conceptions of culture and person revised. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 47, 420.Google Scholar
Kashima, Y. (2018). What is culture for? In Matsumoto, D. (ed.), Handbook of culture and psychology (2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kashima, Y., Bain, P., & Perfors, A. (2019). The psychology of cultural dynamics: What is it, what is known, and what is yet to be known? Annual Review of Psychology, 70, 499529.Google Scholar
Kashima, Y., & Gelfand, M. J. (2012). History of culture and psychology. In Kruglanski, A. & Stroebe, W. (eds.), Handbook of the history of social psychology. New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Kashima, Y., & Kashima, E. S. (2003). Individualism, GNP, climate, and pronoun drop: Is individualism determined by affluence and climate, or does language use play a role? Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 34, 125134.Google Scholar
Katz, D., & Braly, K. (1933). Racial stereotypes of one hundred college students. The Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 28, 280290.Google Scholar
Koplenig, A., & Müller-Spitzer, C. (2016). Population size predicts lexical diversity, but so does the mean sea level: Why it is important to correctly account for the structure of temporal data. PLoS ONE, 11, 114.Google Scholar
Leung, K., & Bond, M. H. (2004). Social axioms: A model of social beliefs in multi-cultural perspective. In Zanna, M. (ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (vol. 36, pp. 118197). San Diego: Elsevier Academic Press.Google Scholar
Matsumoto, D., & van de Vijver, F. J. (2010). Cross-cultural research methods in psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A. (2011). Cultural evolution: How Darwinian theory can explain human culture and synthesize the social sciences. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Michel, J.-B., Shen, Y. K., Aiden, A. P., Veres, A., Gray, M. K., Team, T. G. B., & Aiden, E. L. (2011). Quantitative analysis of culture using millions of digitized books. Science, 331, 176182.Google Scholar
Morris, M. W., Chiu, C. Y., & Liu, Z. (2015). Polycultural psychology. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 631659.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Naroll, R. (1961). Two solutions to Galton’s problem. Philosophy of Science, 28, 1539.Google Scholar
Park, H., Twenge, J. M., & Greenfield, P. M. (2014). The Great Recession: Implications for adolescent values and behavior. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 5, 310318.Google Scholar
Roberts, S., & Winters, J. (2013). Linguistic diversity and traffic accidents: Lessons from statistical studies of cultural traits. PLoS ONE, 8, 113.Google Scholar
Santos, H. C., Varnum, M. E., & Grossmann, I. (2017). Global increases in individualism. Psychological Science, 28, 12281239.Google Scholar
Schwartz, S. H. (2011). Studying values: Personal adventure, future directions. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 42, 307319.Google Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1969). Speech acts: An essay in the philosophy of language. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Twenge, J. M., & Campbell, W. K. (2001). Age and birth cohort differences in self-esteem: A cross-temporal meta-analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5, 321344.Google Scholar
Uz, I. (2015). The index of cultural tightness and looseness among 68 countries. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 46, 319335.Google Scholar
Valente, T. W. (2010). Social networks and health: Models, methods, and applications (vol. 1), Oxford: Oxford University Press New York.Google Scholar
Van de Vijver, F. J. R., & Leung, K. (1997). Methods and data analysis for cross-cultural research (vol. 1), Newcastle upon Tyne: Sage.Google Scholar
Van de Vijver, F. J. R., & Leung, K. (2011). Equivalence and bias: A review of concepts, models, and data analytic procedures. In Matsumoto, D. & van de Vijver, F. J. R. (eds.), Cross-cultural research methods in psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Van de Vijver, F. J. R., Van Hemert, D. A., & Poortinga, Y. H. (2015). Multilevel analysis of individuals and cultures. London: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Werner, O., & Campbell, D. T. (1970). Translating, working through interpreters, and the problem of decentering. In A handbook of method in cultural anthropology (pp. 398420). American Museum of Natural History.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×