Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-18T17:24:26.882Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

9 - Cognitive and behavioral probes of developmental landmarks for use in functional neuroimaging

from Part 3 - Normal development

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  06 January 2010

Monique Ernst
Affiliation:
National Institutes of Health, Baltimore
Judith M. Rumsey
Affiliation:
National Institute of Mental Health, Bethesda, Maryland
Get access

Summary

Introduction

Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) allows neuroscientists to examine the function of the human brain, especially the developing human brain, in a relatively noninvasive manner. Previous approaches were relatively indirect techniques such as scalp-recorded electroencephalography (EEG) and event-related brain potentials or brain imaging techniques that required exposure to ionizing radiation, for example positron emission tomography (PET), single photon emission computed tomography (SPECT), and X-ray computed tomography (CT). While these latter techniques may be used with pediatric patient populations when clinically warranted, the ethics of exposing children to unnecessary radiation for the advancement of science are still under debate (Casey and Cohen, 1996; Morton, 1996; Zametkin et al., 1996).With fMRI, developmental research is possible. This technique is described in detail in Chapter 3. In the current chapter age-appropriate behavioral paradigms and task designs for use with fMRI in the context of developmental studies are described.

The significance of studying functional brain changes in a developmental context becomes more apparent when we consider aspects of brain development in general (see Chapter 7). The brain continues to develop after birth and well into childhood.Huttenlocher (1990, 1997) has demonstrated that pruning and reorganization of some cortical regions are relatively protracted. While synaptic density reaches adult levels by approximately 5 months in the visual cortex, prefrontal cortex still shows a 10% greater synaptic density at 7 years than in adulthood.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2000

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×