Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-24T18:06:28.224Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

10 - Creating Equitable Opportunities for Language and Literacy Development in Childhood and Adolescence

from Part One - Factors Influencing Language Development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  11 August 2022

James Law
Affiliation:
University of Newcastle upon Tyne
Sheena Reilly
Affiliation:
Griffith University, Queensland
Cristina McKean
Affiliation:
University of Newcastle upon Tyne
Get access

Summary

Language is one of the most remarkable developmental accomplishments of childhood and a tool for life. Over the course of childhood and adolescence, language and literacy develop in dynamic complementarity, shaped by children’s developmental circumstances. Children’s developmental circumstances include characteristics of the child, their parents, family, communities and schools, and the social and cultural contexts in which they grow up. This chapter uses data collected in Growing up in Australia: The Longitudinal Study of Australian Children (LSAC) that was linked to Australia’s National Assessment of Literacy and Numeracy (NAPLAN) to quantify the effects of multiple risk factors on children’s language and literacy development. Latent class analysis and growth curve modelling are used to identify children’s developmental circumstances (i.e. risk profiles) and quantify the effects of different clusters of risk factors on children’s receptive vocabulary growth and reading achievement from age 4 to 15. The developmental circumstances that gave rise to stark inequalities in language and literacy comprise distinct clustering of sociodemographic, cognitive and non-cognitive risk factors. The results point to the need for cross-cutting social, health and education policies and coordinated multi-agency interventions efforts to address social determinants and break the cycle of developmental disadvantage.

Type
Chapter
Information
Language Development
Individual Differences in a Social Context
, pp. 231 - 256
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2022

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Australian Curriculum Assessment and Reporting Authority. (2015a). About My School. www.myschool.edu.au/about/Google Scholar
Australian Curriculum Assessment and Reporting Authority. (2015b). About NAPLAN. www.nap.edu.au/about/test-developmentGoogle Scholar
Australian Curriculum Assessment and Reporting Authority. (2015c). ICSEA 2015: Technical report. Sydney.Google Scholar
Baker, C., Maguire, B., Daraganova, G., & Sipthorp, M. (2016). Using My School data in the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children (LSAC). LSAC technical paper no. 16. Melbourne: Australian Institute of Family Studies.Google Scholar
Britto, P. R., Lye, S. J., Proulx, K., Yousafzai, A. K., Matthews, S. G., Vaivada, T., … Bhutta, Z. A. (2017). Nurturing care: Promoting early childhood development. The Lancet, 389(10064), 91102. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(16)31390-3Google Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1979). The ecology of human development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U., & Morris, P. (2006). The bioecological model of human development. In Lerner, R. & Damon, W. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Theoretical models of human development (pp. 793828). Hoboken, NJ: Wiley.Google Scholar
Christensen, D., Taylor, C. L., & Zubrick, S. R. (2017). Patterns of multiple risk exposures for low receptive vocabulary growth 4–8 years in the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children. PLOS ONE, 12(1), e0168804. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0168804Google Scholar
Christensen, D., Zubrick, S. R., Lawrence, D., Mitrou, F., & Taylor, C. L. (2014). Risk factors for low receptive vocabulary abilities in the preschool and early school years in the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children. PLOS ONE, 9(7), e101476. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0101476Google Scholar
Cumming, G., & Calin-Jageman, R. (2017). Introduction to the new statistics: Estimation, open science, and beyond. New York, NY: Routledge.Google Scholar
Daraganova, G., Edwards, B., & Sipthorp, M. (2013). Using National Assessment Program — Literacy and Numeracy (NAPLAN) data in the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children (LSAC). LSAC technical paper no. 8. Melbourne: Australian Institute of Family Studies.Google Scholar
de Lemos, M., & Doig, B. (1999). Who am I? Developmental assessment manual. Melbourne: Australian Council for Education Research.Google Scholar
Dunn, L. M., Dunn, L. M., & Williams, K. T. (1997). Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test-III. Circle Pines, MN: American Guidance Service.Google Scholar
Goss, P., & Chisholm, C. (2016). Widening gaps: What NAPLAN tells us about student progress. Technical report. Melbourne: Grattan Institute.Google Scholar
Heckman, J. (2008). Schools, skills and synapses. Economic Inquiry, 46(3), 289324.Google Scholar
Hosmer, D., & Lemeshow, S. (1989). Applied logistic regression. New York, NY: Wiley Interscience.Google Scholar
Kagan, J. (2018). Kinds of individuals defined by patterns of variables. Development and Psychopathology, 30(4), 11971209. doi:10.1017/S095457941800055XGoogle Scholar
Kessler, R. C., Andrews, G., Colpe, L. J., Hiripi, E., Mrocrek, D. K., & Normand, S. L. (2002). Short screening scales to monitor population prevalence and trends in non-specific psychological distress. Psychological Medicine, 32, 959976. doi:10.1017/s0033291702006074CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lanza, S. T., & Cooper, B. R. (2016). Latent class analysis for developmental research. Child Development Perspectives, 10(1), 5964. doi:10.1111/cdep.12163Google Scholar
Lanza, S. T., & Rhoades, B. L. (2011). Latent class analysis: An alternative perspective on subgroup analysis in prevention and treatment. Prevention Science, 14(2), 157168. doi:10.1007/s11121-011-0201-1CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lanza, S. T., Rhoades, B. L., Nix, R. L., & Greenberg, M. T. (2010). Modeling the interplay of multilevel risk factors for future academic and behavior problems: A person-centered approach. Development and Psychopathology, 22(2), 313. doi:10.1017/S0954579410000088Google Scholar
Law, J., Rush, R., King, T., Westrupp, E., & Reilly, S. (2018). Early home activities and oral language skills in middle childhood: A quantile analysis. Child Development, 89(1), 295309.Google Scholar
Longitudinal Study of Australian Children. (2022). Accessing LSAC data. https://growingupinaustralia.gov.au/data-and-documentation/accessing-lsac-dataGoogle Scholar
Marmot, M. (2018). An inverse care law for our time. British Medical Journal, 362, k3216. doi:10.1136/bmj.k3216CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marmot, M., Friel, S., Bell, R., Houweling, T., & Taylor, S. (2008). Closing the gap in a generation: Health equity through action on the social determinants of health. The Lancet, 372(9650), 16611669. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(08)61690-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLachlan, R., Gilfillan, G., & Gordon, J. (2013). Deep and persistent disadvantage in Australia. Staff working paper. Canberra: Productivity Commission.Google Scholar
Misson, S., & Sipthorp, N. (2007). LSAC technical paper no. 5: Wave 2 weighting and non-response. SSRN. doi:10.2139/ssrn.3857985Google Scholar
Muthén, L., & Muthén, B. (2015). Mplus user’s guide (7th ed.). Los Angeles, CA: Muthén and Muthén.Google Scholar
Organization for Economic Co-operation and Development (OECD). (2017). Starting strong 2017: Key OECD indicators on early childhood education and care. http://dx.doi.org/10.1787/9789264276116-enGoogle Scholar
Rothman, S. (2003). An Australian version of the Adapted PPVT-lll for use in research. Melbourne: Australian Council for Educational Research.Google Scholar
Sanson, A., Prior, M., Garino, E., Oberklaid, F., & Sewell, J. (1987). The structure of infant temperament: Factor analysis of the Revised Infant Temperament Questionnaire. Infant Behavior and Development, 10, 97104.Google Scholar
Sawyer, A. C. P., Chittleborough, C. R., Mittinty, M. N., Miller‐Lewis, L. R., Sawyer, M. G., Sullivan, T., & Lynch, J. W. (2015). Are trajectories of self‐regulation abilities from ages 2–3 to 6–7 associated with academic achievement in the early school years? Child: Care, Health and Development, 41(5), 744754. doi:10.1111/cch.12208Google Scholar
Sen, A. (1999). Development as freedom. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Singer, J., & Willett, J. (2003). Applied longitudinal data analysis: Modeling change and event occurrence. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Soloff, C., Lawrence, D., & Johnstone, R. (2005). LSAC sample design. LSAC technical paper no. 1. Melbourne: Australian Institute of Family Studies.Google Scholar
Soloff, C., Lawrence, D., Misson, S., & Johnstone, R. (2006). Wave 1 weighting and non-response. LSAC technical paper no. 3. (18329918). Melbourne: Australian Institute of Family Studies www.aifs.gov.au/growingup/pubs/technical/tp1.pdfGoogle Scholar
Taylor, C., Christensen, D., Lawrence, D., Mitrou, F., & Zubrick, S. (2013). Risk factors for children’s receptive vocabulary development from four to eight years in the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children. PLOS ONE, 8(9). doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0073046Google Scholar
Taylor, C., Zubrick, S., & Christensen, D. (2016). Barriers to parent–child book reading in early childhood. International Journal of Early Childhood, 48(3), 295309. doi:10.1007/s13158-016-0172-2Google Scholar
Taylor, C. L., Zubrick, S. R., & Christensen, D. (2019). Multiple risk exposures for reading achievement in childhood and adolescence. Journal of Epidemiology and Community Health, 73(5), 427434. doi:10.1136/jech-2018-211323Google Scholar
United Nations. (2019). Sustainable development goals. www.un.org/sustainabledevelopment/development-agenda/Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (2004). The Wechsler intelligence scale for children (4th ed.). London: Pearson Assessment.Google Scholar
Zubrick, S. (2016). Longitudinal research: Applications for the design, conduct and dissemination of early childhood research. In Farrell, A., Kagan, S., & Tisdall, E. (Eds.), The SAGE handbook of early childhood research (pp. 201222). London: SAGE. doi:10.4135/9781473920859Google Scholar
Zubrick, S. R., Lucas, N., Westrupp, E., & Nicholson, J. M. (2014). Parenting measures in the Longitudinal Study of Australian Children: Construct validity and measurement quality, Waves 1 to 4. LSAC technical paper no. 12. Canberra: Department of Social Services.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×