Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-j824f Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-02T21:57:57.297Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 5 - Aspects of classroom treatment: toward a more comprehensive view of second language education

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  05 October 2012

Patrick Allen
Affiliation:
Ontario Institute for Studies in Education
Merrill Swain
Affiliation:
Ontario Institute for Studies in Education
Birgit Harley
Affiliation:
Ontario Institute for Studies in Education
Jim Cummins
Affiliation:
Ontario Institute for Studies in Education
Get access

Summary

In an influential paper published in the early eighties, H. H. Stern distinguished between learning a language through use in the environment (i.e., functionally) and through processes of language study and practice (i.e., formally). As Stern (1981) pointed out, this aspect of language behavior can be characterized as a psycholinguistic/pedagogic continuum, or P-scale. There is nothing inherently good or bad about activities at either end of the scale, and in organized language teaching we often find an interplay between formal and functional approaches. In this paper the term experiential is used to refer to activities at the functional end of Stern's P-scale, while analytic refers to activities at the formal end. The experiential-analytic distinction is analogous (although not necessarily identical) to distinctions made by other investigators with regard to general pedagogic orientation. Barnes (1976), for example, discusses interpretive versus transmission teaching; Wells (1982) distinguishes between collaborative and transmission orientations; while Cummins (1984) uses the labels reciprocal interaction and transmission to refer to these two dimensions.

The relationship between experiential and analytic activities in the classroom has recently emerged as one of the key issues in second language pedagogy. According to some authorities (e.g., Krashen 1982) analytic or grammar-based activities are of minimal benefit, since conscious “learning” cannot be converted into the central process of unconscious L2 “acquisition.” Others (e.g., McLaughlin, Rossman, and McLeod 1983) have argued that “controlled” processes may precede the development of “automatic” processes.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 1990

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×