Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-94fs2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-04T18:13:40.632Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Part II - Conceptual issues

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 October 2015

Walter Glannon
Affiliation:
University of Calgary
Get access
Type
Chapter
Information
Free Will and the Brain
Neuroscientific, Philosophical, and Legal Perspectives
, pp. 25 - 80
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2015

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Buszaki, G. 2006. Rhythms of the Brain. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Farah, M. 2005. Neuroethics: the practical and the philosophical. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9: 3440.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fried, I., Mukamel, R. and Kreiman, G. 2011. Internally generated preactivation of single neurons on human medial frontal cortex predicts volition. Neuron 69: 548562.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haggard, P. 2005. Conscious intention and motor cognition. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 9: 290295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haggard, P. 2008. Human volition: towards a neuroscience of will. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 9: 934946.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haynes, J.-D. 2010. Beyond Libet: long-term prediction of free choices from neuroimaging signals. In Sinnott-Armstrong, W. and Nadel, L. (eds.), Conscious Will and Responsibility: A Tribute to Benjamin Libet. New York: Oxford University Press, Ch. 8.Google Scholar
Haynes, J.-D., Sakai, K., Rees, G., Gilbert, S. Frith, C. and Passinghuam, R. 2007. Reading hidden intentions in the human brain. Current Biology 17: 323328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Libet, B. 1985. Unconscious cerebral initiative and the role of conscious will in voluntary action. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 8: 529566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Northoff, G. 2011. Neuropsychoanalysis in Practice: Brain, Self and Objects. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Northoff, G. 2012. Immanuel Kant's mind and the brain's resting state. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 16 (July): 356359.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Northoff, G. 2014a. Unlocking the Brain. Vol. I: Coding. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Northoff, G. 2014b. Unlocking the Brain. Vol. II: Consciousness. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Roskies, A. 2008. Neuroimaging and inferential distance. Neuroethics 1 (1): 1930.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spence, S. 2009. The Actor's Brain: Exploring the Cognitive Neuroscience of Free Will. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walter, H. 2001. Neurophilosophy of Free Will: From Libertarian Illusions to a Concept of Natural Autonomy, trans. Klohr, C.. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

References

Aristotle, (1925) Nichomachean Ethics (trans. Ross, D.). Oxford University Press, (pagination is standard).Google Scholar
Banks, W. and Isham, E. (2009) We infer rather than perceive the moment when we decided to act. Psychological Science 20.1: 1721.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blair, J. (2003) Neurobiological basis of psychopathy. British Journal of Psychiatry 182: 57.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Breiter, H. and Rauch, S. (1996) Functional MRI and the study of OCD: from symptom provocation to cognitive-behavioral Probes of cortico-striatal systems and the amygdala. Neuroimage 4: s127–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chamberlain, S., Menzies, L., Hampshire, A., Suckling, J., Fineberg, N. et al. (2008) Orbitofrontal dysfunction in patients with obsessive compulsive disorder Science 321: 421–2.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, A. (2008) Supersizing the Mind: Embodiment, Action and Cognitive Extension. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Danquah, A., Farrell, M. and O'Boyle, D. (2008) Biases in the subjective timing of perceptual events: Libet et al. (1983) revisited. Consciousness and Cognition 17: 616–27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davidson, D. (1980) Essays on Actions and Events. Oxford: Clarendon.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. (1991). Consciousness Explained. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. (2003) The self as responding – and responsible – artefact. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences 1001: 3950.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freud, S. (1986) The Essentials of Psychoanalysis (ed. Freud, A.). London: Penguin.Google Scholar
Friedlander, L. and Desrocher, M. (2006) Neuroimaging studies of obsessive-compulsive disorder in adults and children Clinical Psychology Review 26: 3249.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gillett, G. (1992) Actions, causes and mental ascriptions, in Objections to physicalism (ed. Robinson, H). Oxford University Press, pp. 81100.Google Scholar
Gillett, G. (1993) Freedom of the will and mental content. Ratio 6: 89107.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gillett, G. (2008) Subjectivity and Being Somebody: Human Identity, and Neuroethics. Exeter: Imprint Academic.Google Scholar
Gillett, G. (2010) Intentional action, moral responsibility and psychopaths, in Responsibility and Psychopathy: Interfacing Law, Psychiatry and Philosophy (ed. Malatesti, Luca & McMillan, John). Oxford University Press, pp. 283–98.Google Scholar
Gillett, G. and Liu, S. (2012) Free will and Necker's cube: reason, language and top-down control in cognitive neuroscience. Philosophy 87.1: 2950.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haggard, P. (2005) Conscious intention and motor control. Trends in Cognitive Science 9.6: 290–5.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hampshire, S. (1969) Some difficulties in knowing, in Philosophy, Science and Method: Essays in Honor of Ernest Nagel (ed. Morgenbesser, S., Suppes, P. & White, M.). New York: St Martin's Press.Google Scholar
Hanna, R. (2009) Freedom teleology and rational causation. Kant Yearbook 1: 99142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughlings Jackson, J. (1878) On affectations of speech from disease of the brain (1). Brain 1: 304–30.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hughlings Jackson, J. (1879) On affectations of speech from disease of the brain (2). Brain 1: 203–22.Google Scholar
Hughlings Jackson, J. (1884) On affectations of speech from disease of the brain (2). British Medical Journal 12 April: 703–7.Google Scholar
Hughlings Jackson, J. (1887) Remarks on the evolution and dissolution of the nervous system. British Journal of Psychiatry 33: 2548.Google Scholar
Jeannerod, M. 2006: Motor Cognition: What Actions Tell the Self. New York: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kant, I. (1789 [1929]) The Critique of Pure Reason (trans. Smith, N. Kemp). London: MacmillanGoogle Scholar
Kant, I. (1948) The Moral Law (trans. Paton, H. J.) London: Hutchinson, p. 76.Google Scholar
Kiehl, K., Smith, A. M., Hare, R. D., Mendrek, A., Forster, B. B., et al. (2001) Limbic abnormalities in affective processing by criminal psychopaths as revealed by functional Magnetic Resonance Imaging. Biological Psychiatry 50: 677–84.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kwon, J. S., Kim, J., Lee, D., Lee, J., Kim, M. et al. (2003) Neural correlates of clinical symptoms and cognitive dysfunctions in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatric Research: Neuroimaging 122: 3747.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Libet, B. (1985) Unconscious cerebral initiative and the role of conscious will in voluntary action. The Behavioural and Brain Sciences 8: 529–66.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Libet, B. (2002) The timing of mental events: Libet's experimental findings and their implications. Consciousness and cognition 11: 291–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lorenz, E. (1963) Deterministic nonperiodic flow. Journal of the Atmospheric Sciences 20: 130–41.2.0.CO;2>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luria, A. R. (I 973) The Working Brain. Harmondsworth: Penguin.Google Scholar
Lynam, D. and Gudonis, L. (2005) The development of psychopathy. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology 1: 381407.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mercier, H. and Sperber, D. (2011) Why do humans reason? Arguments for an argumentative theory. Behavioral and Brain Sciences 34: 94111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, M. (1986) Causes of behaviour. Philosophical Quarterly 36.143: 123–44.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neisser, U. (1976) Cognition and Reality. San Francisco: Freeman.Google Scholar
Nietzsche, F. (1886 [1975]) Beyond Good and Evil (trans. Hollingdale, R. J.) London: Penguin.Google Scholar
Roskies, A. (2010) How does neuroscience affect our conception of volition? Annual Review of Neuroscience 33: 109–30.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spitzer, M. (1999) The Mind within the Net. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whiteside, S., Port, J. and Abramowitz, J. (2004) A meta-analysis of finctional neuroimaging in obsessive-compulsive disorder. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging 132: 6979.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Williams, B. (1985) Ethics and the Limits of Philosophy. London: Fontana.Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (1953) Philosophical Investigations (PI) (trans. Anscombe, G. E. M.). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Wolf, S. (1993) Freedom within Reason. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Zhu, J. (2004) Locating volition. Consciousness and Cognition 13: 302–22.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

References

Bays, P. M., Wolpert, D. M., and Flanagan, J. R. (2005) Perception of the consequences of self-action is temporally tuned and event driven. Current Biology 15:11251128.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bell, C. C. (2001) Memory-based expectations in electrosensory systems. Current Opinion in Neurobiology 11:481487.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blakemore, S. J., Frith, C. D., Wolpert, D. M. (1999) Spatio-temporal prediction modulates the perception of self-produced stimuli. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 11:551559.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crapse, T. B. and Sommer, M. A. (2008) Corollary discharge across the animal kingdom. Nature Reviews Neuroscience 9:587600.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davies, M., Coltheart, M., Langdon, R., and Breen, N. (2001) Monothematic delusions: towards a two-factor account. Philosophy, Psychiatry & Psychology 8:133158.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dicke, P. W., Chakraborty, S., and Thier, P. (2008) Neuronal correlates of perceptual stability during eye movements. European Journal of Neuroscience 27:9911002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ernst, M. O. and Banks, M. S. (2002) Humans integrate visual and haptic information in a statistically optimal fashion. Nature 415:429433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ernst, M. O. and Bulthoff, H. H. (2004) Merging the senses into a robust percept. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8:162169.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fischer, J. M., Kane, R., Pereboom, D., and Vargas, M., eds. (2007) Four Views on Free Will. Walden, MA: Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
Frith, C. (1992) The Cognitive Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Frith, C. (1994) Theory of mind in schizophrenia. In The Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia (David, A. S. & Cutting, J. C., eds.), pp. 147161. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Frith, C. (2005) The self in action: lessons from delusions of control. Consciousness and Cognition 14:752770.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frith, C. D., Blakemore, S., and Wolpert, D. M. (2000a) Explaining the symptoms of schizophrenia: abnormalities in the awareness of action. Brain Research Reviews 31:357363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frith, C. D. (2000b) Abnormalities in the awareness and control of action. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, B: Biological Sciences 355:17711788.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gallagher, S. (2000) Philosophical conceptions of the self: implications for cognitive science. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 4:1421.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haggard, P., Clark, S., and Kalogeras, J. (2002). Voluntary action and conscious awareness. Nature Neuroscience 5: 382385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kording, K. P. and Wolpert, D. M. (2004) Bayesian integration in sensorimotor learning. Nature 427:244247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Libet, B., Gleason, C. A., Wright, E. W., and Pearl, D. K. (1983) Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential): the unconscious initiation of a freely voluntary act. Brain 106 (Pt 3):623642.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lindner, A., Thier, P., Kircher, T. T., Haarmeier, T., and Leube, D. T. (2005) Disorders of agency in schizophrenia correlate with an inability to compensate for the sensory consequences of actions. Current Biology 15:11191124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, J. W. and Fletcher, P. C. (2012) Sense of agency in health and disease: a review of cue integration approaches. Consciousness and Cognition 21:5968.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, J. W., Wegner, D. M., and Haggard, P. (2009) Modulating the sense of agency with external cues. Consciousness and Cognition 18:10561064.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Connor, T. (2009) Conscious willing and the emerging sciences of brain and behavior. In Downward Causation and the Neurobiology of Free Will (Ellis, G. F. R., Murphy, N., and O'Connor, T., eds.), pp. 173186. New York: Springer Publications.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pereboom, D. (2005) Defending hard incompatibilism. Midwest Studies in Philosophy 29:228247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shergill, S. S., Samson, G., Bays, P. M., Frith, C. D., and Wolpert, D. M. (2005) Evidence for sensory prediction deficits in schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry 162:23842386.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sperry, R. W. (1950) Neural basis of the spontaneous optokinetic response produced by visual inversion. Journal of Comparative Physiology and Psychology 43:482489.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Synofzik, M. and Voss, M. (2010) Disturbances of the sense of agency in schizophrenia. In Neuropsychology of the Sense of Agency (Balconi, M., ed.), pp. 145156. New York/Heidelberg: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Synofzik, M. and Vosgerau, G. (2012) Beyond the comparator model. Consciousness and Cognition 21:13.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Synofzik, M., Vosgerau, G., and Newen, A. (2008) Beyond the comparator model: a multifactorial two-step account of agency. Consciousness and Cognition 17:219239.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Synofzik, M., Vosgerau, G., and Lindner, A. (2009) Me or not me: an optimal integration of agency cues? Consciousness and Cognition 18:10651068.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Synofzik, M., Thier, P., Leube, D. T., Schlotterbeck, P., and Lindner, A. (2010) Misattributions of agency in schizophrenia are based on imprecise predictions about the sensory consequences of one's actions. Brain 133:262271.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tomasello, M. (2008) Origins of Human Communication. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2009) Why We Cooperate: Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
von Holst, E. and Mittelstaedt, H. (1950) Das Reafferenzprinzip. Naturwissenschaften 37:464476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Voss, M., Moore, J., Hauser, M., Gallinat, J., Heinz, A., and Haggard, P. (2010) Altered awareness of action in schizophrenia: a specific deficit in predicting action consequences. Brain 133:31043112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilke, C., Synofzik, M., and Lindner, A. (2012) The valence of action outcomes modulates the perception of one's actions. Consciousness and Cognition 21:1829.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolpert, D. M., Ghahramani, Z., and Jordan, M. I. (1995) An internal model for sensorimotor integration. Science 269:18801882.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolpert, D. M., Miall, R. C., and Kawato, M. (1998) Internal models in the cerebellum. Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2:338347.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×