Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-m6dg7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-03T05:27:55.510Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

1 - The faculty of language: what is it, who has it, and how did it evolve?

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 June 2012

Marc D. Hauser
Affiliation:
Department of Psychology, Harvard University
Noam Chomsky
Affiliation:
Department of Linguistics and Philosophy, Massachusetts Institute of Technology
W. Tecumseh Fitch
Affiliation:
Department of Neurobiology and Cognition, University of Vienna
Richard K. Larson
Affiliation:
State University of New York, Stony Brook
Viviane Déprez
Affiliation:
Rutgers University, New Jersey
Hiroko Yamakido
Affiliation:
Lawrence University, Wisconsin
Get access

Summary

We argue that an understanding of the faculty of language requires substantial interdisciplinary cooperation. We suggest how current developments in linguistics can be profitably wedded to work in evolutionary biology, anthropology, psychology, and neuroscience. We submit that a distinction should be made between the faculty of language in the broad sense (FLB) and in the narrow sense (FLN). FLB includes a sensory-motor system, a conceptual-intentional system, and the computational mechanisms for recursion, providing the capacity to generate an infinite range of expressions from a finite set of elements. We hypothesize that FLN only includes recursion and is the only uniquely human component of the faculty of language. We further argue that FLN may have evolved for reasons other than language, hence comparative studies might look for evidence of such computations outside of the domain of communication (for example, number, navigation, and social relations).

If a martian graced our planet, it would be struck by one remarkable similarity among Earth's living creatures and a key difference. Concerning similarity, it would note that all living things are designed on the basis of highly conserved developmental systems that read an (almost) universal language encoded in DNA base pairs. As such, life is arranged hierarchically with a foundation of discrete, unblendable units (codons, and, for the most part, genes) capable of combining to create increasingly complex and virtually limitless varieties of both species and individual organisms. In contrast, it would notice the absence of a universal code of communication (Figure 1.1).

Type
Chapter
Information
The Evolution of Human Language
Biolinguistic Perspectives
, pp. 14 - 42
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2010

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Chomsky, N., Aspects of the Theory of Syntax (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1965).Google Scholar
Chomsky, N., Reflections on Language (Pantheon, New York, 1975).Google Scholar
Hauser, M. D., The Evolution of Communication (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1996).Google Scholar
Jackendoff, R., Foundations of Language (Oxford University Press, New York, 2002).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jenkins, L., Biolinguistics (Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 2000).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lenneberg, E. H., Biological Foundations of Language (Wiley, New York, 1967).Google Scholar
Lieberman, P., The Biology and Evolution of Language (Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, 1984).Google Scholar
Liberman, A., Speech: A Special Code (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1996).Google Scholar
Fitch, W. T., Trendsin Cognitive Science 4, 258 (2000).CrossRef
Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., How Monkeys See the World: Inside the Mind of Another Species (University of Chicago Press, Chicago, 1990).Google Scholar
Doupe, A., Kuhl, P., Annual Revue of Neuroscience 22, 567 (1999).CrossRef
Marler, P., American Scientist 58, 669 (1970).
Darwin, C., On the Origin of Species (John Murray, London, 1859).Google Scholar
Darwin, C., The Descent of Man and Selection in Relation to Sex (John Murray, London, 1871).Google Scholar
Liberman, A. M., Harris, K. S., Hoffman, H. S., Griffith, B. C., Journal of Experimental Psychology 54, 358 (1957).CrossRef
Liberman, A. M., Cooper, F. S., Shankweiler, D. P., Studdert-Kennedy, M., Psycholical Revue. 74, 431 (1967).CrossRef
Kuhl, P. K., Miller, J. D., Science 190, 69 (1975).CrossRef
Kuhl, P. K., Padden, D. M., Perception and Psychophysics 32, 542 (1982).CrossRef
Kluender, K. R., Diehl, R., Killeen, P. R., Science 237, 1195 (1987).CrossRef
Gould, S. J., in Evolution, Brain and Behavior: Persistent Problems, Masterton, R. B., Hodos, W., Jerison, H., Eds. (Wiley, New York, 1976), pp. 175–179.Google Scholar
Gehring, W. J., Master Control Genes in Development and Evolution: The Homeobox Story (Yale University Press, New Haven, CT, 1998).Google Scholar
Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L., in The Design of Animal Communication, Hauser, M. D., Konishi, M., Eds. (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1999), pp. 391–418.Google Scholar
Marler, P., Journal of Neurobiology 33, 1 (1997).3.0.CO;2-8>CrossRef
Nottebohm, F., in The Design of Animal Communication, Hauser, M. D., Konishi, M., Eds. (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1999), pp. 63–110.Google Scholar
Petitto, L. A., Marentette, P., Science 251, 1483 (1991).CrossRef
Kluender, K. R., Lotto, A. J., Holt, L. L., in Listening to Speech: An Auditory Perspective, Greenberg, S., Ainsworth, W., Eds. (Erlbaum, Mahwah, NJ, in press).
Jackendoff, R., Trendsin Cognitive Science 3, 272 (1999).CrossRef
Pinker, S., Bloom, P., Behavioral and Brain Sciences 13, 707 (1990).CrossRef
Dawkins, R., The Blind Watchmaker (Norton, New York, 1986).Google Scholar
Bickerton, D., Species and Language (University of Chicago Press, Chicago, 1990).Google Scholar
Dunbar, R., Grooming, Gossip and the Evolution of Language (Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, 1996).Google Scholar
Kimura, D., Neuromotor Mechanisms in Human Communication (Oxford University Press, Oxford, 1993).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chomsky, N., Rules and Representations (Columbia University Press, New York, 1980).Google Scholar
Hauser, M. D., in Language, Brain, and Cognitive Development: Essays in Honor of Jacques Mehler, Dupoux, E., Ed. (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 2001), pp. 417–434.Google Scholar
Enard, W.et al., Nature 418, 869 (2002).CrossRef
Smith, J. Maynard, Szathmary, E., The Major Transitions of Evolution (Freeman, Oxford, 1995).Google Scholar
Barrett, L., Dunbar, R., Lycett, J., Human Evolutionary Psychology (Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ, 2002).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Buss, D., Evolutionary Psychology (Allyn & Bacon, London, 1999).Google Scholar
Gould, S. J., Lewontin, R. C., Proceedings of the Royal Society of London 205, 281 (1979).
Williams, G. C., Adaptation and Natural Selection (Princeton University Press, Princeton, NJ, 1966).Google Scholar
Chomsky, N., The Minimalist Program (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1995).Google Scholar
Collins, C., Local Economy (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1997).Google Scholar
Epstein, S. D., Hornstein, N., Working Minimalism (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1999).Google Scholar
Haegeman, L., Introduction to Government and Binding Theory (Blackwell, Oxford, 1991).Google Scholar
Hurford, J. R., Studdert-Kennedy, M., Knight, C., Eds., Approaches to the Evolution of Language: Social and Cognitive Bases (Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 1998).
Wray, A., Ed., The Transition to Language (Oxford Univ. Press, Oxford, 2002).
Liberman, A., Whalen, D. H., Trendsin Cognitive Science 4, 187 (2000).CrossRef
Lieberman, P., Uniquely Human (Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, 1991).Google Scholar
Dooling, R. J., Best, C. T., Brown, S. D., Journal of the Acoustic Society of America 97, 1839 (1995).CrossRef
Sinnott, J. M., Brown, C. H., Journal of the Acoustic Society of America 102, 588 (1997).CrossRef
Sommers, M. S., Moody, D. B., Prosen, C. A., Stebbins, W. C., Journal of the Acoustic Society of America 91, 3499 (1992).CrossRef
Kluender, K. R., Lotto, A. J., Holt, L. L., Bloedel, S. L., Journal of the Acoustic Society of America 104, 3568 (1998).CrossRef
Ramus, F., Hauser, M. D., Miller, C. T., Morris, D., Mehler, J., Science 288, 349 (2000).CrossRef
Fitch, W. T., Journal of the Acoustic Society of America 102, 1213 (1997).CrossRef
Fitch, W. T., Kelley, J. P., Ethology 106, 559 (2000).CrossRef
Hauser, M. D., Evans, C. S., Marler, P., Animal Behaviour 45, 423 (1993).CrossRef
Owren, M. J., Bernacki, R., Journal of the Acoustic Society of America 83, 1927 (1988).CrossRef
Owren, M. J., Journal of Comparative Psychology 104, 20 (1990).CrossRef
Rendall, D., Owren, M. J., Rodman, P. S., Journal of the Acoustic Society of America 103, 602 (1998).CrossRef
Negus, V. E., The Comparative Anatomy and Physiology of the Larynx (Hafner, New York, 1949).Google Scholar
Fitch, W. T., Reby, D., Proceedings of the Royal Society of London Series B 268, 1669 (2001).CrossRef
Trout, J. D., Psychological Review 108, 523 (2000).CrossRef
Donald, M., in Approaches to the Evolution of Language: Social and Cognitive Bases, Hurford, J. R., Studdert-Kennedy, M., Knight, C., Eds. (Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 1998), pp. 44–67.Google Scholar
Studdert-Kennedy, M., Human Neurobiology 2, 191 (1983).
Janik, V. M., Slater, P. J. B., Animal Behaviour 60, 1 (2000).CrossRef
Tomasello, M., Call, J., Primate Cognition (Oxford University Press, Oxford, 1997).Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., Arbib, M. A., Trendsin Cognitive Science 2, 188 (1998).
Rizzolatti, G., Fadiga, L., Fogassi, L., Gallese, V., Archives of Italian Biology 137, 169 (1999).
Chaminade, T., Meltzoff, A. N., Decety, J., Neuroimage 15, 318 (2002).CrossRef
Decety, J., Chaminade, T., Grezes, J., Meltzoff, A. N., Neuroimage 15, 265 (2002).CrossRef
Iacoboni, M.et al., Science 286, 2526 (1999).CrossRef
Nowak, M. A., Komarova, N. L., Niyogi, P., Science 291, 114 (2001).CrossRef
Nowak, M. A., Komarova, N. L., Trendsin Cognitive Science 5, 288 (2001).CrossRef
Nowak, M. A., Plotkin, J. B., Jansen, V. A., Nature 404, 495 (2000).CrossRef
Nowak, M. A., Komarova, N. L., Niyogi, P., Nature 417, 611 (2002).CrossRef
Heyes, C. M., Huber, F., The Evolution of Cognition (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 2000).Google Scholar
Shettleworth, S., Cognition, Evolution and Behavior (Oxford University Press, New York, 1998).Google Scholar
Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., in The Tanner Lectures on Human Values, Peterson, G., Ed. (University of Utah Press, Salt Lake City, UT, 1998), pp. 173–210.Google Scholar
Hauser, M. D., Wild Minds: What Animals Really Think (Holt, New York, 2000).Google Scholar
Gallistel, C. R., The Organization of Learning (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1990).Google Scholar
Pepperberg, I. M., The Alex Studies (Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA, 2000).Google Scholar
Premack, D., Gavagai! or the Future History of the Animal Language Controversy (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1986).Google Scholar
Premack, D.Woodruff, G., Behavioural and Brain Sciences 4, 515 (1978).CrossRef
Premack, D., Premack, A., Original Intelligence (McGraw-Hill, New York, 2002).Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C., Behavioral and Brain Sciences 6, 343 (1983).CrossRef
Hare, B., Call, J., Agnetta, B., Tomasello, M., Animal Behaviour 59, 771 (2000).CrossRef
Hare, B., Call, J., Tomasello, M., Animal Behaviour 61, 139 (2001).CrossRef
Heyes, C. M., Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21, 101 (1998).
Povinelli, D. J., Eddy, T. J., Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 247 (1996).
Seyfarth, R. M., Cheney, D. L., Marler, P., Science 210, 801 (1980).CrossRef
Dittus, W. P. G., Animal Behaviour 32, 470 (1984).CrossRef
Evans, C. S., Marler, P., in Comparative Approaches to Cognitive Science, Roitblatt, H., Ed. (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1995), pp. 241–282.Google Scholar
Fischer, J., Animal Behaviour 55, 799 (1998).CrossRef
Gouzoules, S., Gouzoules, H., Marler, P., Animal Behaviour 32, 182 (1984).CrossRef
Hauser, M. D., Animal Behaviour 55, 1647 (1998).CrossRef
Slobodchikoff, C. N., Kiriazis, J., Fischer, C., Creef, E., Animal Behaviour 42, 713 (1991).CrossRef
Zuberbuhler, K., Cheney, D. L., Seyfarth, R. M., Journal of Comparative Psychology 113, 33 (1999).CrossRef
Bloom, P., Markson, L., Trendsin Cognitive Science 2, 67 (1998).CrossRef
Bloom, P., How Children Learn the Meanings of Words (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 2000).Google Scholar
Gold, E. M., Information and Control 10, 447 (1967).CrossRef
Dehaene, S., The Number Sense (Oxford University Press, Oxford, 1997).Google Scholar
Gallistel, C. R., Gelman, R., Trendsin Cognitive Science 4, 59 (2000).CrossRef
Carey, S., Mind Lang. 16, 37 (2001).CrossRef
Boysen, S. T., Bernston, G. G., Journal of Comparative Psychology 103, 23 (1989).CrossRef
Kawai, N., Matsuzawa, T., Nature 403, 39 (2000).CrossRef
Matsuzawa, T., Nature 315, 57 (1985).CrossRef
Wynn, K., Cognitive Psychology 24, 220 (1992).CrossRef
Chomsky, N., Logical Structure of Linguistic Theory/Excerpted Manuscript (Plenum, New York, 1975).Google Scholar
Chomsky, N.IRE Trans. Inform. Theory 2 (no. 2), 113 (1956).
Chomsky, N.Miller, G., Inform. Control 1, 91 (1958).CrossRef
Harris, Z. S., Language 31, 190 (1955).CrossRef
Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., Newport, E. L., Science 274, 1926 (1996).CrossRef
Saffran, J., Johnson, E., Aslin, R. N., Newport, E., Cognition 70, 27 (1999).CrossRef
Hauser, M. D., Newport, E. L., Aslin, R. N., Cognition 78, B53 (2001).CrossRef
Marcus, G., Vijayan, S., Rao, S. Bandi, Vishton, P. M., Science 283, 77 (1999).CrossRef
Hauser, M. D., Weiss, D., Marcus, G., Cognition 86, B15 (2002).CrossRef
Fitch, W. T., Hauser, M. D., in preparation.
Clayton, N. S., Dickinson, A., Nature 395, 272 (1998).CrossRef
Gallistel, C. R., Cramer, A. E., Journal of Experimental Biology 199, 211 (1996).
Kuhl, P., Perception and Psychophysics 50, 93 (1991).CrossRef
MacNeilage, P. F., Behavioral and Brain Sciences 21, 499(1998).
Studdert-Kennedy, M., in Approaches to the Evolution of Language: Social and Cognitive Bases, Hurford, J. R., Studdert-Kennedy, M., Knight, C., Eds. (Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, 1998), pp. 202–221.Google Scholar
McGurk, H., MacDonald, J., Nature 264, 746 (1976).CrossRef
Santos, L. R., Sulkowski, G. M., Spaepen, G. M., Hauser, M. D., Cognition 83, 241 (2002).CrossRef
Gergerly, G., Bekkering, H., Kiraly, I., Nature 415, 755 (2002).CrossRef
Meltzoff, A. N., Moore, M. K., Infant Behavior and Development 17, 83 (1994).CrossRef
Whiten, A., Custance, D., in Social Learning in Animals: The Roots of Culture, Heyes, C. M., Galef, J. B. G., Eds. (Academic Press, San Diego, CA, 1996), pp. 291–318.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marler, P., Karakashian, S., Gyger, M., in Cognitive Ethology: The Minds of Other Animals, Ristau, C., Ed. (Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ, 1991), pp. 135–186.Google Scholar
Terrace, H. S., Son, L. K., Brannon, E. M., Psychological Science 14, 1 (2003)CrossRef
Herman, L. M., Richards, D. G., Wolz, J. P., Cognition 16, 129 (1984).CrossRef
Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S.et al., Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 58 (1993).CrossRef
Brannon, E. M., Terrace, H. S., Science 282, 746 (1998).CrossRef
Lerdahl, F., Jackendoff, R., A Generative Theory of Tonal Music (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 1983).Google Scholar
Wallin, N., Merker, B., Brown, S. D., The Origins of Music (MIT Press, Cambridge, MA, 2000).Google Scholar
Zatorre, R., Peretz, I., The Biological Foundations of Music (National Academy Press, New York, 2000).Google Scholar
For comments on an earlier draft of the manuscript, we thank Cheney, D., Jackendoff, R., Jenkins, L., Nowak, M., Piatelli-Palmerini, M., Pinker, S., and Seyfarth, R..

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×