Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-94fs2 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-02T21:03:31.748Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

4 - The Nature of Resemblance: Homologues in the Nervous System and Behavioral Correspondence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  23 July 2009

Aldo Fasolo
Affiliation:
University of Torino
Giovanni Boniolo
Affiliation:
Università degli Studi di Padova, Italy
Gabriele De Anna
Affiliation:
University of St Andrews, Scotland
Get access

Summary

It would be very difficult for any one with even much more knowledge than I possess, to determine how far animals exhibit any traces of … high mental powers. This difficulty arises from the impossibility of judging what passes through the mind of an animal; and again, the fact that writers differ to a great extent in the meaning which they attribute to the above terms, causes a further difficulty.

Darwin 1871, p. 85

INTRODUCTION

Morality is related variously to behavioral tendencies and cognitive capacities. In this chapter, I do not discuss what the forms and modes of these relations can be. Instead, I focus on the topic of the homologues in the nervous system and then consider whether and to what extent the capacities that are relevant for morality can be explained through biological comparisons. Nevertheless, the quest for the biological basis of behavior and cognition must rely on a sound comparative approach, satisfying Theodor Bullock's three Rs requirement (roots, rules, relevance; cf. Bullock 1983) for the study of cognition and highly integrated behavior. The satisfaction of this requirement, indeed, seems to be the only way to discern scientifically plausible biological hypotheses from merely intriguing scientifically sounding metaphors.

In what follows, I argue that any attempt to explain (human) ethics through considerations concerning animal behavior is hopeless, unless we manage to individuate strict homological correspondences among all the feasible generic analogies between human behavior and animal behavior.

This conclusion is reached through an argument that involves five theses.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2006

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abouheif, E. 1997. Developmental Genetics and Homology: A Hierarchical Approach. Trends Ecol. Evol. 12: 405–408.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barton, R. A., and Harvey, P. H. 2000. Mosaic Evolution of Brain Structure in Mammals. Nature 405: 1055–1058.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bateson, W. 1882. On Numerical Variation in Teeth with a Discussion of the Conception of Homology. Proc. Zool. Soc. London: 102–115.Google Scholar
Bateson, W. 1884. Materials for Study of Variation. Reprint, Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press, 1992.
Bjorklund, D. F., and Pellegrini, D. A. 2003. The Origin of the Human Nature: Evolutionary Developmental Psychology. Washington, D.C.: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Bota, M., and Arbib, M. A. 2001. The Neurohomology Database. Neurocomputing 38–40: 1627–1631.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bullock, T. H. 1983. Implications for Neuroethology from Comparative Neurophysiology. In Advances in Vertebrate Neuroethology, ed. Ewert, J. P., Caprinica, R. R., and Ingle, D. J., 53–75. New York: Plenum.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butler, A. B., and Hodos, W. 1996. Comparative Vertebrate Neuroanatomy: Evolution and Adaptation. New York: Wiley-Liss.Google Scholar
Butler, A. B., and Saidel, W. M. 2000. Defining Sameness: Historical, Biological and Generative Homology. BioEssays 22: 846–853.3.0.CO;2-R>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Catania, K. C. 2000. Cortical Organization in Insectivora: The Parallel Evolution of Sensory Cortex and the Brain. Brain Behavior and Evolution 55: 311–321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Changeux, J. P. 1983. L'homme neuronal. Paris: Fayard.Google Scholar
Changeux, J. P. 2002. L'homme de verité. Paris: Odile Jacob.Google Scholar
Cosmides, L., and Tooby, J. 1992. Cognitive Adaptations for Social Exchange. In The Adapted Mind, ed. Barkow, J., Cosmides, L., and Tooby, J., 163–228. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. 1871. The Descent of Man. Amherst, N.Y.: Prometheus Books, 1998.Google Scholar
Beer, G. R. 1971. Homology, an Unsolved Problem. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Deacon, T. W. 1990. Rethinking Mammalian Brain Evolution. Amer. Zool. 30: 629–705.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deacon, T. W. 1995. On Telling Growth from Parcellation in Brain Evolution. In Behavioural Brain Research in Naturalistic and Seminaturalistic Settings: Possibilities and Perspectives, ed. Alleva, E., Lipp, H.-P., Fasolo, A., Nadel, L., and Ricceri, L., 37–72. Dordrecht: Kluwer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deacon, T. W. 1997. The Symbolic Species: The Co-evolution of Language and the Brain. New York: W. W. Norton.Google Scholar
Deacon, T. W. 2000. Evolutionary Perspectives on Language and Brain Plasticity. J. Commun. Disord. 33: 273–291.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Edelman, G. M. 1987. Neural Darwinism: The Theory of Neuronal Group Selection. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Edelman, G. M. 1988. Topobiology: An Introduction to Molecular Embryology. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Edelman, G. M. 1993. Neural Darwinism: Selection and Reentrant Signaling in Higher Brain Function. Neuron 10: 115–125.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. 1970. Ethology: The Biology of Behavior. New York: Holt & Rinehart.Google Scholar
Enard, W., et al. 2002. Intra- and Interspecific Variation in Primate Gene Expression Patterns. Science 296: 340–343.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fasolo, A., and Malacarne, G. 1988. Comparing the Structure of Brains: Implications for Behavioral Homologies. In Intelligence and Evolutionary Biology, ed. Jerison, H. and Jerison, I., 119–142. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Finlay, B. L., Darlington, R. B., and Nicastro, N. 2001. Developmental Structure in Brain Evolution. Behav. Brain. Sci. 24: 263–278.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fritzsch, B. 1998. Of Mice and Genes: Evolution of Vertebrate Brain Development. Brain. Behav. Evol. 52(4–5): 207–217.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilbert, S. F., and Bolker, J. A. 2001. Homologies of Process and Modular Elements of Embryonic Construction. J. Exp. Zool. 291(1): 1–12.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodwin, B. C. 1984. Changing from an Evolutionary to a Generative Paradigm. In Evolutionary Theory: Paths into the Future, ed. Pollard, J. W., 99–120. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. 1977. Ontogeny and Phylogeny. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. 2002. The Structure of the Evolutionary Theory. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Hirschfeld, L., and Gelman, S. 1994. Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity in Cognition and Culture. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hirth, F., and Reichert, H. 1999. Conserved Genetic Programs in Insect and Mammalian Brain Development. BioEssays 21: 677–684.3.0.CO;2-8>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hodos, W. 1976. The Concept of Homology and the Evolution of Behavior. In Evolution, Brain and Behavior: Persistent Problems, ed. Masterton, R. B., Hodos, W., and Jerison, H. J., 153–167. Hillsdale, N.J.: Lawrence A Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Houle, D. 2001. Characters as the Units of Evolutionary Change. In The Character Concept in Evolutionary Biology, ed. Wagner, G. P., 109–140. San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Jarman, P. J. 1981. Prospects for Interspecific Comparison in Sociobiology. In Current Problems in Sociobiology, ed. King's College Sociobiology Group, Cambridge, 323–342. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Jerison, H. J. 1973. Evolution of the Brain and Intelligence. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Jerison, H. J. 1991. Brain Size and the Evolution of Mind. The 59th James Arthur Lecture on the Evolution of the Human Brain. American Museum of Natural History, New York.
Kaas, J. H. 2000. Organizing Principles of Sensory Representations. In Evolutionary Developmental Biology of the Cerebral Cortex, ed. Bock, G. R. and Cardew, G.. Novartis Foundation Symposium 228: 188–189. Chichester: Wiley.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Karmiloff-Smith, A. 1992. Beyond Modularity: A Developmental Perspective on Cognitive Science. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Katz, P. S., and Harris-Warrick, R. M. 1999. The Evolution of Neuronal Circuits Underlying Species-Specific Behavior. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 9(5): 628–633.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kim, J., and Kim, M. 2001. The Mathematical Structure of Characters and Modularity. In The Character Concept in Evolutionary Biology, ed. Wagner, G. P., 215–236. San Diego: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Krebs, J. R., and Davies, N. B. 1997. Behavioural Ecology: An Evolutionary Approach. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Krubitzer, L. 1995. The Organization of Neocortex in Mammals: Are Species Differences Really So Different?Trends in Neuroscience 18: 408–417.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Medina, L., and Reiner, A. 1999. Do Birds Possess Homologues of Mammalian Primary Visual Somatosensory and Motor Cortices?Trends in Neuroscience 23: 1–12.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, P. H. 1996. “Mapping the Mind”: Where Are the State Lines?Cogn. Devel. 11: 141–155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Minelli, A. 1998. Molecules, Developmental Modules, and Phenotypes: A Combinatorial Approach to Homology. Molec. Phylog. Evol. 3: 340–347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Minelli, A. 2003. The Development of Animal Form. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Müller, G. B., and Newman, S. A. 1999. Generation, Integration, Autonomy: Three Steps in the Evolution of Homology. In Homology, ed. Bock, G. R. and Cardew, G.. Novartis Foundation Symposium 222: 65–79. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
Nieuwenhuys, R. 1998. Comparative Neuroanatomy: Place, Principles and Program. In The Central Nervous System of Vertebrates, ed. Nieuwenhuys, R., Donkelaar, H. J. Ten, and Nicholson, C., 273–326. Berlin: Springer-Verlag.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Northcutt, R. G. 1981. Evolution of the Telencephalon in Nonmammals. Ann. Rev. Neurosci. 4: 301–350.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Northcutt, R. G. 1984. Evolution of the Vertebrate Central Nervous System: Patterns and Processes. Amer. Zool. 24: 701–716.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Northcutt, R. G. 2001. Evolution of the Nervous System. Changing Views of Brain Evolution. Brain Research Bulletin 55: 663–674.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Northcutt, R. G., and Kaas, J. H. 1995. The Emergence and Evolution of Mammalian Neocortex. Trends in Neuroscience 18: 373–379.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O'Leary, D., and Nakagawa, Y. 2002. Patterning Centers, Regulatory Genes and Extrinsic Mechanisms Controlling Arealization of the Neocortex. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 12: 14–25.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Patterson, C. 1982. Morphological Characters and Homology. In Problems of Phylogenetic Reconstruction, ed. Joysey, K. A. and Frida, A. E., 21–74. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J., and Preuss, T. M. 1995. Theory of Mind: Evolutionary History of a Cognitive Specialization. Trends in Neuroscience 18: 418–424.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Preuss, T. M. 1995. The Argument from Animals to Humans in Cognitive Neuroscience. In The Cognitive Neurosciences, ed. Gazzaniga, M., 1127–1241. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Puelles, L., and Medina, L. 2002. Field Homology as a Way to Reconcile Genetic and Developmental Variability with Adult Homology. Brain Research Bulletin 57: 243–255.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Purves, D. 1994. Neural Activity and the Growth of the Brain. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Redies, C., and Puelles, L. 2001. Modularity in Vertebrate Brain Development and Evolution. BioEssays 23: 1100–1111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rifkin, S. 1995. The Evolution of Primate Intelligence. Harvard Brain, vol. 2. http://hcs.harvard.edu/~husn/BRAIN/vol2/Primate.html.Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., Fogassi, L., and Gallese, V. 2002. Motor and Cognitive Functions of the Ventral Premotor Cortex. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 12: 149–154.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roth, G., and Wullimann, M. (eds.). 2001. Brain Evolution and Cognition. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Schlaggar, B. L.. 2002. Functional Neuroanatomical Differences between Adults and School-Age Children in the Processing of Single Words. Science 296: 1476–1479.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sereno, M. I. 1998. Brain Mapping in Animals and Humans. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 8: 188–194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Striedter, G. F. 1998. Stepping into the Same River Twice: Homologues as Recurrent Attractors in Epigenetic Landscapes. Brain Behav. Evolution 38: 177–189.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Striedter, G. F. 2002. Brain Homology and Function: An Uneasy Alliance. Brain Research Bulletin 57: 239–242.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tooby, J., and Cosmides, L. 1995. Mapping the Evolved Functional Organization of Brain and Mind. In The Cognitive Neurosciences, ed. Gazzaniga, M., 1185–1198. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Valen, L. M. 1982. Homology and Causes. J. Morphol. 173: 305–312.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waddington, C. 1957. The Strategy of the Genes. London: Allen and Unwin.Google Scholar
Wagner, G. P. 1994. Homology and the Mechanisms of Development. In Homology: The Hierarchical Basis of Comparative Biology, ed. Hall, B. K., 274–301. New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Wagner, G. P. 1996. Homologues, Natural Kinds and the Evolution of Modularity. Am. Zool. 36: 36–43.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, G. P., and Altenberg, L. 1996. Complex Adaptations and the Evolution of Evolvability. Evolution 50: 967–976.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wagner, G. P., Mezey, J., and Calabretta, R. 2005. Natural Selection and the Origin of Modules. In Modularity: Understanding the Development and Evolution of Complex Natural Systems, ed. Callabaut, W. and Rasskin-Gutman, D., 33–49. Cambridge, Mass.: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Wake, D. B. 1994. Comparative Terminology. Science 265: 268–269.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wake, D. B. 1999. Homoplasy, Homology and the Problem of “Sameness” in Biology. In Homology, ed. Bock, G. R. and Cardew, G.. Novartis Foundation Symposium 222: 24–46. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar
West-Ebehard, M. J. 2003. Developmental Plasticity and Evolution. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Wiley, E. O. 1981. Phylogenetics: The Theory and Practice of Phylogenetic Systematics. New York: Wiley.Google Scholar
Williams, G. C. 1966. Adaptation and Natural Selection: A Critique of Some Current Evolutionary Thought. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Wray, G. A. 1999. Evolutionary Dissociations between Homologous Genes and Homologous Structures. In Homology, ed. Bock, G. R. and Cardew, G.. Novartis Foundation Symposium 222: 189–203. Chichester: Wiley.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×