Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-dh8gc Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-09T15:35:53.187Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

1 - Contemporary enquiries into a long-standing domain: Drawing research

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  22 September 2009

Chris Lange-Küttner
Affiliation:
London Metropolitan University
Annie Vinter
Affiliation:
Université de Bourgogne, France
Chris Lange-Küttner
Affiliation:
London Metropolitan University
Annie Vinter
Affiliation:
Université de Bourgogne, France
Get access

Summary

drawing behaviour has occupied European psychologists from the turn of the last century (Kerschensteiner, 1905; Luquet, 1927; Ricci, 1887; Rouma, 1913), maintained their interest ever since, and subsequently also attracted some attention from psychologists in other continents such as the United States. The main contribution of the early work was to describe how this typically human behaviour develops, and in particular which stages it follows (Piaget and Inhelder, 1956). Still, perhaps like many other scientists nowadays, Piaget saw drawing only as a figurative, illustrative instrument of representation, as opposed to rational and operational thought devoted to the genuine understanding of reality. Thus, drawing behaviour was not frequently studied in relation to cognitive development. However, a notable exception was the ‘Draw-a-person test’ designed by Goodenough (Goodenough, 1926; Goodenough and Harris, 1950) which assesses mental age in children via the human figure drawing, and, because it has a high correlation with intelligence tests until adolescence, it is still in use today.

Since the 1970s and 1980s, a refreshed interest in drawing from developmental and cognitive psychologists from an empirical, experimental, statistically underpinned perspective has flourished, as evident in two books by Freeman (Freeman, 1980, see also his current contribution to this book; Freeman and Cox, 1985).

Type
Chapter
Information
Drawing and the Non-Verbal Mind
A Life-Span Perspective
, pp. 1 - 20
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2008

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Antonelli, I. R., Corsonello, A., Pedone, C., Trojano, L., Acanfora, D., Spada, A., Izzo, O. and Rengo, F. (2006). Drawing impairment predicts mortality in severe COPD. Chest, 130, 1687–94.Google Scholar
Bahrick, L. E. and Watson, J. S. (1985). Detection of intermodal proprioceptive-visual contingency as a potential basis of self-perception in infancy. Developmental Psychology, 21, 963–73.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cox, M. V. (1986). Children's drawings. London: Penguin.Google Scholar
Cox, M. V. (2005). The pictorial world of the child. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Davis, A. (1985). Conflict between canonicality and array-specificity in young children's drawings. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 3, 363–72.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edwards, B. (1992). Drawing on the right side of the brain. London: Souvenir Press.Google Scholar
Freeman, N. H. (1980). Strategies of representation in young children. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Freeman, N. H. and Cox, M. V. (eds.) (1985). Visual order. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Frith, U. (2003). Autism: explaining the enigma. Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Gallup, G. G. Jr (1970). Chimpanzees: self-recognition. Science, 167, 86–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gellert, E. (1968). Comparison of children's self-drawing with their drawings of other persons. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 26, 123–38.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gergely, G. (2006). The development of understanding self and agency. In Goswami, U. (ed.), Childhood cognitive development (pp. 26–46). Oxford: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Golomb, C. (1973). Children's representation of the human figure: the effects of models, media and instruction. Genetic Psychology Monographs, 87, 197–251.Google Scholar
Goodenough, F. L. (1926). Measurement of intelligence by drawings. New York: World Book Company.Google Scholar
Goodenough, F. L. and Harris, D. B. (1950). Studies in the psychology of children's drawings. Ⅱ. Psychological Bulletin, 47, 369–433.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodnow, J. J. (1977). Children's drawings. London: Fontana.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodnow, J. J. and Levine, R. A. (1973). The ‘grammar of action’: sequence and syntax in children's copying behaviour. Cognitive Psychology, 4, 82–98.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goswami, U. (1992). Analogical reasoning in children. Hove: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gross, J. and Hayne, H. (1999). Young children's recognition and description of their own and others’ drawings. Developmental Science, 2, 476–89.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hayworth, K. J. and Biederman, I. (2006). Neural evidence for intermediate representations in object recognition. Vision Research, 46, 4024–31.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hund, A. M. and Plumert, J. M. (2002). Delay-induced bias in children's memory for location. Child Development, 73, 829–40.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hund, A. M. and Plumert, J. M. (2005). The stability and flexibility of spatial categories. Cognitive Psychology, 50, 1–44.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ino, T.,Asada, T., Ito, J., Kimura, T. and Fukuyama, H. (2003). Parieto-frontal networks for clock drawing revealed with fMRI. Neuroscience Research, 45, 71–7.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Karmiloff-Smith, A. (1990). Constraints on representational change: evidence from children's drawings. Cognition, 34, 57–83.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kerschensteiner, G. (1905). Die Entwicklung der zeichnerischen Begabung. Munich: Gerber.Google Scholar
Kho, K. H., Bergh, W. M., Spetgensx, W. P. J. and Leijten, F. S. S. (2006). Figuring out drawing-induced epilepsy. Neurology, 66, 723–6.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lange-Küttner, C. (1998). Pressure, velocity and time in speeded drawing of basic graphic pattern by young children. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 86, 1299–1310.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lange-Küttner, C. and Thomas, G. V. (eds.) (1995). Drawing and looking. London: Pearson/Prentice Hall (distributed by www.amazon.co.uk).Google Scholar
Liben, L. S. (1988). Conceptual issues in the development of spatial cognition. In Stiles-Davis, J., Kritchevsky, M. and Bellugi, U. (eds.), Spatial cognition: brain bases and development (pp. 167–94). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Longcamp, M., Zerbato-Poudou, , M.-T. and Velay, J.-L. (2005). The influence of writing practice on letter recognition in preschool children: a comparison between handwriting and typing. Acta Psychologica, 119, 67–79.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lowery, N., Giovanni, L., Mozley, Harper L., Arnold, S. E., Bilker, W. B., Gur, R. E. and Moberg, P. J. (2003). Relationship between clock-drawing and neuropsychological and functional status in elderly institutionalized patients with schizophrenia. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 11, 621–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luquet, G. H. (1927). Le dessin enfantin [Children's drawing]. Paris: Alcan.Google Scholar
Makuuchi, M., Kaminaga, T. and Sugishita, M. (2003). Both parietal lobes are involved in drawing: a functional MRI study and implications for constructional apraxia. Cognitive Brain Research, 16, 338–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mandler, J. M. (1988). The development of spatial cognition: on topological and euclidian representation. In Stiles-Davis, J., Kritchevsky, M. and Bellugi, U. (eds.), Spatial cognition: brain bases and development (pp. 423–32). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Miller, B. L. (2006). Figuring out drawing-induced epilepsy. Commentary. Neurology, 66, 619.Google Scholar
Moritz, C. H., Johnson, S. C., McMillan, , K. M., Haughton, , V. M. and Meyerand, , M. E. (2004). Functional MRI neuroanatomic correlates of the Hooper Visual Organization Test. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 10, 939–47.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newcombe, N. S. and Huttenlocher, , J. (2003). Making space: the development of spatial representation and reasoning. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Nitschke, M. F., Erdmann, C., Trillenberg, P., Sprenger, A., Kock, N., Sperner, J. and Klein, C. (2006). Functional MRI reveals activation of a subcortical network in a 5-year-old girl with genetically confirmed myoclonus-dystonia. Neuropediatrics, 37, 79–82.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pew, R. W. (1974). Levels of analysis in motor control. Brain Research, 71, 393–400.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piaget, J. and Inhelder, B. (1956). The child's conception of space. London: Routledge and Kegan Paul.Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J. (1995). The unduplicated self. In Rochat, P. (ed.), The self in infancy: theory and research (pp. 161–92). Amsterdam: Elsevier.Google Scholar
Povinelli, D. J. and Simon, B. B. (1998). Young children's understanding of briefly versus extremely delayed images of the self: emergence of the autobiographical stance. Developmental Psychology, 34, 188–94.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ricci, C. (1887). L'arte dei bambini [The art of children]. Bologna.Google Scholar
Rochat, P. and Morgan, R. (1995). Spatial determinants in the perception of self-produced leg movements in 3- to 5-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 31, 626–36.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rouma, G. (1913). Le langage graphique de l'enfant [The graphic language of children]. Paris: Alcan.Google Scholar
Schmuckler, M. A. (1996). Visual-proprioceptive intermodal perception in infancy. Infant Behaviour and Development, 19, 221–32.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Selfe, L. (1977). Nadia: a case of extraordinary drawing ability in an autistic child. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Selfe, L. (1983). Normal and anomalous representational drawing ability in children. London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Selfe, L. (1985). Anomalous drawing development. In Freeman, N. H. and Cox, M. V. (eds.), Visual order: the nature and development of pictorial representation (pp. 135–54). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Servos, P., Goodale, M. A. and Humphrey, G. K. (1993). The drawing of objects by a visual form agnosic: contribution of surface properties and memorial representations. Neuropsychologia, 31, 251–9.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solso, R. L. (2001). Brain activities in a skilled versus a novice artist: an fMRI study. Leonardo, 34, 31–4.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spencer, J. P. and Hund, A. M. (2003). Developmental continuity in the processes that underlie spatial recall. Cognitive Psychology, 47, 432–80.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sutton, P. J. and Rose, D. H. (1998). The role of strategic visual attention in children's drawing development. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 68, 87–107.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, G. V. and Silk, A. M. J. (1990). An introduction to the psychology of children's drawings. New York:New York University Press.Google Scholar
Thomassen, A. J. W. M. and Van Galen, G. P. (1992). Handwriting as a motor task. In Summers, J. J. (ed.), Approaches to the study of motor control and learning (pp. 113–44). Amsterdam: North-Holland.Google Scholar
Treiman, R., Goswami, U. and Bruck, M. (1990). Not all nonwords are alike: implication for reading development and theory. Memory and Cognition, 18, 559–67.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sommers, P. (1984). Drawing and cognition: descriptive and experimental studies of graphic production processes. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sommers, P. (1989). A system for drawing and drawing-related neuropsychology. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 6, 117–64.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Willats, J. (1985). Drawing systems revisited: the role of denotation systems in children's figure drawings. In Freeman, N. H. and Cox, M. V. (eds.), Visual order (pp. 78–100). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Willats, J. (1995). An information-processing approach to drawing development. In Lange-Küttner, C. and Thomas, G. V. (eds.), Drawing and looking (pp. 27–43). London: Pearson/Prentice Hall (distributed by www.amazon.co.uk).Google Scholar
Willats, J. (2005). Making sense of children's drawings. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.
Wilson, M. and Wilson, B. (1982). The case of the disappearing of the two-eyed profile or how little children influence the drawings of little children. Review of Research in Visual Arts Education, 15, 19–32.Google Scholar
Wulf, G. and Schmidt, R. A. (1997). Variability of practice and implicit motor learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, 23, 987–1006.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×