Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-g8jcs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T20:46:00.827Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Part I - Dynamics of Culture, Mind, and Brain

Models and Evidence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 September 2020

Laurence J. Kirmayer
Affiliation:
McGill University, Montréal
Carol M. Worthman
Affiliation:
Emory University, Atlanta
Shinobu Kitayama
Affiliation:
University of Michigan, Ann Arbor
Robert Lemelson
Affiliation:
University of California, Los Angeles
Constance A. Cummings
Affiliation:
The Foundation for Psychocultural Research
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Culture, Mind, and Brain
Emerging Concepts, Models, and Applications
, pp. 51 - 362
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Allen, M., & Friston, K. J. (2016). From cognitivism to autopoiesis: Towards a computational framework for the embodied mind. Synthese, 195(6), 124. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229–016-1288-5Google Scholar
Anderson, J. R., & Gallup, G. G. Jr. (2015). Mirror self-recognition: A review and critique of attempts to promote and engineer self-recognition in primates. Primates, 56(4), 317–26. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10329–015-0488-9Google Scholar
Antón, S. C., Potts, R., & Aiello, L. C. (2014). Evolution of early Homo: An integrated biological perspective. Science, 345(6192), 1236828. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1236828CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Apel, J., & Knutsson, K. (2006). Skilled production and social reproduction: Aspects of traditional stone-tool technologies: Proceedings of a symposium in Uppsala, Sweden, August 20–24, 2003. Societas ologica Upsaliensis.Google Scholar
Arbib, M. A. (2012). How the brain got language: The mirror system hypothesis. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Barkow, J. H., Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1992). The adapted mind: Evolutionary psychology and the generation of culture. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F., & Simmons, W. K. (2015). Interoceptive predictions in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(7), 419–29. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3950CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(4), 577660. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X99002149Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. W. (2003). Abstraction in perceptual symbol systems. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 358(1435), 1177–87. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2003.1319CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–45. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639Google Scholar
Bedny, M., Pascual-Leone, A., Dodell-Feder, D., Fedorenko, E., & Saxe, R. (2011). Language processing in the occipital cortex of congenitally blind adults. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(11), 4429–34. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1014818108Google Scholar
Berwick, R. C., Friederici, A. D., Chomsky, N., & Bolhuis, J. J. (2013). Evolution, brain, and the nature of language. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(2), 8998. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.12.002Google Scholar
Bogin, B., Bragg, J., & Kuzawa, C. (2014). Humans are not cooperative breeders but practice biocultural reproduction. Annals of Human Biology, 41(4), 368–80. https://doi.org/10.3109/03014460.2014.923938Google Scholar
Bonner, J. T. (1980). The evolution of culture in animals. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Bornkessel-Schlesewsky, I., Schlesewsky, M., Small, S. L., & Rauschecker, J. P. (2015). Neurobiological roots of language in primate audition: Common computational properties. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(3), 142–50. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.12.008Google Scholar
Bourdieu, P. (1972). Outline of a theory of practice. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Boyd, R., Richerson, P. J., & Henrich, J. (2011). The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(Supplement 2), 10918–25. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1100290108Google Scholar
Boyette, A. H., & Hewlett, B. S. (2017). Autonomy, equality, and teaching among Aka foragers and Ngandu farmers of the Congo Basin. Human Nature, 28(3), 289322. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110–017-9294-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buckner, R. L., & Krienen, F. M. (2013). The evolution of distributed association networks in the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(12), 648–65. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burdett, E. R. R., Dean, L. G., & Ronfard, S. (2018). A diverse and flexible teaching toolkit facilitates the human capacity for cumulative culture. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 807–18. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164–017-0345-4Google Scholar
Byrge, L., Sporns, O., & Smith, L. B. (2014). Developmental process emerges from extended brain-body-behavior networks. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(8), 395403. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.010Google Scholar
Byrne, R. W. (2016). Evolving insight: How it is we can think about why things happen. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cartmill, M. (2002). Paleoanthropology: Science or mythological charter? Journal of Anthropological Research, 58(2), 183201. https://doi.org/10.1086/jar.58.2.3631035Google Scholar
Cavalli-Sforza, L. L., & Feldman, M. W. (1981). Cultural transmission and evolution: A quantitative approach. Princeton University Press.Google ScholarPubMed
Chomsky, N. (1957). Syntactic structures. Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christiansen, M. H., & Chater, N. (2016). Creating language: Integrating evolution, acquisition, and processing. MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, A. (1997). Being there: Putting brain, body, and world together again. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2006). Language, embodiment, and the cognitive niche. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10(8), 370–4. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2006.06.012Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(3), 181204. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12000477CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cole, M. (1996). Cultural psychology: A once and future discipline. Belknap Press.Google Scholar
Cook, R., Bird, G., Catmur, C., Press, C., & Heyes, C. (2014). Mirror neurons: From origin to function. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 37(2), 177–92. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X13000903Google Scholar
Crick, F. (1970). Central dogma of molecular biology. Nature, 227(5258), 561–3. https://doi.org/10.1038/227561a0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crowther-Heyck, H. (1999). George A. Miller, language, and the computer metaphor and mind. History of Psychology, 2(1), 3764. https://doi.org/10.1037/1093-4510.2.1.37Google Scholar
D’Andrade, R. G. (1982). Cultural meaning systems. In Adams, R. M., Smelser, N. J., & Treiman, D. J. (Eds.), Behavioral and social science research: A national resource. Part II (pp. 197236). National Academy Press.Google Scholar
D’Errico, F., Henshilwood, C., Lawson, G., Vanhaeren, M., Tillier, A.-M., Soressi, M., Bresson, F., Maureille, B., Nowell, A., Lakarra, J., Backwell, L., & Julien, M. (2003). Archaeological evidence for the emergence of language, symbolism, and music: An alternative multidisciplinary perspective. Journal of World Prehistory, 17(1), 170. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1023980201043Google Scholar
Danchin, É., Charmantier, A., Champagne, F. A., Mesoudi, A., Pujol, B., & Blanchet, S. (2011). Beyond DNA: Integrating inclusive inheritance into an extended theory of evolution. Nature Reviews Genetics, 12(7), 475–86. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrg3028Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man, and selection in relation to sex. John Murray.Google Scholar
David, N., & Kramer, C. (2001). Ethnoarchaeology in action. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1976). The selfish gene. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Deacon, T. W. (1997). The symbolic species: The co-evolution of language and the brain. W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S. (1997). The number sense: How the mind creates mathematics. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dehaene, S., & Cohen, L. (2007). Cultural recycling of cortical maps. Neuron, 56(2), 384–98. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2007.10.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dehaene, S., Cohen, L., Morais, J., & Kolinsky, R. (2015). Illiterate to literate: Behavioural and cerebral changes induced by reading acquisition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(4), 234–44. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3924Google Scholar
Duncan, J. (2010). The multiple-demand (MD) system of the primate brain: Mental programs for intelligent behaviour. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(4), 172–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.01.004Google Scholar
Edelman, G. M. (1987). Neural Darwinism: The theory of neuronal group selection. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Edelman, G. M., & Gally, J. A. (2001). Degeneracy and complexity in biological systems. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 98(24), 13763–8. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.231499798Google Scholar
Eren, M. I., Lycett, S. J., Patten, R. J., Buchanan, B., Pargeter, J., & O’Brien, M. J. (2016). Test, model, and method validation: The role of experimental stone artifact replication in hypothesis-driven archaeology. Ethnoarchaeology, 8(2), 103–36. https://doi.org/10.1080/19442890.2016.1213972Google Scholar
Ericsson, K. A., Krampe, R. T., & Tesch-Römer, C. (1993). The role of deliberate practice in the acquisition of expert performance. Psychological Review, 100(3), 363406. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.100.3.363Google Scholar
Faisal, A., Stout, D., Apel, J., & Bradley, B. (2010). The manipulative complexity of Lower Paleolithic stone toolmaking. PLoS ONE, 5(11), e13718. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0013718Google Scholar
Feldman, R. (2016). The neurobiology of human attachments. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(2), 8099. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.11.007Google Scholar
Flynn, E. G., Laland, K. N., Kendal, R. L., & Kendal, J. R. (2013). Target article with commentaries: Developmental niche construction. Developmental Science, 16(2), 296313. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12030Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1975). The language of thought. Thomas Y. Crowell.Google Scholar
Fragaszy, D. M., Biro, D., Eshchar, Y., Humle, T., Izar, P., Resende, B., & Visalberghi, E. (2013). The fourth dimension of tool use: Temporally enduring artefacts aid primates learning to use tools. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 368(1630), 20120410. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0410CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fragaszy, D. M., Eshchar, Y., Visalberghi, E., Resende, B., Laity, K., & Izar, P. (2017). Synchronized practice helps bearded capuchin monkeys learn to extend attention while learning a tradition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114(30), 77987805. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1621071114CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gärdenfors, P., & Högberg, A. (2017). The archaeology of teaching and the evolution of Homo docens. Current Anthropology, 58(2), 188208. https://doi.org/10.1086/691178Google Scholar
Geertz, C. (1973). The interpretation of cultures. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Giddens, A. (1976). New rules of sociological method: A positive critique of interpretative sociologies. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gómez-Robles, A., Hopkins, W. D., Schapiro, S. J., & Sherwood, C. C. (2015). Relaxed genetic control of cortical organization in human brains compared with chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112(48), 14799–804. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1512646112Google Scholar
Goodenough, W. H. (1957). Cultural anthropology and linguistics. In Garvin, P. L. (Ed.), Report on the seventh annual round table meeting on linguistics and language study (pp. 167–73). Georgetown University Press.Google Scholar
Hasson, U., & Frith, C. D. (2016). Mirroring and beyond: Coupled dynamics as a generalized framework for modelling social interactions. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1693), 20150366. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0366Google Scholar
Hecht, E. E., Gutman, D. A., Bradley, B. A., Preuss, T. M., & Stout, D. (2015). Virtual dissection and comparative connectivity of the superior longitudinal fasciculus in chimpanzees and humans. NeuroImage, 108, 124–37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.12.039Google Scholar
Hecht, E. E., Gutman, D. A., Khreisheh, N., Taylor, S. V., Kilner, J., Faisal, A. A., Bradley, B. A., Chaminade, T., & Stout, D. (2014). Acquisition of Paleolithic toolmaking abilities involves structural remodeling to inferior frontoparietal regions. Brain Structure and Function, 220(4), 2315–31. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429–014-0789-6Google Scholar
Hecht, E. E., Gutman, D. A., Preuss, T. M., Sanchez, M. M., Parr, L. A., & Rilling, J. K. (2013). Process versus product in social learning: Comparative diffusion tensor imaging of neural systems for action execution-observation matching in macaques, chimpanzees, and humans. Cerebral Cortex, 23(5), 1014–24. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhs097CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hecht, E. E., Mahovetz, L. M., Preuss, T. M., & Hopkins, W. D. (2017). A neuroanatomical predictor of mirror self-recognition in chimpanzees. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(1), 3748. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw159CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hecht, E. E., Murphy, L. E., Gutman, D. A., Votaw, J. R., Schuster, D. M., Preuss, T. M., Orban, G. A., Stout, D., & Parr, L. A. (2013). Differences in neural activation for object-directed grasping in chimpanzees and humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(35), 14117–34. https://doi.org/10.1523%2FJNEUROSCI.2172-13.2013Google Scholar
Heidegger, M. (1977). The question concerning technology, and other essays (Levitt, W., Trans.). Harper & Row. (Original work published 1954)Google Scholar
Henrich, J. P. (2016). The secret of our success: How culture is driving human evolution, domesticating our species, and making us smarter. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (2008). Five misunderstandings about cultural evolution. Human Nature, 19(2), 119–37. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110–008-9037-1Google Scholar
Henrich, J., & McElreath, R. (2003). The evolution of cultural evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 12(3), 123–35. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.10110Google Scholar
Henshilwood, C. S., & d’Errico, F. (Eds.). (2011). Homo symbolicus: The dawn of language, imagination and spirituality. John Benjamins Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Hewlett, B. S., & Roulette, C. J. (2016). Teaching in hunter-gatherer infancy. Royal Society Open Science, 3(1), 150403. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.150403Google Scholar
Heyes, C. (2003). Four routes of cognitive evolution. Psychological Review, 110(4), 713–27. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.110.4.713Google Scholar
Heyes, C. (2016). Blackboxing: Social learning strategies and cultural evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1693), 20150369. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0369Google Scholar
Heyes, C. (2018). Cognitive gadgets: The cultural evolution of thinking. Harvard University.Google Scholar
Heyes, C. M., & Frith, C. D. (2014). The cultural evolution of mind reading. Science, 344(6190), 1243091. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1243091CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hihara, S., Notoya, T., Tanaka, M., Ichinose, S., Ojima, H., Obayashi, S., Fujii, N., & Iriki, A. (2006). Extension of corticocortical afferents into the anterior bank of the intraparietal sulcus by tool-use training in adult monkeys. Neuropsychologia, 44(13), 2636–46. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2005.11.020Google Scholar
Hill, J., Inder, T., Neil, J., Dierker, D., Harwell, J., & Van Essen, D. (2010). Similar patterns of cortical expansion during human development and evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(29), 13135–40. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1001229107Google Scholar
Hill, K., Barton, M., & Hurtado, A. M. (2009). The emergence of human uniqueness: Characters underlying behavioral modernity. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 18(5), 187200. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.20224Google Scholar
Hodder, I. (2012). Entangled: An archaeology of the relationships between humans and things. Wiley-Blackwell.Google Scholar
Holloway, R. L. (1974). The casts of fossil hominid brains. Scientific American, 231(1), 106–15. http://www.jstor.org/stable/24950124CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Horner, V., Proctor, D., Bonnie, K. E., Whiten, A., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2010). Prestige affects cultural learning in chimpanzees. PLoS ONE, 5(5), e10625. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0010625Google Scholar
Hrdy, S. B. (2009). Mothers and others: The evolutionary origins of mutual understanding. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Ingold, T. (1997). Eight themes in the anthropology of technology. Social Analysis, 41(1), 106–38. http://www.jstor.org/stable/23171736Google Scholar
Ingold, T. (1998). From complementarity to obviation: On dissolving the boundaries between social and biological anthropology, archaeology and psychology. Zeitschrift für Ethnologie, 123(1), 2152. http://www.jstor.org/stable/25842543Google Scholar
Ingold, T. (2001). Beyond art and technology: The anthropology of skill. In Schiffer, M. B. (Ed.), Anthropological perspectives on technology (pp. 1731). University of New Mexico Press.Google Scholar
Iriki, A., & Sakura, O. (2008). The neuroscience of primate intellectual evolution: Natural selection and passive and intentional niche construction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1500), 2229–41. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.2274Google Scholar
Isler, K., & van Schaik, C. P. (2012). How our ancestors broke through the gray ceiling: Comparative evidence for cooperative breeding in early Homo. Current Anthropology, 53(S6), S453S465. https://doi.org/10.1086/667623Google Scholar
Isler, K., & van Schaik, C. P. (2014). How humans evolved large brains: Comparative evidence. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 23(2), 6575. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.21403Google Scholar
Kaplan, H., Gurven, M., Winking, J., Hooper, P. L., & Stieglitz, J. (2010). Learning, menopause, and the human adaptive complex. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1204(1), 3042. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2010.05528.xGoogle Scholar
Kaplan, H., Hill, K., Lancaster, J., & Hurtado, A. M. (2000). A theory of human life history evolution: Diet, intelligence, and longevity. Evolutionary Anthropology: Issues, News, and Reviews, 9(4), 156–85. https://doi.org/10.1002/1520-6505(2000)9:4<156::AID-EVAN5>3.0.CO;2-73.0.CO;2-7>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kivell, T. L. (2015). Evidence in hand: Recent discoveries and the early evolution of human manual manipulation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 370(1682), 20150105. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0105CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kline, M. A. (2015). How to learn about teaching: An evolutionary framework for the study of teaching behavior in humans and other animals. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 38, e31. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X14000090Google Scholar
Kolodny, O., Creanza, N., & Feldman, M. W. (2016). Game-changing innovations: How culture can change the parameters of its own evolution and induce abrupt cultural shifts. PLoS Computational Biology, 12(12), e1005302. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005302Google Scholar
Kramer, K. L. (2010). Cooperative breeding and its significance to the demographic success of humans. Annual Review of Anthropology, 39, 417–36. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.anthro.012809.105054Google Scholar
Kuhn, S. L. (2014). Signaling theory and technologies of communication in the Paleolithic. Biological Theory, 9(1), 4250. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13752–013-0156-5Google Scholar
Laland, K. N., Odling-Smee, J., Hoppitt, W., & Uller, T. (2013). More on how and why: Cause and effect in biology revisited. Biology & Philosophy, 28(5), 719–45. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10539–012-9335-1Google Scholar
Laland, K. N., Uller, T., Feldman, M. W., Sterelny, K., Müller, G. B., Moczek, A., Jablonka, E., & Odling-Smee, J. (2015). The extended evolutionary synthesis: Its structure, assumptions and predictions. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 282(1813), 20151019. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2015.1019Google Scholar
Lashley, K. (1951). The problem of serial order in behavior. In Jeffress, L. A. (Ed.), Cerebral mechanisms in behavior (pp. 112–36). John Wiley.Google Scholar
Lewontin, R. C. (1998). The evolution of cognition: Questions we will never answer. In Scarborough, D. & Sternberg, S. (Eds.), An invitation to cognitive science: Methods models, and conceptual issues (2nd ed., Vol. 4, pp. 107–32). MIT Press.Google Scholar
Magnani, M., Rezek, Z., Lin, S. C., Chan, A., & Dibble, H. L. (2014). Flake variation in relation to the application of force. Journal of Archaeological Science, 46, 3749. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jas.2014.02.029Google Scholar
Malafouris, L. (2013). How things shape the mind: A theory of material engagement. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Mantini, D., Corbetta, M., Romani, G. L., Orban, G. A., & Vanduffel, W. (2013). Evolutionarily novel functional networks in the human brain? Journal of Neuroscience, 33(8), 3259–75. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.4392-12.2013Google Scholar
Marx, L. (1997). “Technology”: The emergence of a hazardous concept. Social Research, 64(3), 965–88. http://www.jstor.org/stable/40971194Google Scholar
Marzke, M. W. (2013). Tool making, hand morphology and fossil hominins. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences 368(1630), 20120414. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0414Google Scholar
Mayr, E. (1961). Cause and effect in biology. Science, 134(3489), 1501–6. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.134.3489.1501Google Scholar
Mayr, E. (1994). Recapitulation reinterpreted: The somatic program. The Quarterly Review of Biology, 69(2), 223–32. https://doi.org/10.1086/418541CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mayr, E. (1997). The objects of selection. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 94(6), 2091–4. https://doi.org/10.2307/41593Google ScholarPubMed
Miller, G. A. (2003). The cognitive revolution: A historical perspective. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(3), 141–4. https://doi.org/10.1016/S1364–6613(03)00029-9Google Scholar
Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. (1960). Plans and the structure of behavior. Henry Holt. https://doi.org/10.1037/10039-000Google Scholar
Milner, A. D., & Goodale, M. A. (1995). The visual brain in action. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mueller, S., Wang, D., Fox, M. D., Yeo, B. T., Sepulcre, J., Sabuncu, M. R., Shafee, R., Liu, J., & Liu, H. (2013). Individual variability in functional connectivity architecture of the human brain. Neuron, 77(3), 586–95. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2012.12.028Google Scholar
Murren, C. J., Auld, J. R., Callahan, H., Ghalambor, C. K., Handelsman, C. A., Heskel, M. A., Kingsolver, J. G., Maclean, H. J., Masel, J., Maughan, H., Pfennig, D. W., Relyea, R. A., Seiter, S., Snell-Rood, E., Steiner, U. K., & Schlichting, C. D. (2015). Constraints on the evolution of phenotypic plasticity: Limits and costs of phenotype and plasticity. Heredity, 115(4), 293301. https://doi.org/10.1038/hdy.2015.8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Musgrave, S., Morgan, D., Lonsdorf, E., Mundry, R., & Sanz, C. (2016). Tool transfers are a form of teaching among chimpanzees. Scientific Reports, 6, 34783. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep34783Google Scholar
Nonaka, T., Bril, B., & Rein, R. (2010). How do stone knappers predict and control the outcome of flaking? Implications for understanding early stone tool technology. Journal of Human Evolution, 59(2), 155–67. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jhevol.2010.04.006Google Scholar
Obayashi, S., Suhara, T., Kawabe, K., Okauchi, T., Maeda, J., Akine, Y., Onoe, H., & Iriki, A. (2001). Functional brain mapping of monkey tool use. NeuroImage 14(4), 853–61. https://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2001.0878Google Scholar
Orban, G. A., & Caruana, F. (2014). The neural basis of human tool use. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 310. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00310Google Scholar
Oswalt, W. H. (1976). An anthropological analysis of food-getting technology. John Wiley and Sons.Google Scholar
Patterson, K., Nestor, P. J., & Rogers, T. T. (2007). Where do you know what you know? The representation of semantic knowledge in the human brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 8(12), 976–87. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2277Google Scholar
Perreault, C., Brantingham, P. J., Kuhn, S. L., Wurz, S., & Gao, X. (2013). Measuring the complexity of lithic technology. Current Anthropology, 54(S8), S397S406. https://doi.org/10.1086/673264Google Scholar
Pickering, M. J., & Garrod, S. (2013). An integrated theory of language production and comprehension. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(4), 329–47. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12001495Google Scholar
Pigliucci, M. (2009). An extended synthesis for evolutionary biology. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1168, 218–28. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2009.04578.xGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S., & Bloom, P. (1990). Natural language and natural selection. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 13(4), 707–27. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00081061Google Scholar
Powell, A., Shennan, S., & Thomas, M. G. (2009). Late Pleistocene demography and the appearance of modern human behavior. Science, 324(5932), 12981301. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1170165Google Scholar
Power, J. D., Fair, D. A., Schlaggar, B. L., & Petersen, S. E. (2010). The development of human functional brain networks. Neuron, 67(5), 735–48. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.08.017Google Scholar
Preuss, T. M. (2012). Human brain evolution: From gene discovery to phenotype discovery. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 109(Supplement 1), 10709–16. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1201894109Google Scholar
Pulvermüller, F., & Fadiga, L. (2010). Active perception: Sensorimotor circuits as a cortical basis for language. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(5), 351–60. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2811Google Scholar
Rakic, P. (2009). Evolution of the neocortex: A perspective from developmental biology. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(10), 724–35. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2719Google Scholar
Roepstorff, A., Niewöhner, J., & Beck, S. (2010). Enculturing brains through patterned practices. Neural Networks, 23(8–9), 1051–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neunet.2010.08.002Google Scholar
Roux, V., Bril, B., & Dietrich, G. (1995). Skills and learning difficulties involved in stone knapping: The case of stone-bead knapping in Khambhat, India. World Archaeology, 27(1), 6387. http://www.jstor.org/stable/124778Google Scholar
Saussure, F. (1966). Course in general linguistics (Baskin, W., Trans.; Bally, C. & Sechehaye, A., Eds.). McGraw-Hill. (Original work published 1916)Google Scholar
Schiffer, M. B. (1999). The material life of human beings: Artifacts, behavior and communication. Routledge.Google Scholar
Shannon, C. E., & Weaver, W. (1949). The mathematical theory of communication. University of Illinois Press.Google Scholar
Shennan, S. J., & Steele, J. (1999). Cultural learning in hominids: A behavioral ecological approach. In Box, H. O. & Gibson, K. R. (Eds.), Mammalian social learning: Comparative and ecological perspectives (pp. 367–88). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Smith, J. M. (2000). The concept of information in biology. Philosophy of Science, 67(2), 177–94. http://www.jstor.org/stable/188717Google Scholar
Somel, M., Liu, X., & Khaitovich, P. (2013). Human brain evolution: Transcripts, metabolites and their regulators. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14(2), 112–27. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3372Google Scholar
Stiner, M. C., Barkai, R., & Gopher, A. (2009). Cooperative hunting and meat sharing 400–200 kya at Qesem Cave, Israel. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(32), 13207–12. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0900564106Google Scholar
Stout, D. (2002). Skill and cognition in stone tool production: An ethnographic case study from Irian Jaya. Current Anthropology, 45(3), 693722. https://doi.org/10.1086/342638Google Scholar
Stout, D. (2011). Stone toolmaking and the evolution of human culture and cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1567), 1050–9. https://doi.org/10.1098%2Frstb.2010.0369Google Scholar
Stout, D. (2013). Neuroscience of technology. In Richerson, P. J. & Christiansen, M. H. (Eds.), Cultural evolution: Society, technology, language, and religion (pp. 157–73). MIT Press.Google Scholar
Stout, D. (2018). Human brain evolution: History or science? In Schwartz, J. H. (Ed.), Rethinking human evolution (pp. 297318). MIT Press.Google Scholar
Stout, D., & Chaminade, T. (2007). The evolutionary neuroscience of tool making. Neuropsychologia, 45(5), 10911100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.09.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stout, D., & Chaminade, T. (2012). Stone tools, language and the brain in human evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1585), 7587. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2011.0099Google Scholar
Stout, D., & Hecht, E. E. (2017). Evolutionary neuroscience of cumulative culture. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114(30), 7861–68. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1620738114Google Scholar
Stout, D., Hecht, E., Khreisheh, N., Bradley, B., & Chaminade, T. (2015). Cognitive demands of lower Paleolithic toolmaking. PLoS ONE, 10(4), e0121804. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0121804Google Scholar
Stout, D., & Khreisheh, N. (2015). Skill learning and human brain evolution: An experimental approach. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 25(4), 867–75. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0959774315000359Google Scholar
Stout, D., Passingham, R., Frith, C., Apel, J., & Chaminade, T. (2011). Technology, expertise and social cognition in human evolution. European Journal of Neuroscience, 33(7), 1328–38. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1460-9568.2011.07619.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stout, D., Rogers, M. J., Jaeggi, A. V., & Semaw, S. (2019). Archaeology and the origins of human cumulative culture: A case study from the earliest Oldowan at Gona, Ethiopia. Current Anthropology, 60(3), 309430. https://doi.org/10.1086/703173Google Scholar
Stout, D., Toth, N., Schick, K. D., & Chaminade, T. (2008). Neural correlates of Early Stone Age tool-making: Technology, language and cognition in human evolution. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 363(1499), 1939–49. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0001Google Scholar
Szathmáry, E., & Smith, J. M. (1995). The major evolutionary transitions. Nature, 374(6519), 227–32. https://doi.org/10.1038/374227a0Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (1999). The cultural origins of human cognition. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Toth, N., & Schick, K. (2009). The Oldowan: The tool making of early hominins and chimpanzees compared. Annual Review of Anthropology, 38, 289305. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-anthro-091908-164521Google Scholar
Turing, A. M. (1950). Computing machinery and intelligence. Mind, 59(236), 433–60. https://doi.org/10.1093/mind/LIX.236.433Google Scholar
Turner, V. (1975). Symbolic studies. Annual Review of Anthropology, 4, 145–61. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.an.04.100175.001045Google Scholar
Twomey, T. (2013). The cognitive implications of controlled fire use by early humans. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 23(1), 113–28. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0959774313000085Google Scholar
Tylor, E. B. (1871). Primitive culture: Researches into the development of mythology, philosophy, religion, art, and custom (Vol. 2). John Murray.Google Scholar
van Schaik, C. P., & Burkart, J. M. (2011). Social learning and evolution: The cultural intelligence hypothesis. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1567), 1008–16. https://doi.org/10.1098%2Frstb.2010.0304Google Scholar
van Schaik, C. P., Isler, K., & Burkart, J. M. (2012). Explaining brain size variation: From social to cultural brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(5), 277–84. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.04.004Google Scholar
Varela, F. G., Maturana, H. R., & Uribe, R. (1974). Autopoiesis: The organization of living systems, its characterization and a model. Biosystems, 5(4), 187–96. https://doi.org/10.1016/0303-2647(74)90031-8Google Scholar
Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D. P., Druks, J., Barber, H., & Cappa, S. F. (2011). Nouns and verbs in the brain: A review of behavioural, electrophysiological, neuropsychological and imaging studies. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(3), 407–26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.04.007Google Scholar
Wadley, L., Hodgskiss, T., & Grant, M. (2009). Implications for complex cognition from the hafting of tools with compound adhesives in the Middle Stone Age, South Africa. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(24), 9590–4. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0900957106Google Scholar
West‐Eberhard, M. J. (2005). Phenotypic accommodation: Adaptive innovation due to developmental plasticity. Journal of Experimental Zoology Part B: Molecular and Developmental Evolution, 304(6), 610–18. https://doi.org/10.1002/jez.b.21071Google Scholar
Whiten, A. (2015). Experimental studies illuminate the cultural transmission of percussive technologies in Homo and Pan. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 370(1682), 20140359. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2014.0359CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolpert, D. M., Doya, K., & Kawato, M. (2003). A unifying computational framework for motor control and social interaction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 358(1431), 593602. https://doi.org/10.1098%2Frstb.2002.1238Google Scholar
Wylie, A. (1985). The reaction against analogy. In Schiffer, M. B. (Ed.), Advances in archaeological method and theory (Vol. 8, pp. 63111). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978–0-12-003108-5.50008-7Google Scholar
Yopak, K. E., Lisney, T. J., Darlington, R. B., Collin, S. P., Montgomery, J. C., & Finlay, B. L. (2010). A conserved pattern of brain scaling from sharks to primates. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(29), 12946–51. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1002195107Google Scholar

References

Bakerman`s-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2006). Gene–environment interaction of the dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) and observed maternal insensitivity predicting externalizing behavior in preschoolers. Developmental Psychobiology, 48(5), 406–9. https://doi.org/10.1002/dev.20152Google Scholar
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2011). Differential susceptibility to rearing environment depending on dopamine-related genes: New evidence and a meta-analysis. Development and Psychopathology, 23(1), 3952. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579410000635Google Scholar
Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., van IJzendoorn, M. H., Pijlman, F. T. A., Mesman, J., & Juffer, F. (2008). Experimental evidence for differential susceptibility: Dopamine D4 receptor polymorphism (DRD4 VNTR) moderates intervention effects on toddlers’ externalizing behavior in a randomized controlled trial. Developmental Psychology, 44(1), 293300. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.44.1.293Google Scholar
Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2009). Beyond diathesis stress: Differential susceptibility to environmental influences. Psychological Bulletin, 135(6), 885908. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0017376Google Scholar
Chee, M. W. L., Zheng, H., Goh, J. O. S., Park, D., & Sutton, B. P. (2011). Brain structure in young and old East Asians and Westerners: Comparisons of structural volume and cortical thickness. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23(5), 1065–79. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2010.21513Google Scholar
Choi, I., Nisbett, R. E., & Norenzayan, A. (1999). Causal attribution across cultures: Variation and universality. Psychological Bulletin, 125(1), 4763. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.125.1.47Google Scholar
Damasio, A. (2018). The strange order of things: Life, feeling, and the making of cultures. Pantheon Books.Google Scholar
de Oliveira, S., & Nisbett, R. E. (2017). Beyond East and West: Cognitive style in Latin America. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 48(10), 1554–77. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022117730816Google Scholar
Draganski, B., Gaser, C., Busch, V., Schuierer, G., Bogdahn, U., & May, A. (2004). Neuroplasticity: Changes in grey matter induced by training. Nature, 427(6972), 311–12. https://doi.org/10.1038/427311aGoogle Scholar
Duncan, L. E., & Keller, M. C. (2011). A critical review of the first 10 years of candidate gene-by-environment interaction research in psychiatry. The American Journal of Psychiatry, 168(10), 1041–9. https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11020191Google Scholar
Fellows, L. K. (2011). Orbitofrontal contributions to value-based decision making: Evidence from humans with frontal lobe damage. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1239(1), 51–8. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06229.xGoogle Scholar
Gardner, W. L., Gabriel, S., & Lee, A. Y. (1999). “I” value freedom, but ‘we’ value relationships: Self-construal priming mirrors cultural differences in judgment. Psychological Science, 10(4), 321–6. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00162Google Scholar
Gaser, C., & Schlaug, G. (2003). Brain structures differ between musicians and non-musicians. Journal of Neuroscience, 23(27), 9240–5. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-27-09240.2003Google Scholar
Gehring, W. J., Goss, B., Coles, M. G. H., Meyer, D. E., & Donchin, E. (1993). A neural system for error detection and compensation. Psychological Science, 4(6), 385–90. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.1993.tb00586.xGoogle Scholar
Gilbert, D. T., & Malone, P. S. (1995). The correspondence bias. Psychological Bulletin, 117(1), 2138. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.117.1.21Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. T., & Jones, E. E. (1986). Perceiver-induced constraint: Interpretations of self-generated reality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(2), 269–80. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.50.2.269Google Scholar
Goto, S. G., Ando, Y., Huang, C., Yee, A., & Lewis, R. S. (2010). Cultural differences in the visual processing of meaning: Detecting incongruities between background and foreground objects using the N400. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5(2–3), 242–53. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsp038Google Scholar
Goto, S. G., Yee, A., Lowenberg, K., & Lewis, R. S. (2013). Cultural differences in sensitivity to social context: Detecting affective incongruity using the N400. Social Neuroscience, 8(1), 6374. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2012.739202Google Scholar
Haber, S. (2011). Neuroanatomy of reward: A view from the ventral striatum. In Gottfried, J. A. (Ed.), Neurobiology of sensation and reward. (pp. 235–62). CRC Press. www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK92777/Google Scholar
Hajcak, G., Moser, J. S., Yeung, N., & Simons, R. F. (2005). On the ERN and the significance of errors. Psychophysiology, 42(2), 151–60. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8986.2005.00270.xGoogle Scholar
Han, S., Northoff, G., Vogeley, K., Wexler, B. E., Kitayama, S., & Varnum, M. E. W. (2013). A cultural neuroscience approach to the biosocial nature of the human brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 64(1), 335–59. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-071112-054629Google Scholar
Hedden, T., Ketay, S., Aron, A., Markus, H. R., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2008). Cultural influences on neural substrates of attentional control. Psychological Science, 19(1), 1217. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02038.xGoogle Scholar
Heine, S. J., Lehman, D. R., Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1999). Is there a universal need for positive self-regard? Psychological Review, 106(4), 766–94. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295x.106.4.766Google Scholar
Henrich, J. (2015). The secret of our success: How culture is driving human evolution, domesticating our species, and making us smarter. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture’s consequences: International differences in work-related values. SAGE.Google Scholar
Ji, L.-J., Zhang, Z., & Nisbett, R. E. (2004). Is it culture or is it language? Examination of language effects in cross-cultural research on categorization. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(1), 5765. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.87.1.57Google Scholar
Kim, H., & Markus, H. R. (1999). Deviance or uniqueness, harmony or conformity? A cultural analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(4), 785800. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.77.4.785Google Scholar
Kim, H. S., & Sasaki, J. Y. (2014). Cultural neuroscience: Biology of the mind in cultural contexts. Annual Review of Psychology, 65(1), 487514. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115040Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., Duffy, S., Kawamura, T., & Larsen, J. T. (2003). Perceiving an object and its context in different cultures: A cultural look at new look. Psychological Science, 14(3), 201–6. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.02432Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., Ishii, K., Imada, T., Takemura, K., & Ramaswamy, J. (2006). Voluntary settlement and the spirit of independence: Evidence from Japan’s ‘northern frontier.Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(3), 369–84. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.3.369Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., King, A., Yoon, C., Tompson, S., Huff, S., & Liberzon, I. (2014). The dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4) moderates cultural difference in independent versus interdependent social orientation. Psychological Science, 25(6), 1169–77. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614528338Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., & Kurokawa, M. (2000). Culture, emotion, and well-being: Good feelings in Japan and the United States. Cognition and Emotion, 14(1), 93124. https://doi.org/10.1080/026999300379003Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., Markus, H. R., Matsumoto, H., & Norasakkunkit, V. (1997). Individual and collective processes in the construction of the self: Self-enhancement in the United States and self-criticism in Japan. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72(6), 1245–67. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.72.6.1245Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., Mesquita, B., & Karasawa, M. (2006). Cultural affordances and emotional experience: Socially engaging and disengaging emotions in Japan and the United States. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(5), 890903. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.5.890Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., & Park, J. (2014). Error-related brain activity reveals self-centric motivation: Culture matters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(1), 6270. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031696.suppGoogle Scholar
Kitayama, S., Park, H., Sevincer, A. T., Karasawa, M., & Uskul, A. K. (2009). A cultural task analysis of implicit independence: Comparing North America, Western Europe, and East Asia. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(2), 236–55. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015999Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., & Salvador, C. E. (2017). Culture embrained: Going beyond the nature-nurture dichotomy. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(5), 841–54. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F1745691617707317Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., San Martín, Á., & Savani, K. (2019). Varieties of interdependence and the emergence of the modern West: Toward the globalizing of psychology. Unpublished manuscript, Department of Psychology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI.Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., Snibbe, A. C., Markus, H. R., & Suzuki, T. (2004). Is there any “free” choice? Self and dissonance in two cultures. Psychological Science, 15(8), 527–33. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00714.xGoogle Scholar
Kitayama, S., & Tompson, S. (2015). A biosocial model of affective decision making: Implications for dissonance, motivation, and culture. In Olson, J. M. & Zanna, M. P. (Eds.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 52, pp. 72137). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.aesp.2015.04.001Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., & Uskul, A. K. (2011). Culture, mind, and the brain: Current evidence and future directions. Annual Review of Psychology, 62(1), 419–49. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120709-145357Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., Varnum, M. E. W., & Salvador, C. M. (2019). Cultural neuroscience. In Cohen, D. & Kitayama, S. (Eds.), The handbook of cultural psychology (2nd ed., pp. 79118). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., Yanagisawa, K., Ito, A., Ueda, R., Uchida, Y., & Abe, N. (2017). Reduced orbitofrontal cortical volume is associated with interdependent self-construal. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114(30), 7969–74. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1704831114Google Scholar
Knutson, B., & Greer, S. M. (2008). Anticipatory affect: Neural correlates and consequences for choice. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 363(1511), 3771–86. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0155Google Scholar
Lövdén, M., Wenger, E., Mårtensson, J., Lindenberger, U., & Bäckman, L. (2013). Structural brain plasticity in adult learning and development. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(9, Part B), 2296–310. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.02.014Google Scholar
Maguire, E. A., Gadian, D. G., Johnsrude, I. S., Good, C. D., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R. S. J., & Frith, C. D. (2000). Navigation-related structural change in the hippocampi of taxi drivers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 97(8), 4398–403. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.070039597Google Scholar
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224–53. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224Google Scholar
Masuda, T., & Kitayama, S. (2004). Perceiver-induced constraint and attitude attribution in Japan and the US: A case for the cultural dependence of the correspondence bias. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40(3), 409–16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2003.08.004Google Scholar
Masuda, T., & Nisbett, R. E. (2001). Attending holistically versus analytically: Comparing the context sensitivity of Japanese and Americans. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 81(5), 922–34. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.81.5.922Google Scholar
Miller, J. G. (1984). Culture and the development of everyday social explanation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46(5), 961–78. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.46.5.961Google Scholar
Miller, J. G., Bersoff, D. M., & Harwood, R. L. (1990). Perceptions of social responsibilities in India and in the United States: Moral imperatives or personal decisions? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 58(1), 3347. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.58.1.33Google Scholar
Miyamoto, Y., & Kitayama, S. (2002). Cultural variation in correspondence bias: The critical role of attitude diagnosticity of socially constrained behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(5), 1239–48. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.83.5.1239Google Scholar
Morling, B., Kitayama, S., & Miyamoto, Y. (2002). Cultural practices emphasize influence in the United States and adjustment in Japan. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28(3), 311–23. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167202286003Google Scholar
Morris, M. W., & Peng, K. (1994). Culture and cause: American and Chinese attributions for social and physical events. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 67(6), 949–71. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.67.6.949Google Scholar
Murata, A., Park, J., Kovelman, I., Hu, X., & Kitayama, S. (2015). Culturally non-preferred cognitive tasks require compensatory attention: A functional near infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS) investigation. Culture and Brain, 3(1), 5367. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40167–015-0027-yGoogle Scholar
Na, J., Grossmann, I., Varnum, M. E. W., Kitayama, S., Gonzalez, R., & Nisbett, R. E. (2010). Cultural differences are not always reducible to individual differences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(14), 6192–7. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1001911107Google Scholar
Na, J., & Kitayama, S. (2011). Spontaneous trait inference is culture-specific: Behavioral and neural evidence. Psychological Science, 22(8), 1025–32. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611414727Google Scholar
Nikolova, Y. S., Ferrell, R. E., Manuck, S. B., & Hariri, A. R. (2011). Multilocus genetic profile for dopamine signaling predicts ventral striatum reactivity. Neuropsychopharmacology, 36(9), 1940–7. https://doi.org/10.1038/npp.2011.82Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E., & Cohen, D. (1996). New directions in social psychology. Culture of honor: The psychology of violence in the South. Westview Press.Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I., & Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought: Holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review, 108(2), 291310. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.291Google Scholar
Northoff, G., Heinzel, A., de Greck, M., Bermpohl, F., Dobrowolny, H., & Panksepp, J. (2006). Self-referential processing in our brain: A meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. NeuroImage, 31(1), 440–57. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.002Google Scholar
O’Doherty, J. P. (2011). Contributions of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex to goal-directed action selection. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1239(1), 118–29. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06290.xGoogle Scholar
Oyserman, D., & Lee, S. W. S. (2008). Does culture influence what and how we think? Effects of priming individualism and collectivism. Psychological Bulletin, 134(2), 311–42. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.311Google Scholar
Reich, D. (2018). Who we are and how we got here: Ancient DNA and the new science of the human past. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rhee, E., Uleman, J. S., Lee, H. K., & Roman, R. J. (1995). Spontaneous self-descriptions and ethnic identities in individualistic and collectivistic cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69(1), 142–52. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.1.142Google Scholar
Rolls, E. T., & Grabenhorst, F. (2008). The orbitofrontal cortex and beyond: From affect to decision-making. Progress in Neurobiology, 86(3), 216–44. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2008.09.001Google Scholar
Rosenzweig, M. R., Krech, D., Bennett, E. L., & Zolman, J. F. (1962). Variation in environmental complexity and brain measures. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 55(6), 1092–5. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0042758Google Scholar
Ross, L. (1977). The intuitive psychologist and his shortcomings: Distortions in the attribution process. In Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (pp. 173220). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065–2601(08)60357-3Google Scholar
Ruby, M. B., Falk, C. F., Heine, S. J., Villa, C., & Silberstein, O. (2012). Not all collectivisms are equal: Opposing preferences for ideal affect between East Asians and Mexicans. Emotion, 12(6), 1206–9. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029118Google Scholar
Rychlowska, M., Miyamoto, Y., Matsumoto, D., Hess, U., Gilboa-Schechtman, E., Kamble, S., Muluk, H., Masuda, T., & Niedenthal, P. M. (2015). Heterogeneity of long-history migration explains cultural differences in reports of emotional expressivity and the functions of smiles. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112(19), E2429E2436. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1413661112Google Scholar
Said, E. W. (1979 ). Orientalism. Vintage Books.Google Scholar
San Martín, A., Sinaceur, M., Madi, A., Tompson, S., Maddux, W. W., & Kitayama, S. (2018). Self-assertive interdependence in Arab culture. Nature Human Behaviour, 2, 830–7. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562–018-0435-zGoogle Scholar
Sanchez-Burks, J., Nisbett, R. E., & Ybarra, O. (2000). Cultural styles, relational schemas, and prejudice against out-groups. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(2), 174–89. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.79.2.174Google Scholar
Savani, K., Markus, H. R., & Conner, A. L. (2008). Let your preference be your guide? Preferences and choices are more tightly linked for North Americans than for Indians. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 95(4), 861–76. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0011618Google Scholar
Schultz, W. (2002). Getting formal with dopamine and reward. Neuron, 36(2), 241–63. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0896–6273(02)00967-4Google Scholar
Schwartz, S. (2006). A theory of cultural value orientations: Explication and applications. Comparative Sociology, 5(2–3), 137–82. https://doi.org/10.1163/156913306778667357Google Scholar
Sen, A. (2005). The argumentative Indian: Writings on Indian history, culture and identity. Farrar, Straus and Giroux.Google Scholar
Sheese, B. E., Voelker, P. M., Rothbart, M. K., & Posner, M. P. (2007). Parenting quality interacts with genetic variation in dopamine receptor D4 to influence temperament in early childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 19(4), 1039–46. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0954579407000521Google Scholar
Shweder, R. A. (1991). Thinking through cultures: Expeditions in cultural psychology. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Shweder, R. A., & Bourne, E. J. (1982). Does the concept of the person vary cross-culturally? In Marsella, A. J. & White, G. M. (Eds.), Cultural conceptions of mental health and therapy culture, illness, and healing (pp. 97137). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-010-9220-3_4Google Scholar
Sui, J., Rotshtein, P., & Humphreys, G. W. (2013). Coupling social attention to the self forms a network for personal significance. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110(19), 7607–12. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1221862110Google Scholar
Talhelm, T., Zhang, X., Oishi, S., Shimin, C., Duan, D., Lan, X., & Kitayama, S. (2014). Large-scale psychological differences within China explained by rice versus wheat agriculture. Science, 344(6184), 603–8. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1246850Google Scholar
Telzer, E. H., & Fuligni, A. J. (2009). Daily family assistance and the psychological well-being of adolescents from Latin American, Asian, and European backgrounds. Developmental Psychology, 45(4), 1177–89. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014728Google Scholar
Telzer, E. H., Masten, C. L., Berkman, E. T., Lieberman, M. D., & Fuligni, A. J. (2010). Gaining while giving: An fMRI study of the rewards of family assistance among White and Latino youth. Social Neuroscience, 5(5–6), 508–18. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470911003687913Google Scholar
Tompson, S. H., Huff, S. T., Yoon, C., King, A., Liberzon, I., & Kitayama, S. (2018). The dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4) modulates cultural variation in emotional experience. Culture and Brain, 6(2), 118–29. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40167–018-0063-5Google Scholar
Triandis, H. C. (1995). New directions in social psychology. Individualism & collectivism. Westview Press. https://psycnet.apa.org/psycinfo/1995-97791-000Google Scholar
Triandis, H. C., Marn, G., Lisansky, J., & Betancourt, H. (1984). Simpatía as a cultural script of Hispanics. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 47(6), 1363–75. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.47.6.1363Google Scholar
Uchida, Y., & Kitayama, S. (2009). Happiness and unhappiness in east and west: Themes and variations. Emotion, 9(4), 441–56. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015634Google Scholar
Uchida, Y., Takemura, K., Fukushima, S., Saizen, I., Kawamura, Y., Hitokoto, H., Koizumi, N., & Yoshikawa, S. (2019). Farming cultivates a community-level shared culture through collective activities: Examining contextual effects with multilevel analyses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 116(1), 114. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000138Google Scholar
Uleman, J. S., Saribay, S. A., & Gonzalez, C. M. (2008). Spontaneous inferences, implicit impressions, and implicit theories. Annual Review of Psychology, 59(1), 329–60. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093707Google Scholar
van IJzendoorn, M. H., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Belsky, J., Beach, S., Brody, G., Dodge, K. A., Greenberg, J., Posner, M., & Scott, S. (2011). Gene-by-environment experiments: A new approach to finding the missing heritability. Nature Reviews Genetics, 12(2), 881. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrg2764-c1Google Scholar
van IJzendoorn, M. H., Caspers, K., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Beach, S. R. H., & Philibert, R. (2010). Methylation matters: Interaction between methylation density and serotonin transporter genotype predicts unresolved loss or trauma. Biological Psychiatry, 68(5), 405–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2010.05.008Google Scholar
Varnum, M. E. W., Shi, Z., Chen, A., Qiu, J., & Han, S. (2014). When “Your” reward is the same as “My” reward: Self-construal priming shifts neural responses to own vs. friends’ rewards. NeuroImage, 87, 164–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.10.042CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, F., Peng, K., Chechlacz, M., Humphreys, G. W., & Sui, J. (2017). The neural basis of independence versus interdependence orientations. Psychological Science, 28(4), 519–29. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616689079Google Scholar
Weeland, J., Overbeek, G., de Castro, B. O., & Matthys, W. (2015). Underlying mechanisms of gene-environment interactions in externalizing behavior: A systematic review and search for theoretical mechanisms. Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 18(4), 413–42. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10567–015-0196-4Google Scholar
Wood, A., Rychlowska, M., & Niedenthal, P. M. (2016). Heterogeneity of long-history migration predicts emotion recognition accuracy. Emotion, 16(4), 413–20. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000137Google Scholar
Woollett, K., & Maguire, E. A. (2011). Acquiring “the knowledge” of London’s layout drives structural brain changes. Current Biology, 21(24), 2109–14. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2011.11.018Google Scholar
Yu, Q., Abe, N., King, A., Yoon, C., Liberzon, I., & Kitayama, S. (2019). Cultural variation in the gray matter volume of the prefrontal cortex is moderated by the dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4). Cerebral Cortex, 29(9), 3922–31. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhy271Google Scholar
Zárate, M. A., Uleman, J. S., & Voils, C. I. (2001). Effects of culture and processing goals on the activation and binding of trait conceptsSocial Cognition, 19(3), 295323. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.19.3.295.21469Google Scholar
Zhu, X., Zhang, H., Wu, L., Yang, S., Wu, H., Luo, W., Gu, R., & Luo, Y. (2018). The influence of self-construals on the ERP response to the rewards for self and mother. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 18(2), 366–74. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415–018-0575-7Google Scholar

References

Adair, L. S., Fall, C. H., Osmond, C., Stein, A. D., Martorell, R., Ramirez-Zea, M., Sachdev, H. S., Dahly, D. L., Bas, I., Norris, S. A., Micklesfield, L., Hallal, P., & Victora, C. G., for the COHORTS group. (2013). Associations of linear growth and relative weight gain during early life with adult health and human capital in countries of low and middle income: Findings from five birth cohort studies. Lancet, 382(9891), 525–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140–6736(13)60103-8Google Scholar
Allis, C. D., & Jenuwein, T. (2016). The molecular hallmarks of epigenetic control. Nature Reviews Genetics, 17(8), 487500. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrg.2016.59Google Scholar
Arnett, J. J. (2008). The neglected 95%: Why American psychology needs to become less American. American Psychologist, 63(7), 602–14. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066x.63.7.602Google Scholar
Bachrach, C. A., & Abeles, R. P. (2004). Social science and health research: Growth at the National Institutes of Health. American Journal of Public Health, 94(1), 22–8. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.94.1.22Google Scholar
Balcerak, A., Trebinska-Stryjewska, A., Konopinski, R., Wakula, M., & Grzybowska, E. A. (2019). RNA-protein interactions: Disorder, moonlighting and junk contribute to eukaryotic complexity. Open Biology, 9(6), 190096. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsob.190096Google Scholar
Baldwin, J. M. (1896). A new factor in evolution. American Naturalist, 30(354–355), 441–51, 536–53. https://doi.org/10.1086/276428Google Scholar
Benedict, R. (1934). Patterns of culture. Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Bernard, H. R. (2013). Social research methods: Qualitative and quantitative approaches (2nd ed.). SAGE.Google Scholar
Betancourt, T. S., Borisova, I., Williams, T. P., Meyers-Ohki, S. E., Rubin-Smith, J. E., Annan, J., & Kohrt, B. A. (2013). Psychosocial adjustment and mental health in former child soldiers: A systematic review of the literature and recommendations for future research. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 54(1), 1736. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2012.02620.xGoogle Scholar
Bhutta, Z. A., Das, J. K., Rizvi, A., Gaffey, M. F., Walker, N., Horton, S., Webb, P., Lartey, A., Black, R. E., The Lancet Nutrition Interventions Review Group, & the Maternal and Child Nutrition Study Group. (2013). Evidence-based interventions for improvement of maternal and child nutrition: What can be done and at what cost? Lancet, 382(9890), 452–77. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140–6736(13)60996-4Google Scholar
Black, R. E., Victora, C. G., Walker, S. P., Bhutta, Z. A., Christian, P., de Onis, M., Ezzati, M., Grantham-McGregor, S., Katz, J., Martorell, R., Uauy, R., & the Maternal and Child Nutrition Study Group. (2013). Maternal and child undernutrition and overweight in low-income and middle-income countries. Lancet, 382(9890), 427–51. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140–6736(13)60937-XGoogle Scholar
Blurton-Jones, N. (1986). Bushman birth spacing: A test for optimal interbirth intervals. Ethology and Sociobiology, 7(2), 91105. https://doi.org/10.1016/0162-3095(86)90002-6Google Scholar
Boas, F. (1896). The limitations of the comparative method of anthropology. Science, 4(3), 901908. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.4.103.901Google Scholar
Boas, F. (1911). The mind of primitive man. Macmillan.Google Scholar
Boas, F. (1912). Changes in the bodily form of descendants of immigrants. American Anthropologist, 14(3), 530–62. https://doi.org/10.1525/aa.1912.14.3.02a00080Google Scholar
Bongaarts, J. (2009). Human population growth and the demographic transition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1532), 2985–90. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0137Google Scholar
Bonner, J. T. (1974). On development: The biology of form. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
British Association for the Advancement of Science. (1874). Notes and queries on anthropology: For the use of travellers and residents in uncivilized lands. Edward Stanford.Google Scholar
Bulatao, R. A., & Lee, R. D. (Eds.). (1983). Determinants of fertility in developing countries (Vols. 1–2). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Cavalli, G., & Heard, E. (2019). Advances in epigenetics link genetics to the environment and disease. Nature, 571(7766), 489–99. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41586–019-1411-0Google Scholar
Champagne, F. A., & Curley, J. P. (2009). Epigenetic mechanisms mediating the long-term effects of maternal care on development. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 33(4), 593600. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2007.10.009Google Scholar
Changeux, J.-P., & Chavaillon, J. (Eds.). (1995). Symposia of the Fyssen Foundation. Origins of the human brain. Clarendon Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780198523901.001.0001Google Scholar
Charnov, E. L. (1993). Life history invariants: Some explorations of symmetry in evolutionary ecology. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Clifford, J., & Marcus, G. E. (1986). Writing culture: The poetics and politics of ethnography. University of California Press.Google Scholar
Cole, S. W. (2014). Human social genomics. PLoS Genetics, 10(8), e1004601. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pgen.1004601Google Scholar
Cooper, P. J., Vally, Z., Cooper, H., Radford, T., Sharples, A., Tomlinson, M., & Murray, L. (2014). Promoting mother-infant book sharing and infant attention and language development in an impoverished South African population: A pilot study. Early Childhood Education Journal, 42(2), 143–52. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10643–013-0591-8Google Scholar
Creswell, J. W. (2014). Research design: Qualitative, quantitative, and mixed methods approaches (4th ed.). SAGE.Google Scholar
Dahl, R. E. (2016). The developmental neuroscience of adolescence: Revisiting, refining, and extending seminal models. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 101102. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2015.12.016Google Scholar
Dahl, R. E., Allen, N. B., Wilbrecht, L., & Suleiman, A. B. (2018). Importance of investing in adolescence from a developmental science perspective. Nature, 554(7693), 441–50. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature25770Google Scholar
Damasio, A. (2009). The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness. Harcourt.Google Scholar
Damasio, A., & Carvalho, G. B. (2013). The nature of feelings: Evolutionary and neurobiological origins. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 14(2), 143–52. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3403Google Scholar
de Oliveira, S., & Nisbett, R. E. (2017). Culture changes how we think about thinking: From ‘human inference’ to ‘geography of thought.’ Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(5), 782–90. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617702718Google Scholar
de Onis, M., Dewey, K. G., Borghi, E., Onyango, A. W., Blössner, M., Daelmans, B., Piwoz, E., & Branca, F. (2013). The World Health Organization’s global target for reducing childhood stunting by 2025: Rationale and proposed actions. Maternal & Child Nutrition, 9(S2), 626. https://doi.org/10.1111/mcn.12075Google Scholar
Dehaene, S. (2009). Reading in the brain: The new science of how we read. Penguin Books.Google Scholar
Del Giudice, M., Hinnant, J. B., Ellis, B. J., & El-Sheikh, M. (2012). Adaptive patterns of stress responsivity: A preliminary investigation. Developmental Psychology, 48(3), 775–90. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0026519Google Scholar
Dethlefsen, L., McFall-Ngai, M., & Relman, D. A. (2007). An ecological and evolutionary perspective on human-microbe mutualism and disease. Nature, 449(7164), 811–18. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature06245Google Scholar
Dodge, K. A., Goodman, W. B., Murphy, R., O’Donnell, K., & Sato, J. (2013). Toward population impact from home visiting. Zero to Three, 33(3), 1723. www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC3606025/Google Scholar
Ellis, B. J., & Del Giudice, M. (2019). Developmental adaptation to stress: An evolutionary perspective. Annual Review of Psychology, 70, 111–39. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-122216-011732CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ellison, P. T. (1994). Advances in human reproductive ecology. Annual Review of Anthropology, 23, 255–75. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.an.23.100194.001351Google Scholar
Fox, S. E., Levitt, P., & Nelson, C. A. III. (2010). How the timing and quality of early experiences influence the development of brain architecture. Child Development, 81(1), 2840. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01380.xGoogle Scholar
Friederici, A. D., Chomsky, N., Berwick, R. C., Moro, A., & Bolhuis, J. J. (2017). Language, mind and brain. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(10), 713–22. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562–017-0184-4Google Scholar
Garson, J. G., & Read, C. H. (1892). Notes and queries on anthropology (2nd ed.). Harrison and Sons.Google Scholar
Geertz, C. (1973). The interpretation of cultures: Selected essays. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gluckman, P. D., & Hanson, M. A. (Eds.). (2006). Developmental origins of health and disease. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511544699Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. (1977). Ontogeny and phylogeny. Belknap Press.Google Scholar
Grantham-McGregor, S., Cheung, Y. B., Cueto, S., Glewwe, P., Richter, L., & Strupp, B. (2007). Developmental potential in the first 5 years for children in developing countries. Lancet, 369(9555), 6070. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140–6736(07)60032-4Google Scholar
Gravlee, C. C., Bernard, H. R., & Leonard, W. R. (2003). Heredity, environment, and cranial form: A reanalysis of Boas’s immigrant data. American Anthropologist, 105(1), 125–38. https://doi.org/10.1525/aa.2003.105.1.125Google Scholar
Greenhalgh, S. (1996). The social construction of population science: An intellectual, institutional, and political history of twentieth-century demography. Comparative Studies in Society and History, 38(1), 2666. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0010417500020119Google Scholar
Gunnar, M. R., Doom, J. R., & Esposito, E. A. (2015). Psychoneuroendocrinology of stress: Normative development and individual differences. In Lamb, M. E. & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology and developmental science: Socioemotional processes (7th ed.,Vol. 3, pp. 106–51). John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Gurven, M., Fuerstenberg, E., Trumble, B., Stieglitz, J., Beheim, B., Davis, H., & Kaplan, H. (2017). Cognitive performance across the life course of Bolivian forager-farmers with limited schooling. Developmental Psychology, 53(1), 160–76. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000175Google Scholar
Hacking, I. (1999). The social construction of what? Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Hanson, M. A., & Gluckman, P. D. (2014). Early developmental conditioning of later health and disease: Physiology or pathophysiology? Physiological Reviews, 94(4), 1027–76. https://doi.org/10.1152/physrev.00029.2013Google Scholar
Harkness, S., Super, C. M., & Mavridis, C. J. (2011). Parental ethnotheories about children’s socioemotional development. In Chen, X. & Rubin, K. H. (Eds.), Socioemotional development in cultural context (pp. 7398). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Harkness, S., Super, C. M., Mavridis, C. J., Barry, O., & Zeitlin, M. (2013). Culture and early childhood development: Implications for policy and programs. In Britto, P. R., Engle, P. L., & Super, C. M. (Eds.), Handbook of early childhood development research and its impact on global policy (pp. 142–60). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199922994.003.0007Google Scholar
Harkness, S., Super, C. M., Moscardino, U., Rha, J.-H., Blom, M., Huitrón, B., Johnston, C., Sutherland, M. A., Hyun, O.-K., Axia, G., & Palacios, J. (2007). Cultural models and developmental agendas: Implications for arousal and self-regulation in early infancy. Journal of Developmental Processes, 1(2), 539.Google Scholar
Hay, M. C. (Ed.) (2016). Methods that matter: Integrating mixed methods for more effective social science research. University of Chicago Press. https://doi.org/10.7208/chicago/9780226328836.001.0001Google Scholar
Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Bowles, S., Camerer, C., Fehr, E., & Gintis, H. (Eds.). (2004). Foundations of human sociality: Economic experiments and ethnographic evidence from fifteen small-scale societies. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/0199262055.001.0001Google Scholar
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 6183. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0999152XGoogle Scholar
Hill, K., & Kaplan, H. (1999). Life history traits in humans: Theory and empirical studies. Annual Review of Anthropology, 28, 397430. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.anthro.28.1.397Google Scholar
Hobcraft, J., McDonald, J. W., & Rutstein, S. (1983). Child-spacing effects on infant and early child mortality. Population Index, 49(4), 585618. https://doi.org/10.2307/2737284Google Scholar
Howell, N. (1979). Demography of the Dobe !Kung. Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
Howie, P. W., & McNeilly, A. S. (1982). Effect of breast-feeding patterns on human birth intervals. Journal of Reproduction and Fertility, 65(2), 545–57. https://doi.org/10.1530/jrf.0.0650545Google Scholar
Irwin, M. R., & Cole, S. W. (2011). Reciprocal regulation of the neural and innate immune systems. Nature Reviews Immunology, 11(9), 625–32. https://doi.org/10.1038/nri3042Google Scholar
Jablonka, E. (2013). Epigenetic inheritance and plasticity: The responsive germline. Progress in Biophysics and Molecular Biology, 111(2–3), 99107. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pbiomolbio.2012.08.014Google Scholar
Jablonka, E. (2017). The evolutionary implications of epigenetic inheritance. Interface Focus, 7(5), 20160135. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsfs.2016.0135Google Scholar
Jaeggi, A. V., Hooper, P. L., Beheim, B. A., Kaplan, H., & Gurven, M. (2016). Reciprocal exchange patterned by market forces helps explain cooperation in a small-scale society. Current Biology, 26(16), 2180–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.06.019Google Scholar
Jordans, M. J. D., Komproe, I. H., Tol, W. A., Kohrt, B. A., Luitel, N. P., Macy, R. D., & de Jong, J. T. V. M.(2010). Evaluation of a classroom-based psychosocial intervention in conflict-affected Nepal: A cluster randomized controlled trial. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 51(7), 818–26. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7610.2010.02209.xGoogle Scholar
Karki, R., Kohrt, B. A., & Jordans, M. J. D. (2009). Child led indicators: Pilot testing a child participation tool for psychosocial support programmes for former child soldiers in Nepal. Intervention, 7(2), 92109. https://doi.org/10.1097/WTF.0b013e3283302725Google Scholar
Kitayama, S. (2017). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology: Attitudes and social cognition [Editorial]. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 112(3), 357–60. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspa0000077Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., & Markus, H. R. (Eds.). (1994). Emotion and culture: Empirical studies of mutual influence. American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., & Park, J. (2017). Emotion and biological health: The socio-cultural moderation. Current Opinion in Psychology, 17, 99105. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.06.016Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., Varnum, M. E. W., & Salvador, C. E. (2019). Cultural neuroscience. In Cohen, D. & Kitayama, S. (Eds.), Handbook of cultural psychology (2nd ed., pp. 79118). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Kohrt, B. (2013). Social ecology interventions for post-traumatic stress disorder: What can we learn from child soldiers? British Journal of Psychiatry, 203(3), 165–7. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.112.124958Google Scholar
Kohrt, B. A., Jordans, M. J. D., Tol, W. A., Perera, E., Karki, R., Koirala, S., & Upadhaya, N. (2010). Social ecology of child soldiers: Child, family, and community determinants of mental health, psychosocial well-being, and reintegration in Nepal. Transcultural Psychiatry, 47(5), 727–53. https://doi.org/10.1177/1363461510381290Google Scholar
Kohrt, B. A., Jordans, M. J. D., Tol, W. A., Speckman, R. A., Maharjan, S. M., Worthman, C. M., & Komproe, I. H. (2008). Comparison of mental health between former child soldiers and children never conscripted by armed groups in Nepal. [Erratum appears in JAMA (2010), 303(20), 2034]. JAMA, 300(6), 691702.Google Scholar
Kohrt, B. A., Perera, E., Jordans, M. J. D., Koirala, S., Karki, R., Karki, R., Shrestha, P., Tol, W. A., & Upadhaya, N. (2010). Psychosocial support model for children associated with armed forces and armed groups in Nepal. Transcultural Psychosocial Organization-Nepal/UNICEF.Google Scholar
Kohrt, B. A., Worthman, C. M., Adhikari, R. P., Luitel, N. P., Arevalo, J. M. G., Ma, J., McCreath, H., Seeman, T. E., Crimmins, E. M., & Cole, S. W. (2016). Psychological resilience and the gene regulatory impact of posttraumatic stress in Nepali child soldiers. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113(29), 8156–61. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1601301113Google Scholar
Konner, M. J. (1972). Aspects of the developmental ethology of a foraging people. In Jones, N. B. (Ed.), Ethological studies of child behaviour (pp. 285304). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Konner, M. J. (1977). Infancy among the Kalahari Desert San. In Leiderman, P. H., Tulkin, S. R., & Rosenfeld, A. (Eds.), Culture and infancy: Variations in the human experience (pp. 287328). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Konner, M., & Worthman, C. (1980). Nursing frequency, gonadal function, and birth spacing among !Kung hunter-gatherers. Science, 207(4432), 788–91. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.7352291Google Scholar
Kuzawa, C. W., & Bragg, J. M. (2012). Plasticity in human life history strategy: Implications for contemporary human variation and the evolution of genus Homo. Current Anthropology, 53(S6), S369S382. https://doi.org/10.1086/667410Google Scholar
Kuzawa, C. W., & Thayer, Z. M. (2011). Timescales of human adaptation: The role of epigenetic processes. Epigenomics, 3(2), 221–34. https://doi.org/10.2217/epi.11.11Google Scholar
Laland, K. N., Uller, T., Fellman, M. W., Sterelny, K., Müller, G. B., Moczek, A., Jablonka, E., & Odling-Smee, J. (2015). The extended evolutionary synthesis: Its structure, assumptions and predictions. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 282(1813), 20151019. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2015.1019Google Scholar
le Roux, I. M., le Roux, K., Comulada, W. S., Greco, E. M., Desmond, K. A., Mbewu, N., & Rotheram-Borus, M. J. (2010). Home visits by neighborhood Mentor Mothers provide timely recovery from childhood malnutrition in South Africa: Results from a randomized controlled trial. Nutrition Journal, 9, 56. https://doi.org/10.1186/1475-2891-9-56Google Scholar
le Roux, I. M., Tomlinson, M., Harwood, J. M., O’Connor, M. J., Worthman, C. M., Mbewu, N., Stewart, J., Hartley, M., Swendeman, D., Comulada, W., Weiss, R., & Rotheram-Borus, M. J. (2013). Outcomes of home visits for pregnant mothers and their infants: A cluster randomized controlled trial. AIDS, 27(9), 1461–71. https://doi.org/10.1097/QAD.0b013e3283601b53Google Scholar
Lee, R. B. (1972). Population growth and the beginnings of sedentary life among the !Kung Bushmen. In Spooner, B. (Ed.), Population growth: Anthropological implications (pp. 329–42). MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lee, R. B., & DeVore, I. (Eds.). (1976). Kalahari hunter-gatherers: Studies of the !Kung San and their neighbors. Harvard University Press. https://doi.org/10.4159/harvard.9780674430600Google Scholar
Luitel, N. P., Jordans, M. J. D., Sapkota, R. P., Tol, W. A., Kohrt, B. A., Thapa, S. B., Komproe, I. H., & Sharma, B. (2013). Conflict and mental health: A cross-sectional epidemiological study in Nepal. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 48(2), 183–93. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00127–012-0539-0Google Scholar
Lupien, S. J., McEwen, B. S., Gunnar, M. R., & Heim, C. (2009). Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(6), 434–45. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2639Google Scholar
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224–53. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224Google Scholar
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (2010). Cultures and selves: A cycle of mutual constitution. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5(4), 420–30. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691610375557Google Scholar
Maternal Child Nutrition Study Group. (2013). Maternal and child nutrition: Building momentum for impact. Lancet, 382(9890), 372–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140–6736(13)60988-5Google Scholar
McDade, T. W., Ryan, C., Jones, M. J., MacIsaac, J. L., Morin, A. M., Meyer, J. M., Borja, J. B., Miller, G. E., Kobor, M. S., & Kuzawa, C. W. (2017). Social and physical environments early in development predict DNA methylation of inflammatory genes in young adulthood. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114(29), 7611–16. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1620661114Google Scholar
Mead, M. (1928). Coming of age in Samoa: A psychological study of primitive youth for Western civilization. William Morrow & Company.Google Scholar
Mead, M. (1930). Growing up in New Guinea: A comparative study of primitive education. William Morrow & Company.Google Scholar
Mead, M., & Macgregor, F. C. (1951). Growth and culture: A photographic study of Balinese childhood. Putnam.Google Scholar
Miller, A. H., & Raison, C. L. (2016). The role of inflammation in depression: From evolutionary imperative to modern treatment target. Nature Reviews Immunology, 16(1), 2234. https://doi.org/10.1038/nri.2015.5Google Scholar
Miska, E. A., & Ferguson-Smith, A. C. (2016). Transgenerational inheritance: Models and mechanisms of non-DNA sequence-based inheritance. Science, 354(6308), 5963. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aaf4945Google Scholar
Murray, D. R., Haselton, M. G., Fales, M., & Cole, S. W. (2019). Subjective social status and inflammatory gene expression. Health Psychology, 38(2), 182–6. https://doi.org/10.1037/hea0000705Google Scholar
National Institutes of Health. (1994). NIH guidelines on the inclusion of women and minorities as subjects in clinical research. http://grants.nih.gov/grants/guide/notice-files/not94–100.htmlGoogle Scholar
Newen, A., De Bruin, L., & Gallagher, S. (Eds.). (2018). The Oxford handbook of 4E cognition. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780198735410.001.0001Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E. (1993). Violence and U. S. regional culture. American Psychologist, 48(4), 441–9. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.48.4.441Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E., Peng, K., Choi, I., & Norenzayan, A. (2001). Culture and systems of thought: Holistic versus analytic cognition. Psychological Review, 108(2), 291310. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.108.2.291Google Scholar
Noel, G. L., Suh, H. K., & Frantz, A. G. (1974). Prolactin release during nursing and breast stimulation in postpartum and nonpostpartum subjects. Journal of Clinical Endocrinology and Metabolism, 38(3), 413–23. https://doi.org/10.1210/jcem-38-3-413Google Scholar
Nusslock, R., & Miller, G. E. (2016). Early-life adversity and physical and emotional health across the lifespan: A neuroimmune network hypothesis. Biological Psychiatry, 80(1), 2332. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.05.017Google Scholar
O’Neil, S., & van Broeckhoven, K. (Eds.). (2018). Cradled by conflict: Child involvement with armed groups in contemporary conflict. United Nations University.Google Scholar
Odling-Smee, F. J., Laland, K. N., & Feldman, M. W. (1996). Niche construction. American Naturalist, 147(4), 641–8. https://doi.org/10.1086/285870Google Scholar
Olds, D. L., Kitzman, H., Knudtson, M. D., Anson, E., Smith, J. A., & Cole, R. (2014). Effect of home visiting by nurses on maternal and child mortality: Results of a 2-decade follow-up of a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Pediatrics, 168(9), 800806. https://doi.org/10.1001/jamapediatrics.2014.472Google Scholar
Olds, D. L., Robinson, J., O’Brien, R., Luckey, D. W., Pettitt, L. M., Henderson, C. R. Jr., Ng, R. K., Sheff, K. L., Korfmacher, J., Hiatt, S., & Talmi, A. (2002). Home visiting by paraprofessionals and by nurses: A randomized, controlled trial. Pediatrics, 110(3), 486–96. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.110.3.486Google Scholar
Ortner, S. B. (1984). Theory in anthropology since the Sixties. Comparative Studies in Society and History, 26(1), 126–66. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0010417500010811Google Scholar
Ortner, S. B. (2016). Dark anthropology and its other: Theory since the eighties. HAU: Journal of Ethnographic Theory, 6(1), 4773. https://doi.org/10.14318/hau6.1.004Google Scholar
Park, J., Kitayama, S., Miyamoto, Y., & Coe, C. L. (2019). Feeling bad is not always unhealthy: Culture moderates the link between negative affect and diurnal cortisol profiles. Emotion. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1037/emo0000605Google Scholar
Paul, B. D. (Ed.) (1955). Health, culture, & community: Case studies of public reaction to health programs. Russell SAGE Foundation.Google Scholar
Paul, R. A. (2015). Mixed messages: Cultural and genetic inheritance in the constitution of human society. University of Chicago Press. https://doi.org/10.7208/chicago/9780226241050.001.0001Google Scholar
Pertea, M., Shumate, A., Pertea, G., Varabyou, A., Breitwieser, F. P., Chang, Y.-C., Madugundu, A. K., Pandey, A., & Salzberg, S. L. (2018). CHESS: A new human gene catalog curated from thousands of large-scale RNA sequencing experiments reveals extensive transcriptional noise. Genome Biology, 19(1), 208. https://doi.org/10.1186/s13059–018-1590-2Google Scholar
Petrosini, L., De Bartolo, P., Foti, F., Gelfo, F., Cutuli, D., Leggio, M. G., & Mandolesi, L. (2009). On whether the environmental enrichment may provide cognitive and brain reserves. Brain Research Reviews, 61(2), 221–39. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresrev.2009.07.002Google Scholar
Pocheville, A. (2015). The ecological niche: History and recent controversies. In Hearns, T., Huneman, P., Lecointre, G., & Silberstein, M. (Eds.), Handbook of evolutionary thinking in the sciences (pp. 547–86). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-017-9014-7_26Google Scholar
Pressman, S. D., Jenkins, B. N., & Moskowitz, J. T. (2019). Positive affect and health: What do we know and where next should we go? Annual Review of Psychology, 70, 627–50. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-102955Google Scholar
Ramstead, M. J. D., Veissière, S. P. L., & Kirmayer, L. J. (2016). Cultural affordances: Scaffolding local worlds through shared intentionality and regimes of attention. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1090. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01090Google Scholar
Reeve, H. K., & Sherman, P. W. (1993). Adaptation and the goals of evolutionary research. Quarterly Review of Biology, 68(1), 132. https://doi.org/10.1086/417909Google Scholar
Rollins, N. C., Bhandari, N., Hajeebhoy, N., Horton, S., Lutter, C. K., Martines, J. C., Piwoz, E. G., Richter, L. M., Victora, C. G., & The Lancet Breastfeeding Series Group. (2016). Why invest, and what it will take to improve breastfeeding practices? Lancet, 387(10017), 491504. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140–6736(15)01044-2Google Scholar
Rosaldo, R. (1989). Culture & truth: The remaking of social analysis. Beacon Press.Google Scholar
Rotheram-Borus, M. J., le Roux, I. M., Tomlinson, M., Mbewu, N., Comulada, W. S., le Roux, K., Stewart, J., O’Connor, M. J., Hartley, M., Desmond, K., Greco, E., Worthman, C. M., Idemundia, F., & Swendeman, D. (2011). Philani Plus (+): A mentor mother community health worker home visiting program to improve maternal and infants’ outcomes. Prevention Science, 12(4), 372–88. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121–011-0238-1Google Scholar
Ruben, R. J. (1997). A time frame of critical/sensitive periods of language development. Acta Oto-Laryngologica, 117(2), 202–5. https://doi.org/10.3109/00016489709117769Google Scholar
Save the Children. (2012). Nutrition in the first 1,000 days: State of the world’s mothers 2012. Save the Children.Google Scholar
Sawyer, S. M., Azzopardi, P. S., Wickremarathne, D., & Patton, G. C. (2018). The age of adolescence. The Lancet Child & Adolescent Health, 2(3), 223–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/S2352–4642(18)30022-1Google Scholar
Schneider, J., & Schneider, P. (1984). Demographic transition in a Sicilian rural town. Journal of Family History, 9(3), 245–72. https://doi.org/10.1177/036319908400900305Google Scholar
Shore, B. (1996). Culture in mind: Cognition, culture, and the problem of meaning. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Shostak, M. (1981). Nisa, the life and words of a !Kung woman. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Shweder, R. A. (1990). Ethical relativism: Is there a defensible version? Ethos, 18(2), 205–18. https://doi.org/10.1525/eth.1990.18.2.02a00050Google Scholar
Shweder, R. A., Minow, M., & Markus, H. (2002). Engaging cultural differences: The multicultural challenge in liberal democracies. Russell SAGE Foundation.Google Scholar
Simons, R. C., & Hughes, C. C. (1985). The culture-bound syndromes: Folk illnesses of psychiatric and anthropological interest. D. Reidel. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-94-009-5251-5Google Scholar
Stearns, S. C. (1992). The evolution of life histories. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Stevens, E. E., Patrick, T. E., & Pickler, R. (2009). A history of infant feeding. Journal of Perinatal Education, 18(2), 32–9. https://doi.org/10.1624/105812409X426314Google Scholar
Stevens, J. S., & Jovanovic, T. (2019). Role of social cognition in post-traumatic stress disorder: A review and meta-analysis. Genes, Brain, & Behavior, 18(1), e12518. https://doi.org/10.1111/gbb.12518Google Scholar
Stotz, K. (2014). Extended evolutionary psychology: The importance of transgenerational developmental plasticity. Frontiers in Psychology, 5. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00908Google Scholar
Strauss, C., & Quinn, N. (1997). A cognitive theory of cultural meaning. Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9781139167000Google Scholar
Super, C. M., Axia, G., Harkness, S., Welles-Nyström, B., Zylicz, P. O., Parmar, P., Bonichini, S., Bermúdez, M. R., Moscardino, U., Kolar, V., Palacios, J., Andrzej, E., & McGurk, H. (2008). Culture, temperament, and the “difficult child”: A study in seven Western cultures. European Journal of Developmental Science, 2(1–2), 136–57. https://doi.org/10.3233/DEV-2008-21209Google Scholar
Super, C. M., & Harkness, S. (1986). The developmental niche: A conceptualization at the interface of child and culture. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 9(4), 545–69. https://doi.org/10.1177/016502548600900409Google Scholar
The Women’s Health Initiative Study Group. (1998). Design of the Women’s Health Initiative clinical trial and observational study. Controlled Clinical Trials, 19, 61109. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0197–2456(97)00078-0Google Scholar
Theise, N. D., & Kafatos, M. C. (2013). Complementarity in biological systems: A complexity view. Complexity, 18(6), 1120. https://doi.org/10.1002/cplx.21453Google Scholar
Tomlinson, M., Rotheram-Borus, M. J., Harwood, J., le Roux, I. M., O’Connor, M., & Worthman, C. (2015). Community health workers can improve child growth of antenatally-depressed, South African mothers: A cluster randomized controlled trial. BMC Psychiatry, 15, 225. https://doi.org/10.1186/s12888–015-0606-7Google Scholar
Tomlinson, M., Rotheram-Borus, M. J., le Roux, I. M., Youssef, M., Nelson, S. H., Scheffler, A., Weiss, R. E., O’Connor, M., & Worthman, C. M. (2016). Thirty-six-month outcomes of a generalist paraprofessional perinatal home visiting intervention in South Africa on maternal health and child health and development. Prevention Science, 17(8), 937–48. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11121–016-0676-xGoogle Scholar
Trevathan, W. R., Smith, E. O., & McKenna, J. J. (2008). Evolutionary medicine and health: New perspectives. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Triandis, H. C. (1989). The self and social behavior in differing cultural contexts. Psychological Review, 96(3), 506–20. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.96.3.506Google Scholar
Tylor, E. B. (1871). Primitive culture: Researches into the development of mythology, philosophy, religion, art, and custom (Vol. 1). John Murray.Google Scholar
Tyson, J. E., Friesen, H. G., & Anderson, M. S. (1972). Human lactational and ovarian response to endogenous prolactin release. Science, 177(4052), 897900. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.177.4052.897Google Scholar
Uchida, Y., Kitayama, S., Akutsu, S., Park, J., & Cole, S. W. (2018). Optimism and the conserved transcriptional response to adversity. Health Psychology, 37(11), 1077–80. https://doi.org/10.1037/hea0000675Google Scholar
UNICEF. (2007). Paris principles: Principles and guidelines on children associated with armed forces and armed conflict. United Nations International Children’s Emergency Fund.Google Scholar
Victora, C. G., Bahl, R., Barros, A. J. D., França, G. V. A., Horton, S., Krasevec, J., Murch, S., Sankar, M. J., Walker, N., Rollins, N. C., & The Lancet Breastfeeding Series Group. (2016). Breastfeeding in the 21st century: Epidemiology, mechanisms, and lifelong effect. Lancet, 387(10017), 475–90. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140–6736(15)01024-7Google Scholar
Victora, C. G., de Onis, M., Hallal, P. C., Blössner, M., & Shrimpton, R. (2010). Worldwide timing of growth faltering: Revisiting implications for interventions. Pediatrics, 125(3), e473e480. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2009-1519Google Scholar
Vitzthum, V. J. (2009). The ecology and evolutionary endocrinology of reproduction in the human female. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 140(S49), 95136. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.21195Google Scholar
Walker, S. P., Wachs, T. D., Grantham-McGregor, S., Black, M. M., Nelson, C. A., Huffman, S. L., Baker-Henningham, H., Chang, S. M., Hamadani, J. D., Lozoff, B., Gardner, J. M. M., Powell, C. A., Rahman, A., & Richter, L. (2011). Inequality in early childhood: Risk and protective factors for early child development. Lancet, 378(9799), 1325–38. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140–6736(11)60555-2Google Scholar
Walker, S. P., Wachs, T. D., Gardner, J. M., Lozoff, B., Wasserman, G. A., Pollitt, E., & Carter, J. A. (2007). Child development: Risk factors for adverse outcomes in developing countries. Lancet, 369(9556), 145–57. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0140–6736(07)60076-2Google Scholar
West-Eberhard, M. J. (2003). Developmental plasticity and evolution. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Whiting, J. W. M. (1994). Culture and human development: The selected papers of John Whiting. Chasdi, E. H. (Ed.). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511598340Google Scholar
Whiting, B. B., & Edwards, C. P. (1988). Children of different worlds: The formation of social behavior. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Whiting, J. W. M., & Child, I. L. (1953). Child training and personality: A cross-cultural study. Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Whiting, J. W. M., Child, I. L., Lambert, W. W., & the Field Teams for the Six Cultures series. (1966). Field guide for a study of socialization. John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Worthman, C. M. (1978). Psychoneuroendocrine study of human behavior: Some interactions of steroid hormones with affect and behavior in the !Kung San [Unpublished doctoral dissertation]. Harvard University.Google Scholar
Worthman, C. M. (2010). The ecology of human development: Evolving models for cultural psychology. Journal for Cross-Cultural Psychology, 41(4), 546–62. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022110362627Google Scholar
Worthman, C. M. (2016). Ecocultural theory: Foundations and applications. In Hay, M. C. (Ed.), Methods that matter: Integrating mixed methods for more effective social science research (pp. 1338). University of Chicago Press. https://doi.org/10.7208/chicago/9780226328836.003.0002Google Scholar
Worthman, C. M., & Costello, E. J. (2009). Tracking biocultural pathways in population health: The value of biomarkers. Annals of Human Biology, 36(3), 281–97. https://doi.org/10.1080/03014460902832934Google Scholar
Worthman, C. M., Tomlinson, M., & Rotheram-Borus, M. J. (2016). When can parents most influence their child’s development? Expert knowledge and perceived local realities. Social Science & Medicine, 154, 62–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.02.040Google Scholar
Worthman, C. M., & Trang, K. (2018). Dynamics of body time, social time and life history at adolescence. Nature, 554(7693), 451–7. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature25750Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (2006). Descartes’ error. Random House.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. Macmillan.Google Scholar
Sporns, O. (2012). Discovering the human connectome. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Veissière, S. P. L., Constant, A., Ramstead, M. J. D., Friston, K. J., & Kirmayer, L. J. (2020). Thinking through other minds: A variational approach to cognition and culture. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 43, e90, 1–75. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X19001213Google Scholar

References

Anderson, M. L. (2014). After phrenology: Neural reuse and the interactive brain. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Balázsi, G., van Oudenaarden, A., & Collins, J. J. (2011). Cellular decision making and biological noise: From microbes to mammals. Cell, 144(6), 910–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cell.2011.01.030Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F., & Bar, M. (2009). See it with feeling: Affective predictions during object perceptionPhilosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences364(1521), 1325–34. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0312Google Scholar
Boroditsky, L. (2001). Does language shape thought? Mandarin and English speakers’ conceptions of time. Cognitive Psychology, 43(1), 122. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.2001.0748Google Scholar
Branquinho, J. (Ed.). (2001). The foundations of cognitive science. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Brogaard, B., & Chomanski, B. (2015). Cognitive penetrability and high-level properties in perception: Unrelated phenomena? Pacific Philosophical Quarterly, 96(4), 469–86. https://doi.org/10.1111/papq.12111Google Scholar
Brooks, R. A. (1991). Intelligence without representation. Artificial Intelligence, 47(1–3), 139–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/0004-3702(91)90053-MGoogle Scholar
Carruthers, P. (2006). The architecture of the mind: Massive modularity and the flexibility of thought. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199207077.001.0001Google Scholar
Carruthers, P. (2015). Perceiving mental states. Consciousness and Cognition, 36, 498507. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2015.04.009Google Scholar
Ceci, S. J., & Roazzi, A. (1994). The effects of context on cognition: Postcards from Brazil. In Sternberg, R. J. & Wagner, R. K. (Eds.), Mind in context: Interactionist perspectives on human intelligence (pp. 74101). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cibelli, E., Xu, Y., Austerweil, J. L., Griffiths, T. L., & Regier, T. (2016). The Sapir-Whorf hypothesis and probabilistic inference: Evidence from the domain of colorPLoS ONE11(7), e0158725. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0158725Google Scholar
Clark, A. (1997). Being there: Putting brain, body, and the world together again. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2015). Embodied prediction. In Metzinger, T. & Windt, J. M. (Eds.), Open mind: MIND group. MIND Group. https://doi.org/10.15502/9783958570115Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2016). Surfing uncertainty: Prediction, action and the embodied mind. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780190217013.001.0001Google Scholar
Costall, A. (1995). Socializing affordancesTheory & Psychology5(4), 467–81. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0959354395054001Google Scholar
Dedrick, D. (2015). Colour language, thought, and culture. In Sharifian, F. (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of language and culture (pp. 270–93). Routledge.Google Scholar
Delk, J. L., & Fillenbaum, S. (1965). Differences in perceived color as a function of characteristic colorThe American Journal of Psychology78(2), 290–3. https://doi.org/10.2307/1420503Google Scholar
Firestone, C., & Scholl, B. J. (2016). Cognition does not affect perception: Evaluating the evidence for “top-down” effectsBehavioral and Brain Sciences39, e229. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X15000965Google Scholar
Fodor, J. (2009). Where is my mind? London Review of Books31(3), 1315.Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind: An essay on faculty psychology. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Friston, K. (2013). Active inference and free energyBehavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(3), 212–13. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12002142Google Scholar
Friston, K. (2018). Does predictive coding have a future? Nature Neuroscience, 21(8), 1019–21. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593–018-0200-7Google Scholar
Friston, K., Thornton, C., & Clark, A. (2012). Free-energy minimization and the dark-room problemFrontiers in Psychology3(130), 17. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00130Google Scholar
Frith, C. D. (2017). Discovering the social mind: Selected works of Christopher D. Frith. Routledge.Google Scholar
Fuchs, T. (2017). Ecology of the brain: The phenomenology and biology of the embodied mind. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fulda, F. C. (2017). Natural agency: The case of bacterial cognition. Journal of the American Philosophical Association, 3(1), 6990. https://doi.org/10.1017/apa.2017.5Google Scholar
Gallagher, S. (2017). Enactivist interventions: Rethinking the mind. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780198794325.001.0001Google Scholar
Gallagher, S., & Bower, M. (2014). Making enactivism even more embodied. AVANT. Trends in Interdisciplinary Studies, 5(2), 232–47. https://doi.org/10.26913/50202014.0109.0011Google Scholar
Gallagher, S., Hutto, D. D., Slaby, J., & Cole, J. (2013). The brain as part of an enactive system. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(4), 421–2. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12002105Google Scholar
Gibson, J. J. (1977). The theory of affordances. In Shaw, R. & Bransford, J. (Eds.), Perceiving, acting, and knowing: Toward an ecological psychology (pp. 6782). Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Goldstone, R. L. (1994). Influences of categorization on perceptual discrimination. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123(2), 178200. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.123.2.178Google Scholar
Goldstone, R. L., Lippa, Y., & Shiffrin, R. M. (2001). Altering object representations through category learning. Cognition, 78(1), 2743. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010–0277(00)00099-8Google Scholar
Harnad, S. (Ed.). (1987 ). Categorical perception: The groundwork of cognition. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Heyes, C. (2012). New thinking: The evolution of human cognition. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367(1599), 2091–6. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2012.0111Google Scholar
Hohwy, J. (2013). The predictive mind. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199682737.001.0001Google Scholar
Hutto, D. D. (2005). Knowing what? Radical versus conservative enactivism. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 4(4), 389405. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097–005-9001-zGoogle Scholar
Hutto, D. D. (2008). Folk psychological narratives: The sociocultural basis of understanding reasons. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hutto, D. D., & Kirchhoff, M. D. (2015). Looking beyond the brain: Social neuroscience meets narrative practice. Cognitive Systems Research, 34 –35, 517. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogsys.2015.07.001Google Scholar
Hutto, D. D., & Myin, E. (2013). Radicalizing enactivism. Basic minds without content, MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262018548.001.0001Google Scholar
Hutto, D. D., & Myin, E. (2017). Evolving enactivism: Basic minds meet content. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hutto, D. D., & Satne, G. (2015). The natural origins of content. Philosophia, 43(3), 521–36. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11406–015-9644-0Google Scholar
Kay, P. (1999). ColorJournal of Linguistic Anthropology9(1–2), 32–5. https://doi.org/10.1525/jlin.1999.9.1-2.32Google Scholar
Kay, P., & Kempton, W. (1984). What is the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis? American Anthropologist, 86(1), 6579. https://doi.org/10.1525/aa.1984.86.1.02a00050Google Scholar
Locke, J. C. W. (2013). Systems biology: How bacteria choose a lifestyle. Nature, 503, 476–7. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature12837Google Scholar
Losick, R., & Desplan, C. (2008). Stochasticity and cell fate. Science, 320(5872), 65–8. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1147888Google Scholar
Macpherson, F. (2012). Cognitive penetration of colour experience: Rethinking the issue in light of an indirect mechanism. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 84(1), 2462. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1933-1592.2010.00481.xGoogle Scholar
Malafouris, L. (2013). How things shape the mind: A theory of material engagement. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Marchi, F., & Newen, A. (2015). Cognitive penetrability and emotion recognition in human facial expressions. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 828. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00828Google Scholar
Marr, D. (1982). Vision: A computational approach. Freeman. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262514620.001.0001Google Scholar
Newen, A., & Vetter, P. (2017). Why cognitive penetration of our perceptual experience is still the most plausible account. Consciousness and Cognition, 47, 2637. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2016.09.005Google Scholar
Newen, A., Marchi, F., & Brössel, P. (2017). Introduction – Cognitive penetration and predictive coding: Pushing the debate forward with the recent achievements of cognitive science. Consciousness and Cognition, 47, 15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2016.12.001Google Scholar
Noë, A. (2004). Action in perception. MIT press.Google Scholar
O’Callaghan, C., Kveraga, K., Shine, J. M., Adams, R. B. Jr., & Bar, M. (2017). Predictions penetrate perception: Converging insights from brain, behaviour and disorderConsciousness and Cognition47, 6374. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2016.05.003Google Scholar
O’Regan, J. K., & Noë, A. (2001). A sensorimotor account of vision and visual consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(5), 939–73. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X01000115Google Scholar
Overmann, K. A. (2016). Beyond writing: The development of literacy in the ancient Near East. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 26(2), 285303. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0959774316000019Google Scholar
Perkins, T. J., & Swain, P. S. (2009). Strategies for cellular decision-making. Molecular Systems Biology, 5(1), 326. https://doi.org/10.1038/msb.2009.83Google Scholar
Pylyshyn, Z. (1999). Is vision continuous with cognition?: The case for cognitive impenetrability of visual perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(3), 341–65. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X99002022Google Scholar
Raftopoulos, A. (2001). Is perception informationally encapsulated?: The issue of the theory-ladenness of perceptionCognitive Science25(3), 423–51. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog2503_4Google Scholar
Raftopoulos, A. (2015). The cognitive impenetrability of perception and theory-ladennessJournal for General Philosophy of Science46(1), 87103. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10838–015-9288-6Google Scholar
Raftopoulos, A. (2017). Pre-cueing, the epistemic role of early vision, and the cognitive impenetrability of early visionFrontiers in Psychology8, 1156. https://doi.org/10.3389%2Ffpsyg.2017.01156Google Scholar
Raftopoulos, A., & Lupyan, G. (2018). Pre-cueing effects on perception and cognitive penetrabilityFrontiers in Psychology9, 230. https://doi.org/10.3389%2Ffpsyg.2018.00230Google Scholar
Ramstead, M. J. D., Veissière, S. P. L., & Kirmayer, L. J. (2016). Cultural affordances: Scaffolding local worlds through shared intentionality and regimes of attentionFrontiers in Psychology7, 1090. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01090Google Scholar
Rauss, K., Schwartz, S., & Pourtois, G. (2011). Top-down effects on early visual processing in humans: A predictive coding frameworkNeuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews35(5), 1237–53. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2010.12.011Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., & Sinigaglia, C. (2008). Mirrors in the brain: How our minds share actions and emotions (Anderson, F., Trans.). Oxford University Press. (Original work published in 2006)Google Scholar
Roepstorff, A., Niewöhner, J., & Beck, S. (2010). Enculturing brains through patterned practices. Neural Networks, 23(8–9), 1051–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neunet.2010.08.002Google Scholar
Shuler, M. G., & Bear, M. F. (2006). Reward timing in the primary visual cortex. Science, 311(5767), 1606–9. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1123513Google Scholar
Siegel, S. (2012). Cognitive penetrability and perceptual justification. Noûs, 46(2), 201–22. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1468-0068.2010.00786.xGoogle Scholar
Sohoglu, E., Peelle, J. E., Carlyon, R. P., & Davis, M. H. (2012). Predictive top-down integration of prior knowledge during speech perceptionJournal of Neuroscience32(25), 8443–53. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5069-11.2012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tagkopoulos, I., Liu, Y.-C., & Tavazoie, S. (2008). Predictive behavior within microbial genetic networks. Science, 320(5881), 1313–17. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1154456Google Scholar
Thierry, G., Athanasopoulos, P., Wiggett, A., Dering, B., & Kuipers, J.-R. (2009). Unconscious effects of language-specific terminology on preattentive color perceptionProceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America106(11), 4567–70. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0811155106Google Scholar
Thompson, E., Lutz, A., & Cosmelli, D. (2005). Neurophenomenology: An introduction for neurophilosophers. In Brook, A. & Atkins, K. (Eds.), Cognition and the brain: The philosophy and neuroscience movement (pp. 4097). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511610608.003Google Scholar
Toribio, J. (2018). Visual experience: Rich but impenetrableSynthese195(8), 3389–406. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229–015-0889-8Google Scholar
Vetter, P., & Newen, A. (2014). Varieties of cognitive penetration in visual perception. Consciousness and Cognition, 27, 6275. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2014.04.007Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (1983). Remarks on the foundations of mathematics (Anscombe, G. E. M., Trans.; von Wright, G. H. & Rhees, R., Eds.). Blackwell. (Original work published 1956)Google Scholar
Wittgenstein, L. (2009). Philosophical investigations (4th ed., Anscombe, G.E.M., Hacker, P. M. S., & Schulte, J., Trans., Hacker, P. M. S., & Schulte, J., Eds.). Blackwell Publishing. (Original work published 1953)Google Scholar
Wright, C. (2007). Rule-following without reasons: Wittgenstein’s quietism and the constitutive questionRatio20(4), 481502. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9329.2007.00379.xGoogle Scholar
Zeimbekis, J., & Raftopoulos, A. (Eds.). (2015). The cognitive penetrability of perception: New philosophical perspectives. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

References

Adams, R. A., Shipp, S., & Friston, K. J. (2013). Predictions not commands: Active inference in the motor system. Brain Structure and Function, 218(3), 611–43. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429–012-0475-5Google Scholar
Altschul, D., Jensen, G. G., & Terrace, H. S. (2017). Perceptual category learning of photographic and painterly stimuli in rhesus macaques (Macaca mulatta) and humans. PLoS ONE, 12(9), e0185576. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0185576Google Scholar
Atzil, S., Gao, W., Fradkin, I., & Barrett, L. F. (2018). Growing a social brainNature Human Behaviour, 2, 624–36. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562–018-0384-6Google Scholar
Atzil, S., & Gendron, M. (2017). Bio-behavioral synchrony promotes the development of conceptualized emotions. Current Opinion in Psychology, 17(Supplement C), 162–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.07.009Google Scholar
Atzil, S., Touroutoglou, A., Rudy, T., Salcedo, S., Feldman, R., Hooker, J. M., Dickerson, B. C., Catana, C., & Barrett, L. F. (2017). Dopamine in the medial amygdala network mediates human bonding. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114(9), 2361–6. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1612233114Google ScholarPubMed
Barrett, L. F. (2017a). How emotions are made: The secret life of the brain. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F. (2017b). The theory of constructed emotion: An active inference account of interoception and categorization. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(1), 123. https://doi.org/10.1093%2Fscan%2Fnsw154Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F., & Bliss-Moreau, E. (2009). Affect as a psychological primitive. In Zanna, M. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 41, pp. 167218). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065–2601(08)00404-8Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F., & Finlay, B. L. (2018). Concepts, goals and the control of survival-related behaviors. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 24, 172–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.10.001Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F., Quigley, K. S., & Hamilton, P. (2016). An active inference theory of allostasis and interoception in depression. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1708), 20160011. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2016.0011Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F., & Russell, J. A. (1999). Structure of current affect: Controversies and emerging consensus. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8(1), 1014. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.00003Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F., & Satpute, A. B. (2013). Large-scale brain networks in affective and social neuroscience: Towards an integrative functional architecture of the brain. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 23(3), 361–72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2012.12.012Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F., & Satpute, A. B. (2017). Historical pitfalls and new directions in the neuroscience of emotionNeuroscience Letters, 693, 918. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2017.07.045Google Scholar
Barrett, L. F., & Simmons, W. K. (2015). Interoceptive predictions in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 16(7), 419–29. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3950CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barsalou, L. W. (1983). Ad hoc categories. Memory & Cognition, 11(3), 211–27. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03196968Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. W. (1999). Perceptual symbol systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 22(4), 577609. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X99002149Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617–45. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639Google Scholar
Bassett, D. S., & Bullmore, E. (2006). Small-world brain networks. The Neuroscientist, 12(6), 512–23. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858406293182Google Scholar
Beeghly, M., Bretherton, I., & Mervis, C. B. (1986). Mothers’ internal state language to toddlers. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 4(3), 247–61. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.1986.tb01016.xGoogle Scholar
Berry, J. W., Kim, U., Minde, T., & Mok, D. (1987). Comparative studies of acculturative stress. International Migration Review, 21(3), 491511. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F019791838702100303CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyd, R., Richerson, P. J., & Henrich, J. (2011). The cultural niche: Why social learning is essential for human adaptation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(Supplement 2), 10918–25. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1100290108Google Scholar
Buckner, R. L. (2012). The serendipitous discovery of the brain’s default network. NeuroImage, 62(2), 1137–45. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.10.035CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cao, M., Huang, H., & He, Y. (2017). Developmental connectomics from infancy through early childhood. Trends in Neurosciences, 40(8), 494506. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2017.06.003Google Scholar
Chanes, L., & Barrett, L. F. (2016). Redefining the role of limbic areas in cortical processing. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(2), 96106. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.11.005Google Scholar
Chen, M. L., & Waxman, S. R. (2013). “Shall we blick?”: Novel words highlight actors’ underlying intentions for 14-month-old infants. Developmental Psychology, 49(3), 426–31. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0029486Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(3), 181204. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12000477Google Scholar
Colombetti, G., & Krueger, J. (2015). Scaffoldings of the affective mind. Journal of Philosophical Psychology, 28(8), 1157–76. https://doi.org/10.1080/09515089.2014.976334Google Scholar
Conant, R. C., & Ashby, W. R. (1970). Every good regulator of a system must be a model of that system. International Journal of Systems Science, 1(2), 8997. https://doi.org/10.1080/00207727008920220Google Scholar
Consedine, N. S., Chentsova-Dutton, Y. E., & Krivoshekova, Y. S. (2014). Emotional acculturation predicts better somatic health: Experiential and expressive acculturation among immigrant women from four ethnic groups. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 33(10), 867–89. https://doi.org/10.1521/jscp.2014.33.10.867Google Scholar
Craig, A. D. (2009). How do you feel – now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(1), 5970. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2555Google Scholar
Craig, A. D. (2014). How do you feel? An interoceptive moment with your neurobiological self. Princeton University Press. https://doi.org/10.23943/princeton/9780691156767.001.0001Google Scholar
Critchley, H. D., Wiens, S., Rotshtein, P., Öhman, A., & Dolan, R. J. (2004). Neural systems supporting interoceptive awareness. Nature Neuroscience, 7(2), 189–95. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn1176Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (1999). The feeling of what happens: Body and emotion in the making of consciousness: Harcourt College.Google Scholar
Danziger, K. (1997). Naming the mind: How psychology found its language. SAGE. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781446221815Google Scholar
Deco, G., Jirsa, V. K., & McIntosh, A. R. (2011). Emerging concepts for the dynamical organization of resting-state activity in the brain. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 12(1), 4356. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2961Google Scholar
De Leersnyder, J. (2017). Emotional acculturation: A first review. Current Opinion in Psychology, 17, 6773. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2017.06.007Google Scholar
De Leersnyder, J., Kim, H., & Mesquita, B. (2015). Feeling right is feeling good: Psychological well-being and emotional fit with culture in autonomy-versus relatedness-promoting situations. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 630. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.00630Google Scholar
De Leersnyder, J., Mesquita, B., & Kim, H. S. (2011). Where do my emotions belong? A study of immigrants’ emotional acculturation. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(4), 451–63. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167211399103Google Scholar
De Leersnyder, J., Mesquita, B., Kim, H., Eom, K., & Choi, H. (2014). Emotional fit with culture: A predictor of individual differences in relational well-being. Emotion, 14(2), 241–5. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0035296CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doamekpor, L. A., & Dinwiddie, G. Y. (2015). Allostatic load in foreign-born and US-born blacks: Evidence from the 2001–2010 National Health and Nutrition Examination Survey. American Journal of Public Health, 105(3), 591–7. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2014.302285Google Scholar
Doan, S. N., & Wang, Q. (2010). Maternal discussions of mental states and behaviors: Relations to emotion situation knowledge in European American and immigrant Chinese children. Child Development, 81(5), 1490–503. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-8624.2010.01487.xGoogle Scholar
Dosenbach, N. U. F., Nardos, B., Cohen, A. L., Fair, D. A., Power, J. D., Church, J. A., Nelson, S. M., Wig, G. S., Vogel, A. C., Lessov-Schlaggar, C. N., Barnes, K. A., Dubis, J. W., Feczko, E., Coalson, R. S., Pruett, J. R. Jr., Barch, D. M., Petersen, S. E., & Schlaggar, B. L. (2010). Prediction of individual brain maturity using fMRI. Science, 329(5997), 1358–61. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1194144Google Scholar
Dreyfus, G., & Thompson, E. (2007). Asian perspectives: Indian theories of mind. In Zelazo, P. D., Moscovitch, M., & Thompson, E. (Eds.), The Cambridge handbook of consciousness (pp. 89114). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dubois, J., Dehaene-Lambertz, G., Kulikova, S., Poupon, C., Hüppi, P. S., & Hertz-Pannier, L. (2014). The early development of brain white matter: A review of imaging studies in fetuses, newborns and infants. Neuroscience, 276, 4871. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroscience.2013.12.044Google Scholar
Dunn, J., Bretherton, I., & Munn, P. (1987). Conversations about feeling states between mothers and their young children. Developmental Psychology, 23(1), 132–9. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.23.1.132Google Scholar
Edelman, G. M., & Tononi, G. (2000). A universe of consciousness: How matter becomes imagination. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Feldman, R. (2017). The neurobiology of human attachments. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 2(21), 8099. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.11.007Google Scholar
Ferry, A. L., Hespos, S. J., & Waxman, S. R. (2010). Categorization in 3- and 4-month-old infants: An advantage of words over tones. Child Development, 81(2), 472–9. https://doi.org/10.1111%2Fj.1467-8624.2009.01408.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Finlay, B. L., & Uchiyama, R. (2015). Developmental mechanisms channeling cortical evolution. Trends in Neurosciences, 38(2), 6976. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tins.2014.11.004Google Scholar
Finlay, B., & Uchiyama, R. (2017). The timing of brain maturation, early experience, and the human social niche. In Kaas, J. H. (Ed.), Evolution of nervous systems (2nd ed., pp. 123–48). Elsevier.Google Scholar
Fisher, H. E., Brown, L. L., Aron, A., Strong, G., & Mashek, D. (2010). Reward, addiction, and emotion regulation systems associated with rejection in love. Journal of Neurophysiology, 104(1), 5160. https://doi.org/10.1152/jn.00784.2009Google Scholar
Fransson, P., Åden, U., Blennow, M., & Lagercrantz, H. (2011). The functional architecture of the infant brain as revealed by resting-state fMRI. Cerebral Cortex, 21(1), 145–54. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhq071Google Scholar
Fransson, P., Skiöld, B., Horsch, S., Nordell, A., Blennow, M., Lagercrantz, H., & Åden, U. (2007). Resting-state networks in the infant brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(39), 15531–6. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0704380104Google Scholar
Friston, K. (2010). The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(2), 127–38. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2787Google Scholar
Friston, K., FitzGerald, T., Rigoli, F., Schwartenbeck, P., & Pezzulo, G. (2017). Active inference: A process theory. Neural Computation, 29(1), 149. https://doi.org/10.1162/NECO_a_00912Google Scholar
Fulmer, C. A., Gelfand, M. J., Kruglanski, A. W., Kim-Prieto, C., Diener, E., Pierro, A., & Higgins, E. T. (2010). On “feeling right” in cultural contexts: How person-culture match affects self-esteem and subjective well-being. Psychological Science, 21(11), 1563–9. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610384742Google Scholar
Gao, W., Gilmore, J. H., Giovanello, K. S., Smith, J. K., Shen, D., Zhu, H., & Lin, W. (2011). Temporal and spatial evolution of brain network topology during the first two years of life. PLoS ONE, 6(9), e25278. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0025278Google Scholar
Gao, W., Lin, W., Chen, Y., Gerig, G., Smith, J. K., Jewells, V., & Gilmore, J. H. (2009). Temporal and spatial development of axonal maturation and myelination of white matter in the developing brain. American Journal of Neuroradiology, 30(2), 290–6. https://doi.org/10.3174/ajnr.A1363Google Scholar
Gao, W., Lin, W., Grewen, K., & Gilmore, J. H. (2017). Functional connectivity of the infant human brain: Plastic and modifiable. The Neuroscientist, 23(2), 169–84. https://doi.org/10.1177/1073858416635986Google Scholar
Gao, W., Zhu, H., Giovanello, K. S., Smith, J. K., Shen, D., Gilmore, J. H., & Lin, W. (2009). Evidence on the emergence of the brain’s default network from 2-week-old to 2-year-old healthy pediatric subjects. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(16), 6790–5. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0811221106Google Scholar
Gebauer, J. E., Sedikides, C., & Neberich, W. (2012). Religiosity, social self-esteem, and psychological adjustment: On the cross-cultural specificity of the psychological benefits of religiosity. Psychological Science, 23(2), 158–60. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0956797611427045Google Scholar
Gelfand, M. J., Raver, J. L., Nishii, L., Leslie, L. M., Lun, J., Lim, B. C., Duan, L., Almaliach, A., Ang, S., Arnadottir, J., Aycan, Z., Boehnke, K., Boski, P., Cabecinhas, R., Chan, D., Chhokar, J., D’Amato, A., Ferrer, M., Fischlmayr, I. C., … Yamaguchi, S. (2011). Differences between tight and loose cultures: A 33-nation study. Science, 332(6033), 1100–4. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1197754Google Scholar
Gelman, S. A. (2009). Learning from others: Children’s construction of concepts. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 115–40. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093659Google Scholar
Gendron, M., Crivelli, C., & Barrett, L. F. (2018). Universality reconsidered: Diversity in meaning making about facial expressions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27(4), 211–19. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0963721417746794Google Scholar
Geng, X., Li, G., Lu, Z., Gao, W., Wang, L., Shen, D., & Gilmore, J. H. (2017). Structural and maturational covariance in early childhood brain development. Cerebral Cortex, 27(3), 1795–807. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhw022Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M., Keller, H., Fuligni, A., & Maynard, A. (2003). Cultural pathways through universal development. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 461–90. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.54.101601.145221Google Scholar
Güngör, D., Bornstein, M. H., De Leersnyder, J., Cote, L., Ceulemans, E., & Mesquita, B. (2013). Acculturation of personality: A three-culture study of Japanese, Japanese Americans, and European Americans. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 44(5), 701–18. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0022022112470749CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heelas, P. (1996). Emotion talk across cultures. In Harré, R. & Parrott, W. G. (Eds.), The emotions: Social, cultural and biological dimensions (pp. 171–99). SAGE. https://doi.org/10.4135/9781446221952.n12Google Scholar
Heine, S. J., & Lehman, D. R. (2004). Move the body, change the self: Acculturative effects on the self-concept. In Schaller, M. & Crandall, C. S. (Eds.), The psychological foundations of culture (pp. 305–31). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Henrich, J., Boyd, R., Derex, M., Kline, M. A., Mesoudi, A., Muthukrishna, M., Powell, A. T., Shennan, S. J., & Thomas, M. G. (2016). Understanding cumulative cultural evolution. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 113(44), E6724E6725. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1610005113Google Scholar
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 6183. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0999152XGoogle Scholar
Hermundstad, A. M., Bassett, D. S., Brown, K. S., Aminoff, E. M., Clewett, D., Freeman, S., Frithsen, A., Johnson, A., Tipper, C. M., Miller, M. B., Grafton, S. T., & Carlson, J. M. (2013). Structural foundations of resting-state and task-based functional connectivity in the human brain. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 110(15), 6169–74. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1219562110Google Scholar
Heyes, C. (2018). Cognitive gadgets: The cultural evolution of thinking. Belknap Press.Google Scholar
Hirschfeld, L. W. (2013). The myth of mentalizing and the primacy of folk sociology. In Banaji, M. R. & Gelman, S. A. (Eds.), Navigating the social world: What infants, children, and other species can teach us (pp. 101–6). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hoemann, K., Wu, R., LobBue, V., Oakes, L. M., Xu, F., & Barrett, L. F. (2020). Developing an understanding of emotion categories: Lessons from objects. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24, 39–51. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2019.10.010Google Scholar
Hoemann, K., Xu, F., & Barrett, L. F. (2019). Emotion words, emotion concepts, and emotional development in children: A constructionist hypothesis. Developmental Psychology, 55, 1830–49. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000686Google Scholar
Hohwy, J. (2013). The predictive mind. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199682737.001.0001Google Scholar
Honey, C., Sporns, O., Cammoun, L., Gigandet, X., Thiran, J.-P., Meuli, R., & Hagmann, P. (2009). Predicting human resting-state functional connectivity from structural connectivity. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 106(6), 2035–40. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0811168106Google Scholar
Hong, Y.-y., Morris, M. W., Chiu, C.-y., & Benet-Martínez, V. (2000). Multicultural minds: A dynamic constructivist approach to culture and cognition. American Psychologist, 55(7), 709–20. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.55.7.709Google Scholar
Hutchinson, J. B., & Barrett, L. F. (2019). The power of predictions: An emerging paradigm for psychological research. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 28(3), 280–91. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0963721419831992Google Scholar
Hyman, I. (2004). Setting the stage: Reviewing current knowledge on the health of Canadian immigrants: What is the evidence and where are the gaps? Canadian Journal of Public Health, 95(3), I4I8. www.jstor.org/stable/41994328Google Scholar
Jablonka, E., & Lamb, M. J. (2007). Précis of evolution in four dimensions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(4), 353–65. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X07002221Google Scholar
James, W. (2007). The principles of psychology (Vol. 1). Dover and Cosimo. (Original work published 1890)Google Scholar
Juster, R.-P., McEwen, B. S., & Lupien, S. J. (2010). Allostatic load biomarkers of chronic stress and impact on health and cognition. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 35(1), 216. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.10.002Google Scholar
Kan, I. P., Barsalou, L. W., Solomon, K. O., Minor, J. K., & Thompson-Schill, S. L. (2003). Role of mental imagery in a property verification task: fMRI evidence for perceptual representations of conceptual knowledge. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 20(3–6), 525–40. https://doi.org/10.1080/02643290244000257Google Scholar
Kashdan, T. B., Barrett, L. F., & McKnight, P. E. (2015). Unpacking emotion differentiation: Transforming unpleasant experience by perceiving distinctions in negativity. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(1), 1016. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0963721414550708Google Scholar
Keck, T., Keller, G. B., Jacobsen, R. I., Eysel, U. T., Bonhoeffer, T., & Hübener, M. (2013). Synaptic scaling and homeostatic plasticity in the mouse visual cortex in vivo. Neuron, 80(2), 327–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2013.08.018Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., Mesquita, B., & Karasawa, M. (2006). Cultural affordances and emotional experience: Socially engaging and disengaging emotions in Japan and the United States. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 91(5), 890903. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.91.5.890Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., & Salvador, C. E. (2017). Culture embrained: Going beyond the nature-nurture dichotomy. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(5), 841–54. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617707317Google Scholar
Kleckner, I. R., Zhang, J., Touroutoglou, A., Chanes, L., Xia, C., Simmons, W. K., Quigley, K. S., Dickerson, B. C., & Barrett, L. F. (2017). Evidence for a large-scale brain system supporting allostasis and interoception in humans. Nature Human Behaviour, 1, 0069. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562–017-0069Google Scholar
Korte, S. M., Koolhaas, J. M., Wingfield, J. C., & McEwen, B. S. (2005). The Darwinian concept of stress: Benefits of allostasis and costs of allostatic load and the trade-offs in health and disease. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 29(1), 338. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2004.08.009Google Scholar
Kousta, S.-T., Vigliocco, G., Vinson, D. P., Andrews, M., & Del Campo, E. (2011). The representation of abstract words: Why emotion matters. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140(1), 1434. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021446Google Scholar
Kuppens, P., Tuerlinckx, F., Russell, J. A., & Barrett, L. F. (2013). The relation between valence and arousal in subjective experience. Psychological Bulletin, 139(4), 917–40. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0030811Google Scholar
Laland, K. N., Odling-Smee, J., & Feldman, M. W. (2000). Niche construction, biological evolution, and cultural change. Behavioral Brain Sciences, 23(1), 131–46. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00002417Google Scholar
Levy, R. I. (1975). Tahitians: Mind and experience in the Society Islands. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Lewontin, R. (2000). The triple helix: Gene, organism, and environment. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Lillard, A. (1998). Ethnopsychologies: Cultural variations in theories of mind. Psychological Bulletin, 123(1), 332. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.123.1.3Google Scholar
Lindquist, K. A., Gendron, M., Oosterwijk, S., & Barrett, L. F. (2013). Do people essentialize emotions? Individual differences in emotion essentialism and emotional experience. Emotion, 13(4), 629–44. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032283Google Scholar
Lindquist, K. A., Satpute, A. B., Wager, T. D., Weber, J., & Barrett, L. F. (2016). The brain basis of positive and negative affect: Evidence from a meta-analysis of the human neuroimaging literature. Cerebral Cortex, 26(5), 1910–22. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhv001Google Scholar
Linnet, J. T. (2011). Money can’t buy me hygge: Danish middle-class consumption, egalitarianism, and the sanctity of inner space. Social Analysis, 55(2), 2144. https://doi.org/10.3167/sa.2011.550202Google Scholar
Lipo, C. P., O’Brien, M. J., Collard, M., & Shennan, S. (2006). Mapping our ancestors: Phylogenetic approaches in anthropology and prehistory. New Aldine Transaction.Google Scholar
Lochmann, T., & Den, ève, S. (2011). Neural processing as causal inference. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 21(5), 774–81. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2011.05.018Google Scholar
Low, L. K., & Cheng, H.-J. (2006). Axon pruning: An essential step underlying the developmental plasticity of neuronal connections. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 361(1473), 1531–44. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2006.1883Google Scholar
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224–53. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224Google Scholar
McEwen, B. S. (2005). Stressed or stressed out: What is the difference? Journal of Psychiatry and Neuroscience, 30(5), 315–18. http://jpn.ca/wp-content/uploads/2014/04/30-5-315.pdfGoogle Scholar
Medin, D., & Ortony, A. (1989). Comments on part I: Psychological essentialism. In Vosniadou, S. & Ortony, A. (Eds.), Similarity and analogical reasoning (pp. 179–96). Cambridge University Press. https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511529863.009Google Scholar
Mesoudi, A., Whiten, A., & Laland, K. N. (2006). Towards a unified science of cultural evolution. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29(4), 329–47. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X06009083Google Scholar
Mesquita, B. (2001). Emotions in collectivist and individualist contexts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(1), 6874. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.80.1.68Google Scholar
Mesquita, B., & Boiger, M. (2014). Emotions in context: A sociodynamic model of emotions. Emotion Review, 6(4), 298302. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F1754073914534480Google Scholar
Mesquita, B., De Leersnyder, J., & Jasini, A. (2019). The cultural psychology of acculturation. In Kitayama, S. & Cohen, D. (Eds.), Handbook of cultural psychology (2nd ed.). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Mesquita, B., & Frijda, N. H. (1992). Cultural variations in emotions: A review. Psychological Bulletin, 112(2), 179204. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.112.2.179Google Scholar
Mithen, S., & Parsons, L. (2008). The brain as a cultural artefact. Cambridge Archaeological Journal, 18(3), 415–22. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0959774308000450Google Scholar
Mitra, A., & Raichle, M. E. (2016). How networks communicate: Propagation patterns in spontaneous brain activity. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1705), 20150546. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0546Google Scholar
Murphy, G. L. (2002). The big book of concepts. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Muthukrishna, M., & Henrich, J. (2016). Innovation in the collective brain. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1690), 20150192. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2015.0192Google Scholar
Nguyen, A.-M. D., & Benet-Martínez, V. (2013). Biculturalism and adjustment: A meta-analysis. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 44(1), 122–59. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022111435097Google Scholar
Niedenthal, P. M., Winkielman, P., Mondillon, L., & Vermeulen, N. (2009). Embodiment of emotion concepts. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(6), 1120–36. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015574Google Scholar
Odling-Smee, F. J., Laland, K. N., & Feldman, M. W. (2003). Niche construction: The neglected process in evolution. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Oosterwijk, S., Mackey, S., Wilson-Mendenhall, C., Winkielman, P., & Paulus, M. P. (2015). Concepts in context: Processing mental state concepts with internal or external focus involves different neural systems. Social Neuroscience, 10(3), 294307. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2014.998840Google Scholar
Pavlenko, A. (2009). Conceptual representation in the bilingual lexicon and second language vocabulary learning. In Pavlenko, A. (Ed.), The bilingual mental lexicon: Interdisciplinary approaches (pp. 125–60). Multilingual Matters.Google Scholar
Pavlenko, A. (2014). The bilingual mind: And what it tells us about language and thought. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Peek, M. K., Cutchin, M. P., Salinas, J. J., Sheffield, K. M., Eschbach, K., Stowe, R. P., & Goodwin, J. S. (2010). Allostatic load among non-Hispanic Whites, non-Hispanic Blacks, and people of Mexican origin: Effects of ethnicity, nativity, and acculturation. American Journal of Public Health, 100(5), 940–5. https://doi.org/10.2105/AJPH.2007.129312Google Scholar
Pezzulo, G., Rigoli, F., & Friston, K. (2015). Active Inference, homeostatic regulation and adaptive behavioural control. Progress in Neurobiology, 134, 1735. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2015.09.001Google Scholar
Pouliasi, K., & Verkuyten, M. (2012). Understanding the relational self: An inter-generational study in the Netherlands and Greece. European Psychologist, 17(3), 182–9. https://doi.org/10.1027/1016-9040/a000095Google Scholar
Ramstead, M. J. D., Veissière, S. P. L., & Kirmayer, L. J. (2016). Cultural affordances: Scaffolding local worlds through shared intentionality and regimes of attention. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1090. https://doi.org/10.3389%2Ffpsyg.2016.01090Google Scholar
Rao, R. P. N., & Ballard, D. H. (1999). Predictive coding in the visual cortex: A functional interpretation of some extra-classical receptive-field effects. Nature Neuroscience, 2(1), 7987. https://doi.org/10.1038/4580Google Scholar
Rebollo, I., Devauchelle, A-D., Béranger, B., & Tallon-Baudry, C. (2018). Stomach-brain synchrony reveals delayed-connectivity resting-state network in humans. eLIFE, 7, e33321. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.33321.001Google Scholar
Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (1980). A circumplex model of affect. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 39(6), 1161–78. http://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/h0077714Google Scholar
Russell, J. A. (1991). Culture and the categorization of emotions. Psychological Bulletin, 110(3), 426–50. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.110.3.426Google Scholar
Sam, D. L., & Berry, J. W. (2010). Acculturation: When individuals and groups of different cultural backgrounds meet. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5(4), 472–81. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691610373075Google Scholar
Savani, K., Morris, M. W., Naidu, N. V. R., Kumar, S., & Berlia, N. V. (2011). Cultural conditioning: Understanding interpersonal accommodation in India and the United States in terms of the modal characteristics of interpersonal influence situations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(1), 84102. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021083Google Scholar
Schulkin, J. (2010). Social allostasis: Anticipatory regulation of the internal milieu. Frontiers in Evolutionary Neuroscience, 2, 111. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnevo.2010.00111Google Scholar
Searle, J. R. (1992). The rediscovery of the mind. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Searle, J. R. (2004). Mind: A brief introduction. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Seeley, W. W., Menon, V., Schatzberg, A. F., Keller, J., Glover, G. H., Kenna, H., Reiss, A. A., & Greicius, M. D. (2007). Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(9), 2349–56. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007Google Scholar
Seth, A. K. (2013). Interoceptive inference, emotion, and the embodied self. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(11), 565–73. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.007Google Scholar
Seth, A. K., Suzuki, K., & Critchley, H. D. (2012). An interoceptive predictive coding model of conscious presence. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 395. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00395Google Scholar
Shweder, R. A., Goodnow, J. J., Hatano, G., LeVine, R. A., Markus, H. R., & Miller, P. J. (1998). The cultural psychology of development: One mind, many mentalities. In Damon, W. & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Theoretical models of human development (Vol. 1, 5th ed., pp. 865937). John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Siegel, E. H., Sands, M. K., Van den Noortgate, W., Condon, P., Chang, Y., Dy, J., Quigley, K. S., & Barrett, L. F. (2018). Emotion fingerprints or emotion populations? A meta-analytic investigation of autonomic features of emotion categoriesPsychological Bulletin, 144(4), 343–93. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000128Google Scholar
Smith, C. A., & Lazarus, R. S. (1993). Appraisal components, core relational themes, and the emotions. Cognition and Emotion, 7(3–4), 233–69. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699939308409189Google Scholar
Smyser, C. D., Snyder, A. Z., & Neil, J. J. (2011). Functional connectivity MRI in infants: Exploration of the functional organization of the developing brain. NeuroImage, 56(3), 1437–52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.02.073Google Scholar
Sporns, O. (2011). Networks of the brain. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Spratling, M. W. (2017). A review of predictive coding algorithms. Brain and Cognition, 112, 92–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2015.11.003Google Scholar
Sterling, P. (2012). Allostasis: A model of predictive regulation. Physiology & Behavior, 106(1), 515. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.06.004Google Scholar
Sterling, P., & Eyer, J. (1988). Allostasis: A new paradigm to explain arousal pathology. In Fisher, S. & Reason, J. (Eds.), Handbook of life stress, cognition and health (pp. 629–49). John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Sterling, P., & Laughlin, S. (2015). Principles of neural design. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Taylor, T. (2006). The human brain is a cultural artefact. Edge: What is your dangerous idea? www.edge.org/response-detail/11835Google Scholar
Theriault, J. E., Young, L., & Barrett, L. F. (2019). The sense of should: A biologically based model of social pressure. PsyArXiv. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/x5rbsGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2014). The ultra-social animal. European Journal of Social Psychology, 44(3), 187–94. https://doi.org/10.1002%2Fejsp.2015Google Scholar
Uchida, Y., Townsend, S. S. M., Markus, H. R., & Bergsieker, H. B. (2009). Emotions as within or between people? Cultural variation in lay theories of emotion expression and inference. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 35(11), 1427–39. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167209347322Google Scholar
van den Heuvel, M. P., & Sporns, O. (2013). Network hubs in the human brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(12), 683–96. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2013.09.012Google Scholar
Vilares, I., & Kording, K. (2011). Bayesian models: The structure of the world, uncertainty, behavior, and the brain. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1224(1), 2239. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.05965.x.Google Scholar
Waxman, S. R., & Gelman, S. A. (2010). Different kinds of concepts and different kinds of words: What words do for human cognition. In Mareschal, D., Quinn, P. C., & Lea, S. E. G. (Eds.), Oxford series in developmental neuroscience. The making of human concepts (pp. 101–30). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199549221.003.06Google Scholar
Wierzbicka, A. (1999). Emotions across languages and cultures: Diversity and universals. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wilson-Mendenhall, C. D., Barrett, L. F., Simmons, W. K., & Barsalou, L. W. (2011). Grounding emotion in situated conceptualization. Neuropsychologia, 49(5), 1105–27. https://doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.neuropsychologia.2010.12.032Google Scholar
Wundt, W. (1897). Outlines of psychology (Judd, C. H., Trans.). Wilhelm Engelmann and Gustav E. Stechert. (Original work published 1896)Google Scholar
Yeo, B. T. T., Krienen, F. M., Eickhoff, S. B., Yaakub, S. N., Fox, P. T., Buckner, R. L., Asplund, C. L., & Chee, M. W. L. (2015). Functional specialization and flexibility in human association cortex. Cerebral Cortex, 25(10), 3654–72. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhu217Google Scholar

References

Blakemore, S. J. (2008). The social brain in adolescence. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(4), 267–77. https://doi.org/10.11038/nrn2353Google Scholar
Brewer, M. B., & Gardner, W. L. (1996). Who is this ‘we’? Levels of collective identity and self representations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(1), 8393. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.71.1.83Google Scholar
Bromm, B., & Chen, A. C. N. (1995). Brain electrical source analysis of laser evoked potentials in response to painful trigeminal nerve stimulation. Electroencephalography of Clinical Neurophysiology, 95(1), 1426. https://doi.org/10.1016/0013-4694(95)00032-TGoogle Scholar
Cheon, B. K., Im, D.-m., Harada, T., Kim, J.-S., Mathur, V. A., Scimeca, J. M., Parrish, T. B., Park, H. W., & Chiao, J. Y. (2011). Cultural influences on neural basis of intergroup empathy. NeuroImage, 57(2), 642–50. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2011.04.031Google Scholar
Chiao, J. Y., & Ambady, N. (2007). Cultural neuroscience: Parsing universality and diversity across levels of analysis. In: Kitayama, S. & Cohen, D. (Eds.), Handbook of cultural psychology (pp. 237–54). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Chiao, J. Y., Harada, T., Komeda, H., Li, Z., Mano, Y., Saito, D., Parrish, T. B., Sadato, N., & Iidaka, T. (2009). Dynamic cultural influences on neural representations of the self. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22, 111. https://doi.org/10.1162/jocn.2009.21192Google Scholar
Christmann, C., Koeppe, C., Braus, D., Ruf, M., & Flora, H. (2007). A simultaneous EEG-fMRI study of painful electrical stimulation. NeuroImage, 34, 1428–37. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.11.006Google Scholar
Colzato, L. S., de Bruijn, E. R., & Hommel, B. (2012). Up to “me” or up to “us”? The impact of self-construal priming on cognitive self-other integration. Frontiers in Psychology, 3, 341. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00341Google Scholar
Gardner, W. L., Gabriel, S., & Lee, A. Y. (1999). ‘I’ value freedom, but ‘we’ value relationships: Self-construal priming mirrors cultural differences in judgment. Psychological Science, 10(4), 321–6. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00162Google Scholar
Han, S. (2015). Understanding cultural differences in human behavior: A cultural neuroscience approach. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 3, 6872. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cobeha.2015.01.013Google Scholar
Han, S. (2017). The sociocultural brain: A cultural neuroscience approach to human nature. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Han, S., & Humphreys, G. W. (2016). Self-construal: A cultural framework for brain function. Current Opinion in Psychology, 8, 1014. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2015.09.013Google Scholar
Han, S., & Ma, Y. (2014). Cultural differences in human brain activity: A quantitative meta-analysis. NeuroImage, 99, 293300. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.05.062Google Scholar
Han, S., & Ma, Y. (2015). A culture-behavior-brain loop model of human development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(11), 666–76. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.08.010Google Scholar
Han, S., & Northoff, G. (2008). Culture-sensitive neural substrates of human cognition: A transcultural neuroimaging approach. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(8), 646–54. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2456Google Scholar
Han, S., Northoff, G., Vogeley, K., Wexler, B. E., Kitayama, S., & Varnum, M. E. W. (2013). A cultural neuroscience approach to the biosocial nature of the human brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 335–59. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-071112-054629Google Scholar
Hedden, T., Ketay, S., Aron, A., Markus, H. R., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (2008). Cultural influences on neural substrates of attentional control. Psychological Science, 19(1), 1217. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02038.xGoogle Scholar
Hong, Y.-y., Morris, M. W., Chiu, C.-y., & Benet-Martínez, V. (2000). Multicultural minds: A dynamic constructivist approach to culture and cognition. American Psychologist, 55(7), 709–20. https://doi.org/10.1037//0003-066x.55.7.709Google Scholar
Hong, Y., Wan, C., No, S., & Chiu, C-.y. (2007). Multicultural identities. In Kitayama, S. & Cohen, D. (Eds.), Handbook of cultural psychology (pp. 323–46). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., Yanagisawa, K., Ito, A., Ueda, R., Uchida, Y., & Abe, N. (2017). Reduced orbitofrontal cortical volume is associated with interdependent self-construal. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 114(30), 7969–74. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1704831114Google Scholar
Kühnen, U., & Oyserman, D. (2002). Thinking about the self influences thinking in general: Cognitive consequences of salient self-concept. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 38(5), 492–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0022-1031(02)00011-2Google Scholar
Lin, Z., & Han, S. (2009). Self-construal priming modulates the scope of visual attention. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 62(4), 802–13. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210802271650Google Scholar
Lin, Z., Lin, Y., & Han, S. (2008). Self-construal priming modulates visual activity underlying global/local perception. Biological Psychology, 77(1), 93–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2007.08.002Google Scholar
Luo, S., Ma, Y., Liu, Y., Li, B., Wang, C., Shi, Z., Li, X., Zhang, W., Rao, Y., & Han, S. (2015). Interaction between oxytocin receptor polymorphism and interdependent culture on human empathy. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(9), 1273–81. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsv019Google Scholar
Luo, S., Yu, D., & Han, S. (2017). 5-HTTLPR moderates the association between interdependence and brain responses to mortality threats. Human Brain Mapping, 38(12), 6157–71. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23819Google Scholar
Ma, Y., Bang, D., Wang, C., Allen, M., Frith, C., Roepstorff, A., & Han, S. (2014). Sociocultural patterning of neural activity during self-reflection. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(1), 7380. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nss103Google Scholar
Ma, Y., Wang, C., Li, B., Zhang, W., Rao, Y., & Han, S. (2014). Does self-construal predict activity in the social brain network? A genetic moderation effect. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 9(9), 1360–7. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nst125Google Scholar
Markus, H. R., & Hamedani, M. G. (2007). Sociocultural psychology: The dynamic interdependence among self systems and social systems. In Kitayama, S., & Cohen, D. (Eds.), Handbook of cultural psychology (pp. 339). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224–53. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224Google Scholar
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (2010). Cultures and selves: A cycle of mutual constitution. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5(4), 420–30. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691610375557Google Scholar
Martínez, A., Anllo-Vento, L., Sereno, M. I., Frank, L. R., Buxton, R. B., Dubowitz, D. J., Wong, E. C., Hinrichs, H., Heinze, H. J., & Hillyard, S. A. (1999). Involvement of striate and extrastriate visual cortical areas in spatial attention. Nature Neuroscience, 2(4), 364–9. https://doi.org/10.1038/7274Google Scholar
Mathur, V. A., Harada, T., Lipke, T., & Chiao, J. Y. (2010). Neural basis of extraordinary empathy and altruistic motivation. NeuroImage, 51(4), 1468–75. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.03.025Google Scholar
Miyamoto, Y. (2013). Culture and analytic versus holistic cognition: Toward multilevel analysis of cultural influences. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 47, 131–88. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-407236-7.00003-6Google Scholar
Mu, Y., Kitayama, S., Han, S., & Gelfand, M. J. (2015). How culture gets embrained: Cultural differences in event-related potentials of social norm violations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112(50), 15348–53. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1509839112Google Scholar
Ng, S. H., Han, S., Mao, L., & Lai, J. C. L. (2010). Dynamic bicultural brains: fMRI study of their flexible neural representation of self and significant others in response to culture primes. Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 13(2), 8391. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-839X.2010.01303.xGoogle Scholar
Ng, S. H., & Lai, J. C. L. (2009). Effects of culture priming on the social connectedness of the bicultural self: A self-reference effect approach. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 40(2), 170–86. https://doi.org/10.1177/0022022108328818Google Scholar
Oyserman, D. (2011). Culture as situated cognition: Cultural mindsets, cultural fluency, and meaning making. European Review of Social Psychology, 22(1), 164214. https://doi.org/10.1080/10463283.2011.627187Google Scholar
Oyserman, D., & Lee, S. W. S. (2007). Priming “culture”: Culture as situated cognition. In Kitayama, S., & Cohen, D.. (Eds.), Handbook of cultural psychology (pp. 255–79). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Oyserman, D., & Lee, S. W. S. (2008). Does culture influence what and how we think? Effects of priming individualism and collectivism. Psychological Bulletin, 134(2), 311–42. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.311Google Scholar
Oyserman, D., Novin, S., Flinkenflögel, N., & Krabbendam, L. (2014). Integrating culture-as-situated-cognition and neuroscience prediction models. Culture and Brain, 2(1), 126. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40167-014-0016-6Google Scholar
Pascual-Leone, A., Amedi, A., Fregni, F., & Merabet, L. B. (2005). The plastic human brain cortex. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 28, 377401. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144216Google Scholar
Peyron, R., Laurent, B., & García-Larrea, L. (2000). Functional imaging of brain responses to pain. A review and meta-analysis (2000). Neurophysiologie Clinique, 30(5), 263–88. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0987-7053(00)00227-6Google Scholar
Shaw, C., & McEachern, J. (Eds.). (2001). Toward a theory of neuroplasticity. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Sui, J., & Han, S. (2007). Self-construal priming modulates neural substrates of self-awareness. Psychological Science, 18(10), 861–6. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01992.xGoogle Scholar
Sui, J., Zhu, Y., & Chiu, C.-y. (2007). Bicultural mind, self-construal, and self-and-mother reference effects: Consequences of cultural priming on recognition memory. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43(5), 818–24. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2006.08.005Google Scholar
Trafimow, D., Triandis, H. C., & Goto, S. G. (1991). Some tests of the distinction between the private self and the collective self. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 60(5), 649–55. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.60.5.649Google Scholar
United Nations. (2018). 2018 Revision of world urbanization prospects. New York, NY: Population Division of the United Nations Department of Economic and Social Affairs. https://population.un.org/wup/Publications/Files/WUP2018-Report.pdfGoogle Scholar
Utz, S. (2004). Self-construal and cooperation: Is the interdependent self more cooperative than the independent self? Self and Identity, 3(3), 177–90. https://doi.org/10.1080/13576500444000001Google Scholar
Varnum, M. E. W., Shi, Z., Chen, A., Qiu, J., & Han, S. (2014). When “your” reward is the same as “my” reward: Self-construal priming shifts neural responses to own vs. friends’ rewards. NeuroImage, 87, 164–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2013.10.042Google Scholar
Wang, C., Ma, Y., & Han, S. (2014). Self-construal priming modulates pain perception: Event-related potential evidence. Cognitive Neuroscience, 5(1), 39. http://doi.org/10.1080/17588928.2013.797388Google Scholar
Wang, C., Oyserman, D., Liu, Q., Li, H., & Han, S. (2013). Accessible cultural mind-set modulates default mode activity: Evidence for the culturally situated brain. Social Neuroscience, 8(3), 203–16. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2013.775966Google Scholar
Wang, C., Oyserman, D., Liu, Q., Li, H., & Han, S. (2015). Challenging emotional prejudice by changing self-concept: Priming independent self-construal reduces racial in-group bias in neural responses to other’s pain. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 10(9), 1195–201. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsv005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, F., Peng, K., Chechlacz, M., Humphreys, G. W., & Sui, J. (2017). The neural basis of independence versus interdependence orientations: A voxel-based morphometric analysis of brain volume. Psychological Science, 28(4), 519–29. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616689079Google Scholar
Wong, R. Y.-M., & Hong, Y.-y. (2005). Dynamic influences of culture on cooperation in the prisoner’s dilemma. Psychological Science, 16(6), 429–34. http://journals.sagepub.com/doi/full/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01552.xGoogle Scholar
Zaslansky, R., Sprecher, E., Tenke, C. E., Hemli, J. A., & Yarnitsky, D. (1996). The P300 in pain evoked potentials. Pain, 66(1), 3949. https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-3959(96)03020-5Google Scholar
Zhu, Y., Zhang, L., Fan, J., & Han, S. (2007). Neural basis of cultural influence on self-representation. NeuroImage, 34(3), 1310–16. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.08.047Google Scholar

References

Azuma, H. (1984). Secondary control as a heterogeneous category. American Psychologist, 39(9): 970–71. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.39.9.970Google Scholar
Bart, V. K. E., Sharavdorj, E., Bazarvaani, K., Munkhbat, T., Wenke, D., & Rieger, M. (2019). It was me: The use of sense of agency cues differs between culturesFrontiers in Psychology10, 650. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.00650Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F. (2008). Free will in scientific psychologyPerspectives on Psychological Science3(1), 1419. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1745-6916.2008.00057.xGoogle Scholar
Beyer, F., Sidarus, N., Bonicalzi, S., & Haggard, P. (2017). Beyond self-serving bias: Diffusion of responsibility reduces sense of agency and outcome monitoringSocial Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience12(1), 138–45. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw160Google Scholar
Blanke, O. (2012). Multisensory brain mechanisms of bodily self-consciousness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13, 556–71. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3292Google Scholar
Botvinick, M., & Cohen, J. (1998). Rubber hands ‘feel’ touch that eyes see. Nature, 391(6669), 756. https://doi.org/10.1038/35784Google Scholar
Brass, M., Lynn, M. T., Demanet, J., & Rigoni, D. (2013). Imaging volition: What the brain can tell us about the willExperimental Brain Research229(3), 301–12. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-013-3472-xGoogle Scholar
Brehm, S. S., & Brehm, J. W. (2013). Psychological reactance: A theory of freedom and control. Academic Press.Google Scholar
Buhrmann, T., & Di Paolo, E. (2017). The sense of agency – A phenomenological consequence of enacting sensorimotor schemesPhenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences16(2), 207–36. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-015-9446–7Google Scholar
Chambon, V., Sidarus, N., & Haggard, P. (2014). From action intentions to action effects: How does the sense of agency come about? Frontiers in Human Neuroscience8, 320. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00320Google Scholar
Chirkov, V. I., Ryan, R. M., & Sheldon, K. M. (Eds.). (2011). Human autonomy in cross-cultural context: Perspectives on the psychology of agency, freedom, and people’s well-being. Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-90-481-9667-8Google Scholar
Christensen, J. F., Di Costa, S., Beck, B., & Haggard, P. (2019). I just lost it! Fear and anger reduce the sense of agency: A study using intentional bindingExperimental Brain Research, 237(5), 1205–12. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-018-5461-6Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2016). Surfing uncertainty: Prediction, action, and the embodied mind. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
David, N. (2012). New frontiers in the neuroscience of the sense of agencyFrontiers in Human Neuroscience6, 161. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00161Google Scholar
David, N., Newen, A., & Vogeley, K. (2008). The “sense of agency” and its underlying cognitive and neural mechanismsConsciousness and Cognition17(2), 523–34. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2008.03.004Google Scholar
Deeley, Q., Oakley, D. A., Walsh, E., Bell, V., Mehta, M. A., & Halligan, P. W. (2014). Modelling psychiatric and cultural possession phenomena with suggestion and fMRICortex53, 107–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.01.004Google Scholar
Dogge, M., Schaap, M., Custers, R., Wegner, D. M., & Aarts, H. (2012). When moving without volition: Implied self-causation enhances binding strength between involuntary actions and effectsConsciousness and Cognition21(1), 501–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2011.10.014Google Scholar
Edge, H., & Suryani, L. (2002). A cross-cultural analysis of volition. Florida Philosophical Review, 2 (2), 5672.Google Scholar
Fallin, M., Whooley, O., & Barker, K. K. (2018). Criminalizing the brain: Neurocriminology and the production of strategic ignorance. BioSocieties, 1–25. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1057/s41292-018-0135-yGoogle Scholar
Fausey, C. M., Long, B. L., Inamori, A., & Boroditsky, L. (2010). Constructing agency: The role of language. Frontiers of Psychology, 1, 162. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2010.00162Google Scholar
Feldman, G. (2017). Making sense of agency: Belief in free will as a unique and important constructSocial and Personality Psychology Compass11(1), e12293. https://doi.org/10.1111/spc3.12293Google Scholar
Frith, C. D. (2014). Action, agency and responsibilityNeuropsychologia55, 137–42. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2013.09.007Google Scholar
Gallagher, S. (2017). Enactivist interventions: Rethinking the mind. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oso/9780198794325.001.0001Google Scholar
Gailliot, M. T., & Baumeister, R. F. (2007). The physiology of willpower: Linking blood glucose to self-controlPersonality and Social Psychology Review11(4), 303–27. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868307303030Google Scholar
Giddens, A. (1979). Central problems in social theory: Actions, structure and contradiction in social analysis. Palgrave. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-349-16161-4_3Google Scholar
Gonzalez-Franco, M., & Lanier, J. (2017). Model of illusions and virtual reality. Frontiers in Psychology8, 1125. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01125Google Scholar
Greely, H. T., & Farahany, N. A. (2019). Neuroscience and the criminal justice systemAnnual Review of Criminology2, 451–71. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-criminol-011518-024433Google Scholar
Grush, R. (2004). The emulation theory of representation: Motor control, imagery, and perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27(3), 377–96. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X04000093Google Scholar
Haggard, P. (2017). Sense of agency in the human brainNature Reviews Neuroscience18(4), 196207. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2017.14Google Scholar
Haggard, P., & Eitam, B. (Eds.). (2015). The sense of agency. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780190267278.001.0001Google Scholar
Hilgard, E. R. (1986). Divided consciousness: Multiple controls in human thought and action (Expanded ed.). Wiley.Google Scholar
Ismail, M. A. F., & Shimada, S. (2016). ‘Robot’ hand illusion under delayed visual feedback: Relationship between the senses of ownership and agencyPLoS ONE11(7), e0159619. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0159619Google Scholar
Job, V., Dweck, C. S., & Walton, G. M. (2010). Ego depletion – Is it all in your head? Implicit theories about willpower affect self-regulationPsychological Science21(11), 1686–93. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610384745Google Scholar
Johansson, P., Hall, L., Sikström, S., & Olsson, A. (2005). Failure to detect mismatches between intention and outcome in a simple decision taskScience310(5745), 116–19. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1111709Google Scholar
Juarrero, A. (1999). Dynamics in action: Intentional behavior as a complex system. MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/2528.001.0001Google Scholar
Kalckert, A., & Ehrsson, H. H. (2012). Moving a rubber hand that feels like your own: A dissociation of ownership and agencyFrontiers in Human Neuroscience6, 40. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00040Google Scholar
Kirmayer, L. J. (1990). Resistance, reactance, and reluctance to change: A cognitive attributional approach to strategic interventionsJournal of Cognitive Psychotherapy4(2), 83104.Google Scholar
Kirmayer, L. J., & Gómez-Carrillo, A. (2019). Agency, embodiment and enactment in psychosomatic theory and practiceMedical Humanities, 45(2), 169–82. https://doi.org/10.1136/medhum-2018-011618Google Scholar
Kirmayer, L. J., & Ramstead, M. J. D. (2017). Embodiment and enactment in cultural psychiatry. In Durt, C., Fuchs, T., & Tewes, C. (Eds.), Embodiment, enaction, and culture: Investigating the constitution of the shared world (pp. 397422). MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262035552.003.0021Google Scholar
Kokkinara, E., Kilteni, K., Blom, K. J., & Slater, M. (2016). First person perspective of seated participants over a walking virtual body leads to illusory agency over the walkingScientific Reports6, 28879. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep28879Google Scholar
Kpanake, L. (2018). Cultural concepts of the person and mental health in Africa. Transcultural Psychiatry55(2), 198218. https://doi.org/10.1177/1363461517749435Google Scholar
Kushnir, T. (2018). The developmental and cultural psychology of free willPhilosophy Compass13(11), e12529. https://doi.org/10.1111/phc3.12529Google Scholar
Lee, B. O. (2016). Transformation in dang-ki healing: The embodied self and perceived legitimacyCulture, Medicine, and Psychiatry40(3), 422–49. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11013-016-9497-4Google Scholar
Lee, B.-O., & Kirmayer, L. J. (2019). Dang-ki healing: An embodied relational healing practice in Singapore. Transcultural Psychiatry. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F1363461519858448Google Scholar
Lee, B. O., Kirmayer, L. J., & Groleau, D. (2010). Therapeutic processes and perceived helpfulness of dang-ki (Chinese shamanism) from the symbolic healing perspectiveCulture, Medicine, and Psychiatry34(1), 56105.Google Scholar
Lenggengager, B., Tadi, T., Metzinger, T., & Blanke, O. (2007). Video ergo sum: Manipulating bodily self-consciousness. Science, 317(5841), 1096–9. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1143439Google Scholar
Libet, B., Gleason, C. A., Wright, E. W., & Pearl, D. K. (1983). Time of conscious intention to act in relation to onset of cerebral activity (readiness-potential): The unconscious initiation of freely voluntary act. Brain, 106(3), 623–42. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/106.3.623Google Scholar
Lynn, S. J., Rhue, J. W., & Weekes, J. R. (1990). Hypnotic involuntariness: A social cognitive analysis. Psychological Review, 97(2), 169–84. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.97.2.169Google Scholar
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (2010). Cultures and selves: A cycle of mutual constitutionPerspectives on Psychological Science5(4), 420–30. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691610375557Google Scholar
Maurer, C. W., LaFaver, K., Ameli, R., Epstein, S. A., Hallett, M., & Horovitz, S. G. (2016). Impaired self-agency in functional movement disorders: A resting-state fMRI studyNeurology87(6), 564–70. https://doi.org/10.1212/WNL.0000000000002940Google Scholar
Miller, G. (2016, March 1). The brain gets its day in court. The Atlantic. www.theatlantic.com/science/archive/2016/03/neurolaw-brain-scans-court/471615Google Scholar
Miller, G. A., Galanter, E., & Pribram, K. H. (1960). Plans and the structure of behavior. Holt, Reinhart, Winston. https://doi.org/10.1037/10039-000Google Scholar
Miller, P. J., Koven, M., & Lin, S. (2011). Language socialization and narrative. In Durant, A., Ochs, E., & Schieffelin, B. (Eds.), The handbook of language socialization (pp. 190208). Wiley. https://doi.org/10.1002/9781444342901.ch8Google Scholar
Mischel, W., Ayduk, O., Berman, M. G., Casey, B. J., Gotlib, I. H., Jonides, J., Kross, E., Teslovich, T., Wilson, N. L., Zayas, V., & Shoda, Y. (2010). ‘Willpower’ over the life span: Decomposing self-regulationSocial Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience6(2), 252–6. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsq081Google Scholar
Moore, J. W. (2016). What is the sense of agency and why does it matter? Frontiers in Psychology7, 1272. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01272Google Scholar
Moretto, G., Walsh, E., & Haggard, P. (2011). Experience of agency and sense of responsibilityConsciousness and Cognition20(4), 1847–54. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2011.08.014Google Scholar
Nahab, F. B., Kundu, P., Maurer, C., Shen, Q., & Hallett, M. (2017). Impaired sense of agency in functional movement disorders: An fMRI studyPLoS ONE12(4), e0172502. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0172502Google Scholar
Oerter, R., Oerter, R., Agostiani, H., Kim, H. O., & Wibowo, S. (1996). The concept of human nature in East Asia: Etic and emic characteristicsCulture & Psychology2(1), 951. https://doi.org/10.1177/1354067X9621002Google Scholar
Papies, E. K., & Aarts, H. (2016). Automatic self-regulation: From habit to goal pursuit. In Vohs, K. & Baumeister, R.F. (Eds.), Handbook of self-regulation: Research, theory, and applications (3rd ed., pp. 203–22). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Polito, V., Barnier, A. J., Woody, E. Z., & Connors, M. H. (2014). Measuring agency change across the domain of hypnosisPsychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 1(1), 319. https://doi.org/10.1037/cns0000010Google Scholar
Powers, W. T. (1973). Behavior: The control of perception. Aldine.Google Scholar
Raine, A. (2019). A neurodevelopmental perspective on male violence. Infant Mental Health 40(1), 8497. https://doi.org/10.1002/imhj.21761Google Scholar
Ramstead, M. J., Veissière, S. P., & Kirmayer, L. J. (2016). Cultural affordances: Scaffolding local worlds through shared intentionality and regimes of attentionFrontiers in Psychology7, 1090. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01090Google Scholar
Rose, N. (1998). Inventing our selves: Psychology, power, and personhood. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Saigle, V., Dubljević, V., & Racine, E. (2018). The impact of a landmark neuroscience study on free will: A qualitative analysis of articles using Libet and colleagues’ methodsAJOB Neuroscience9(1), 2941. https://doi.org/10.1080/21507740.2018.1425756Google Scholar
Seligman, R., & Kirmayer, L. J. (2008). Dissociative experience and cultural neuroscience: Narrative, metaphor and mechanismCulture, Medicine and Psychiatry32(1), 3164. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11013-007-9077-8Google Scholar
Seth, A. K., Suzuki, K., & Critchley, H. D. (2012). An interoceptive predictive coding model of conscious presence. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 395. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00395Google Scholar
Slater, M., Perez-Marcos, D., Ehrsson, H. H., & Sanchez-Vives, M. V. (2009). Inducing illusory ownership of a virtual body. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 3(2), 214–20. https://doi.org/10.3389%2Fneuro.01.029.2009Google Scholar
Synofzik, M., Vosgerau, G., & Newen, A. (2008). Beyond the comparator model: A multifactorial two-step account of agencyConsciousness and Cognition17(1), 219–39. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.concog.2007.03.010Google Scholar
Tilly, C. (2006). Why? What happens when people give reasons … and why. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Veissière, S. P., Constant, A., Ramstead, M. J., Friston, K. J., & Kirmayer, L. J. (2020). Thinking through other minds: A variational approach to cognition and cultureBehavioral and Brain Sciences43, e90: 175. doi:10.1017/ S0140525X19001213Google Scholar
Wegner, D. M. (2003). The mind’s best trick: How we experience conscious will. Trends in Cognitive Science, 7(2), 65–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613%2803%2900002-0Google Scholar
Wegner, D. M., Gilbert, D., & Wheatley, T. (2017). The illusion of conscious will. MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/11151.001.0001Google Scholar
Weisz, J. R., Rothbaum, F. M., & Blackburn, T. C. (1984). Standing out and standing in: The psychology of control in America and JapanAmerican Psychologist39(9), 955–69. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.39.9.955Google Scholar
Wikan, U. (1990). Managing turbulent hearts: A Balinese formula for living. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Amodio, D. M. (2014). The neuroscience of prejudice and stereotyping. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 15(10), 670–82. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn3800Google Scholar
Dikker, S., Silbert, L. J., Hasson, U., & Zevin, J. D. (2014). On the same wavelength: Predictable language enhances speaker–listener brain-to-brain synchrony in posterior superior temporal gyrus. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(18), 6267–72. https://doi.org/10.1523/jneurosci.3796-13.2014Google Scholar
Fotouhi, B., Momeni, N., Allen, B., & Nowak, M. A. (2019). Evolution of cooperation on large networks with community structure. Journal of the Royal Society Interface, 16(152), 20180677. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsif.2018.0677Google Scholar
Harris, L. T., Lee, V. K., Capestany, B. H., & Cohen, A. O. (2014). Assigning economic value to people results in dehumanization brain response. Journal of Neuroscience, Psychology, and Economics, 7(3), 151–63. https://doi.org/10.1037/npe0000020Google Scholar
Koike, T., Tanabe, H. C., Okazaki, S., Nakagawa, E., Sasaki, A. T., Shimada, K., Sugawara, S. K., Takahashi, H. K., Yoshihara, K., Bosch-Bayard, J., & Sadato, N. (2016). Neural substrates of shared attention as social memory: A hyperscanning functional magnetic resonance imaging study. NeuroImage, 125, 401–12. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.09.076Google Scholar
Leander, N. P., Chartrand, T. L., & Wood, W. (2011). Mind your mannerisms: Behavioral mimicry elicits stereotype conformity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(1), 195201. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2010.09.002Google Scholar
Morrison, S., Decety, J., & Molenberghs, P. (2012). The neuroscience of group membership. Neuropsychologia, 50(8), 2114–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2012.05.014Google Scholar
Nguyen, M., Vanderwal, T., & Hasson, U. (2019). Shared understanding of narratives is correlated with shared neural responses. NeuroImage, 184, 161–70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.09.010Google Scholar
Redcay, E., & Schilbach, L. (2019). Using second-person neuroscience to elucidate the mechanisms of social interaction. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 20(8), 495505. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-019-0179-4Google Scholar
Swencionis, J. K., & Fiske, S. T. (2014). How social neuroscience can inform theories of social comparison. Neuropsychologia, 56, 140–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.01.009Google Scholar
Veissière, S. P., Constant, A., Ramstead, M. J., Friston, K. J., & Kirmayer, L. J. (2020). Thinking through other minds: A variational approach to cognition and culture. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 43, e90, 175. https://10.1017/S0140525X19001213Google Scholar
Yeshurun, Y., Swanson, S., Simony, E., Chen, J., Lazaridi, C., Honey, C. J., & Hasson, U. (2017). Same story, different story: The neural representation of interpretive frameworks. Psychological Science, 28(3), 307–19. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616682029Google Scholar

References

Anderson, M. L. (2010). Neural reuse: A fundamental organizational sprinciple of the brain. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(4), 245–66. https://doi.org/https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X10000853Google Scholar
Bellucci, G., Chernyak, S. V., Goodyear, K., Eickhoff, S. B., & Krueger, F. (2017). Neural signatures of trust in reciprocity: A coordinate-based meta-analysisHuman Brain Mapping, 38(3), 1233–48. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23451Google Scholar
Bender, A., & Beller, S. (2016). Current perspectives on cognitive diversity. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 509. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00509Google Scholar
Bickart, K. C., Wright, C. I., Dautoff, R. J., Dickerson, R. J., & Barrett, L. F. (2011). Amygdala volume and social network size in humans. Nature Neuroscience, 14(2), 163–4. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.2724Google Scholar
Bourdieu, P. (1977). Outline of a theory of practice (Nice, R., Trans.). Cambridge University Press. (Original work published 1972). https://doi.org/10.1017/CBO9780511812507Google Scholar
Bourdieu, P. (1984). Distinction: A social critique of the judgment of taste (Nice, R., Trans.). Harvard University Press. (Original work published 1979)Google Scholar
Bourgois, P. (1995). In search of respect: Selling crack in el barrio. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Brothers, L. (1990). The social brain: A project for integrating primate behavior and neurophysiology in a new domain. Concepts in Neuroscience, 1, 2751.Google Scholar
Bruineberg, J., Kiverstein, J., & Rietveld, E. (2018). The anticipating brain is not a scientist: The free-energy principle from an ecological-enactive perspective. Synthese, 195(6), 2417–44. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-016-1239-1Google Scholar
Burbank, V. K. (2012). Life history and real life: An example of neuroanthropology in aboriginal Australia. Annals of Anthropological Practice, 36(1), 149–66. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2153-9588.2012.01097.xGoogle Scholar
Calvo-Merino, B., Glaser, D. E., Grèzes, J., Passingham, R. E., & Haggard, P. (2004). Action observation and acquired motor skills: An fMRI study with expert dancers. Cerebral Cortex, 15(8),1243–9. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhi007Google Scholar
Carruthers, P. (2006). The architecture of the mind: Massive modularity and the flexibility of thought. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199207077.001.0001Google Scholar
Catmur, C., Walsh, V., & Heyes, C. (2007). Sensorimotor learning configures the human mirror system. Current Biology, 17(17), 1527–31. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2007.08.006Google Scholar
Chen, P., & Hong, W. (2018). Neural circuit mechanisms of social behavior. Neuron, 98(1), 1630. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2018.02.026Google Scholar
Chiao, J. Y., Cheon, B. K., Pornpattananangkul, N., Mrazek, A. J., & Blizinsky, K. D. (2013). Cultural neuroscience: Progress and promise. Psychological Inquiry, 24(1), 119. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2013.752715Google Scholar
Chiao, J. Y., Li, S.-C., Seligman, R., & Turner, R. (Eds.). (2016). The Oxford handbook of cultural neuroscience. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199357376.001.0001Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2013). Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(3), 181204. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X12000477Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2015). Surfing uncertainty: Prediction, action, and the embodied mind. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780190217013.001.0001Google Scholar
Constant, A., Ramstead, M. J. D., Veissière, S. P. L., Campbell, J. O., & Friston, K. J. (2018). A variational approach to niche construction. Journal of the Royal Society Interface, 15(141), 20170685. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsif.2017.0685Google Scholar
Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (1997, January 13). Evolutionary psychology: A primer. www.cep.ucsb.edu/primer.html.Google Scholar
Crenshaw, K. (1989). Demarginalizing the intersection of race and sex: A black feminist critique of antidiscrimination doctrine, feminist theory and antiracist politics. University of Chicago Legal Forum, 1989(1), 139–67. https://chicagounbound.uchicago.edu/uclf/vol1989/iss1/8Google Scholar
Crossley, N. (2013). Habit and habitus. Body & Society, 19(2–3), 136–61. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F1357034X12472543Google Scholar
de Vries, G. J., & Forger, N. G. (2015). Sex differences in the brain: A whole body perspectiveBiology of Sex Differences, 6(1), 15. https://doi.org/10.1186%2Fs13293-015-0032-zGoogle Scholar
Decety, J., Jackson, P. L., Sommerville, J. A., Chaminade, T., & Meltzoff, A. N. (2004). The neural bases of cooperation and competition: An fMRI investigation. NeuroImage, 23(2), 744–51. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.05.025Google Scholar
Dikker, S., Wan, L., Davidesco, I., Kaggen, L., Oostrik, M., McClintock, J., Rowland, J., Michalareas, G., Van Bavel, J. J., Ding, M., & Poeppel, D. (2017). Brain-to-brain synchrony tracks real-world dynamic group interactions in the classroom. Current Biology, 27(9), 1375–80. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.04.002Google Scholar
Di Paolo, E., & De Jaegher, H. (2012). The interactive brain hypothesis. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 163. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00163.Google Scholar
Duque, J. F. D., Turner, R., Lewis, E. D., & Egan, G. (2009). Neuroanthropology: A humanistic science for the study of the culture-brain nexusSocial Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5(2–3), 138–47. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsp024Google Scholar
Downey, G. (2007). Producing pain: Techniques and technologies in no-holds-barred fighting. Social Studies of Science, 37(2), 201–26. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0306312706072174Google Scholar
Downey, G. (2010a). “Practice without theory”: A neuroanthropological perspective on embodied learning. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, 16(s1), S22S40. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9655.2010.01608Google Scholar
Downey, G. (2010b). Throwing like a Brazilian: On ineptness and a skill-shaped body. In Sands, R. & Sands, L. (Eds.), Anthropology of sport and human movement: A biocultural perspective (pp. 297326). Lexington Books.Google Scholar
Downey, G. (2012). Balancing across cultures: Sensory plasticity. In Lende, D. H. & Downey, G. (Eds.), The encultured brain: Introduction to neuroanthropology (pp. 169–94). MIT Press.Google Scholar
Downey, G. (2016). Sensory enculturation and neuroanthropology: The case of human echolocation. In Chiao, J. Y., Li, S.-C., Seligman, R., & Turner, R. (Eds.), Oxford handbook of cultural neuroscience (pp. 4157). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199357376.013.23Google Scholar
Downey, G., & Lende, D. H. (2012a). Neuroanthropology and the encultured brain. In Lende, D. H. & Downey, G. (Eds.), The encultured brain: An introduction to neuroanthropology (pp. 2365). MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress%2F9219.003.0004Google Scholar
Downey, G., & Lende, D. H. (2012b). Evolution and the brain. In Lende, D. H. & Downey, G. (Eds.), The encultured brain: An introduction to neuroanthropology (pp. 103–37). MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9219.003.0006Google Scholar
Dressler, W. W. (2017). Culture and the individual: Theory and method of cultural consonance. Routledge.Google Scholar
Dressler, W. W., Balieiro, M. C., & dos Santos, J. E. (2012). Cultural consonance, consciousness, and depression: Genetic moderating effects on the psychological mediators of culture. In Lende, D. H. & Downey, G. (Eds.), The encultured brain: An introduction to neuroanthropology (pp. 363–88). MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9219.003.0018Google Scholar
Dressler, W. W., & Bindon, J. R. (2000). The health consequences of cultural consonance: Cultural dimensions of lifestyle, social support, and arterial blood pressure in an African American community. American Anthropologist, 102(2), 244–60. https://doi.org/10.1525/aa.2000.102.2.244Google Scholar
Dressler, W. W., Borges, C. D., Balieiro, M. C., & dos Santos, J. E. (2005). Measuring cultural consonance: Examples with special reference to measurement theory in anthropology. Field Methods, 17(4), 331–55. https://doi.org/10.1177/1525822X05279899Google Scholar
Dumas, G., Nadel, J., Soussignan, R., Martinerie, J., & Garnero, L. (2010). Inter-brain synchronization during social interactionPLoS ONE5(8), e12166. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0012166Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. M. (1993). Coevolution of neocortical size, group size and language in humans. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16(4), 681–94. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X00032325Google Scholar
Engemann, D. A., Bzdok, D., Eickhoff, S. B., Vogeley, K., & Schilbach, L. (2012). Games people play: Toward an enactive view of cooperation in social neuroscience. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 148. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00148Google Scholar
Fausto-Sterling, A. (2019). Gender/sex, sexual orientation, and identity are in the body: How did they get there? Journal of Sex Research, 56(4–5), 529–55. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2019.1581883Google Scholar
Fodor, J. A. (1983). The modularity of mind. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Friston, K. (2009). The free-energy principle: A rough guide to the brain? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(7), 293301. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.04.005Google Scholar
Friston, K. (2010). The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(2), 127–38. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2787Google Scholar
Friston, K., Fortier, M., & Friedman, D.A. (2018). Of woodlice and men: A Bayesian account of cognition, life and consciousness. An interview with Karl Friston. ALIUS Bulletin, 2, 1743.Google Scholar
Frith, C. D. (2007). The social brain? Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 362(1480), 671–8. https://doi.org/10.1098%2Frstb.2006.2003Google Scholar
Frith, C., & Frith, U. (2000). The physiological basis of theory of mind: Functional neuroimaging studies. In Baron-Cohen, S., Tager-Flusberg, H., & Cohen, D. J. (Eds.), Understanding other minds: Perspectives from developmental cognitive neuroscience (2nd ed., pp. 335–56). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fuentes, A. (2016). The extended evolutionary synthesis, ethnography, and the human niche: Toward an integrated anthropology. Current Anthropology 57(S13), S13S26. https://doi.org/10.1086/685684Google Scholar
Gobbini, M. I., & Haxby, J. V. (2007). Neural systems for recognition of familiar facesNeuropsychologia, 45(1), 3241. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.04.015Google Scholar
Gravlee, C. C. (2009). How race becomes biology: Embodiment of social inequality. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 139(1), 4757. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.20983Google Scholar
Han, S., & Northoff, G. (2008). Culture-sensitive neural substrates of human cognition: A transcultural neuroimaging approach. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(8), 646–54. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2456Google Scholar
Hari, R., Henriksson, L., Malinen, S., & Parkkonen, L. (2015). Centrality of social interaction in human brain function. Neuron, 88(1), 181–93. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.022Google Scholar
Hein, G., & Singer, T. (2008). I feel how you feel but not always: The empathic brain and its modulationCurrent Opinion in Neurobiology, 18(2), 153–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2008.07.012Google Scholar
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 6183. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0999152XGoogle Scholar
Humphrey, N. K. (1976). The social function of intellect. In Bateson, P. P. G. & Hinde, R. A. (Eds.), Growing points in ethology (pp. 303–17). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hutchins, E. (1995). Cognition in the wild. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Hutchins, E. (2011). Enculturating the supersized mind. Philosophical Studies, 152(3), 437–46.Google Scholar
Keiflin, R., & Janak, P. H. (2017). Error-driven learning: Dopamine signals more than value-based errors. Current Biology, 27(24), R1321R1324. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2017.10.043Google Scholar
Kennedy, D. P., & Adolphs, R. (2012). The social brain in psychiatric and neurological disordersTrends in Cognitive Sciences, 16(11), 559–72. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2012.09.006Google Scholar
Kirschner, S., & Ilari, B. (2014). Joint drumming in Brazilian and German preschool children: Cultural differences in rhythmic entrainment, but no prosocial effectsJournal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 45(1), 137–66. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0022022113493139Google Scholar
Koban, L., Jepman, M., Geuter, S., & Wager, T. D. (2017). What’s in a word? How instructions, suggestions, and social information change pain and emotion. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 81(Part A), 2942. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.02.014Google Scholar
Lende, D. H. (2012). Poverty poisons the brain. Annals of Anthropological Practice, 36(1), 183201. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2153-9588.2012.01099.xGoogle Scholar
Lende, D. H., & Downey, G. (Eds.). (2012a). The encultured brain: An introduction to neuroanthropology. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Lende, D. H., & Downey, G. (2012b). Neuroanthropology and its applications: An introduction. Annals of Anthropological Practice, 36(1), 125. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2153-9588.2012.01090.xGoogle Scholar
Lock, M. (1993). Encounters with aging: Mythologies of menopause in Japan and North America. University of California Press.Google Scholar
MacLeod, J. (2018). Ain’t no makin’ it: Aspirations and attainment in a low-income neighborhood. (3rd ed.). Routledge.Google Scholar
Merchant, H., Grahn, J., Trainor, L., Rohrmeier, M., & Fitch, W. T. (2015). Finding the beat: A neural perspective across humans and non-human primates. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 370(1664), 20140093. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2014.0093Google Scholar
Monfardini, E., Redouté, J., Hadj-Bouziane, F., Hynaux, C., Fradin, J. Huguet, P., Costes, N., & Meunier, M. (2016). Others’ sheer presence boosts brain activity in the attention (but not the motivation) networkCerebral Cortex, 26(6), 2427–39. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhv067Google Scholar
Muthukrishna, M., & Henrich, J. (2019). A problem in theoryNature Human Behaviour, 3, 221–9. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41562-018-0522-1Google Scholar
Nguyen, M., Vanderwal, T., & Hasson, U. (2019). Shared understanding of narratives is correlated with shared neural responsesNeuroImage, 184, 161–70. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.09.010Google Scholar
Nummenmaa, L., Lahnakoski, J. M., & Glerean, E. (2018). Sharing the social world via intersubject neural synchronisationCurrent Opinion in Psychology, 24, 714. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2018.02.021Google Scholar
Ochsner, K. N., & Lieberman, M. D. (2001). The emergence of social cognitive neuroscience. American Psychologist, 56(9), 717–34. https://doi.org/10.1037/0003-066X.56.9.717Google Scholar
Panter-Brick, C., Lende, D., & Kohrt, B. A. (2012). Children in global adversity: Physical, mental, behavioral, and symbolic dimensions of health. In King, R. & Maholmes, V. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of poverty and child development (pp. 603–21). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199769100.013.0033Google Scholar
Parkinson, C., Kleinbaum, A. M., & Wheatley, T. (2018). Similar neural responses predict friendshipNature Communications, 9(1), 332. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41467-017-02722-7Google Scholar
Parkinson, C., & Wheatley, T. (2015). The repurposed social brainTrends in Cognitive Sciences, 19(3), 133–41. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2015.01.003Google Scholar
Peacock, V. (2016). Academic precarity as hierarchical dependence in the Max Planck Society. HAU: Journal of Ethnographic Theory, 6(1), 95119. https://doi.org/10.14318/hau6.1.006Google Scholar
Pitts-Taylor, V. (2016). The brain’s body: Neuroscience and corporeal politics. Duke University Press. https://doi.org/10.1215/9780822374374Google Scholar
Racine, T. P., & Carpendale, J. I. M. (2007). The role of shared practice in joint attention. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 25(1), 325. https://doi.org/10.1348/026151006X119756Google Scholar
Ramstead, M. J. D., Badcock, P. B., & Friston, K. J. (2018). Answering Schrödinger’s question: A free-energy formulation. Physics of Life Reviews, 24, 116. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.plrev.2017.09.001Google Scholar
Ramstead, M. J. D., Constant, A., Badcock, P. B., & Friston, K. J. (2019). Variational ecology and the physics of sentient systems. Physics of Life Reviews, 31, 188205. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.plrev.2018.12.002Google Scholar
Ramstead, M. J. D., Kirchhoff, M. D., Constant, A., & Friston, K. J. (2019). Multiscale integration: Beyond internalism and externalism. Synthese. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11229-019-02115-xGoogle Scholar
Ramstead, M. J. D., Veissière, S. P. L., & Kirmayer, L. J. (2016). Cultural affordances: Scaffolding local worlds through shared intentionality and regimes of attention. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1090. https://doi.org/10.3389%2Ffpsyg.2016.01090Google Scholar
Rilling, J. K. (2008). Neuroscientific approaches and applications within anthropology. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 137(S47), 232. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.20947Google Scholar
Rizzolatti, G., & Craighero, L. (2004). The mirror-neuron systemAnnual Review of Neuroscience, 27, 169–92. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.27.070203.144230Google Scholar
Roepstorff, A. (2013). Why am I not just lovin’ cultural neuroscience? Toward a slow science of cultural difference. Psychological Inquiry, 24(1), 61–3. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2013.768058Google Scholar
Roepstorff, A., & Frith, C. (2004). What’s at the top in the top-down control of action? Script-sharing and ‘top-top’ control of action in cognitive experiments. Psychological Research, 68(2–3), 189–98. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-003-0155-4Google Scholar
Roepstorff, A., Niewöhner, J., & Beck, S. (2010). Enculturing brains through patterned practices. Neural Networks, 23(8–9), 1051–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neunet.2010.08.002Google Scholar
Rule, N. O., Freeman, J. B., & Ambady, N. (2013). Culture in social neuroscience: A review. Social Neuroscience, 8(1), 310. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2012.695293Google Scholar
Sallet, J., Mars, R. B., Noonan, M. P., Andersson, J. L., O’Reilly, J. X., Jbabdi, S., Croxson, P. L., Jenkinson, M., Miller, K. L., & Rushworth, M. F. S. (2011). Social network size affects neural circuits in macaques. Science, 334(6056), 697700. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1210027Google Scholar
Schultz, W., Dayan, P., & Montague, P. R. (1997). A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science, 275(5306), 1593–9. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.275.5306.1593Google Scholar
Schurz, M., Radua, J., Aichhorn, M., Richlan, F., & Perner, J. (2014). Fractionating theory of mind: A meta-analysis of functional brain imaging studiesNeuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 42, 934. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2014.01.009Google Scholar
Seeley, W. W., Menon, V., Schatzberg, A. F., Keller, J., Glover, G. H., Kenna, H., Reiss, A. L., & Greicius, M. D. (2007). Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive controlJournal of Neuroscience, 27(9), 2349–56. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5587-06.2007Google Scholar
Shipp, S. (2016). Neural elements for predictive coding. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, 1792. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01792Google Scholar
Shore, B. (1996). Culture in mind: Cognition, culture, and the problem of meaning. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Spunt, R. P., & Adolphs, R. (2017). A new look at domain specificity: Insights from social neuroscienceNature Reviews Neuroscience, 18(9), 559–67. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn.2017.76Google Scholar
Stotz, K. (2010). Human nature and cognitive-developmental niche constructionPhenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 9(4), 483501. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11097-010-9178-7Google Scholar
Super, C. M., & Harkness, S. (1986). The developmental niche: A conceptualization at the interface of child and culture. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 9(4), 545–69. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F016502548600900409Google Scholar
Tarr, B., Launay, J., Cohen, E., & Dunbar, R. (2015). Synchrony and exertion during dance independently raise pain threshold and encourage social bondingBiology Letters, 11(10), 20150767. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2015.0767Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2014). Joint attention as social cognition. In Moore, C. & Dunham, P. J. (Eds.), Joint attention: Its origins and role in development (pp. 103–30). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2019). Becoming human: A theory of ontogeny. Belknap Press.Google Scholar
Van Overwalle, F., & Baetens, K (2009). Understanding others’ actions and goals by mirror and mentalizing systems: A meta-analysisNeuroImage, 48(3), 564–84. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.06.009Google Scholar
Varela, F. J., Thompson, E., & Rosch, E. (1991). The embodied mind: Cognitive science and human experience. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Veissière, S. P. L., & Stendel, M. (2018). Hypernatural monitoring: A social rehearsal account of smartphone addiction. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 141. https://doi.org/10.3389%2Ffpsyg.2018.00141Google Scholar
Wacquant, L. (2016). A concise genealogy and anatomy of habitus. Sociological Review, 64(1), 6472. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-954X.12356Google Scholar
Wallot, S., Mitkidis, P., McGraw, J. J., & Roepstorff, A. (2016). Beyond synchrony: Joint action in a complex production task reveals beneficial effects of decreased interpersonal synchrony. PLoS ONE, 11(12), e0168306. https://doi.org/10.1371%2Fjournal.pone.0168306Google Scholar
Wang, H., Braun, C., & Enck, P. (2017). How the brain reacts to social stress (exclusion): A scoping reviewNeuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 80, 80–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.05.012Google Scholar
Warin, M., Moore, V., Davies, M., & Ulijaszek, S. (2016). Epigenetics and obesity: The reproduction of habitus through intercellular and social environments. Body & Society, 22(4), 5378. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F1357034X15590485Google Scholar
Westermann, G., Mareschal, D., Johnson, M. H., Sirois, S., Spratling, M. W., & Thomas, M. S. C. (2007). Neuroconstructivism. Developmental Science, 10(1), 7583. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00567.xGoogle Scholar
Wexler, B. E. (2006). Brain and culture: Neurobiology, ideology, and social change. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Worthman, C. M. (2009). Habits of the heart: Life history and the developmental neuroendocrinology of emotion. American Journal of Human Biology, 21(6), 772–81. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajhb.20966Google Scholar
Worthman, C. M. (2010). The ecology of human development: Evolving models for cultural psychology. Journal of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 41(4), 546–62. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F0022022110362627Google Scholar
Xygalatas, D., Konvalinka, I., Bulbulia, J., & Roepstorff, A. (2011). Quantifying collective effervescence: Heart-rate dynamics at a fire-walking ritual. Communicative & Integrative Biology, 4(6), 735–8. https://doi.org/10.4161/cib.17609Google Scholar
Young, I. M. (1980). Throwing like a girl: A phenomenology of feminine body comportment, motility and spatiality. Human Studies, 3(2), 137–56. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF02331805Google Scholar

References

Aplin, L. M., Farine, D. R., Morand-Ferron, J., Cockburn, A., Thornton, A., & Sheldon, B. C. (2015). Experimentally induced innovations lead to persistent culture via conformity in wild birds. Nature, 518(7540), 538–41. https://doi.org/10.1038%2Fnature13998Google Scholar
Asch, S. E. (1956). Studies of independence and conformity: I. A minority of one against a unanimous majority. Psychological Monographs: General and Applied, 70(9), 170. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0093718Google Scholar
Balzer, M. M. (1980). The route to eternity: Cultural persistence and change in Siberian Khanty burial ritual. Arctic Anthropology, 17(1), 7789. www.jstor.org/stable/40315968Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1991). Social cognitive theory of self-regulation. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 50(2), 248–87. https://doi.org/10.1016/0749-5978(91)90022-LGoogle Scholar
Bechara, A., Tranel, D., & Damasio, H. (2000). Characterization of the decision-making deficit of patients with ventromedial prefrontal cortex lesions. Brain, 123(11), 2189–202. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/123.11.2189Google Scholar
Beevers, C. G., Ellis, A. J., Wells, T. T., & McGeary, J. E. (2010). Serotonin transporter gene promoter region polymorphism and selective processing of emotional images. Biological Psychology, 83(3), 260–5. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.08.007Google Scholar
Benningfield, M. M., Blackford, J. U., Ellsworth, M. E., Samanez-Larkin, G. R., Martin, P. R., Cowan, R. L., & Zald, D. H. (2014). Caudate responses to reward anticipation associated with delay discounting behavior in healthy youth. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 7, 4352. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dcn.2013.10.009Google Scholar
Berthoz, S., Armony, J. L., Blair, R. J. R., & Dolan, R. J. (2002). An fMRI study of intentional and unintentional (embarrassing) violations of social norms. Brain, 125(8), 1696–708. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awf190Google Scholar
Blair, R. J. R., Morris, J. S., Frith, C. D., Perrett, D. I., & Dolan, R. J. (1999). Dissociable neural responses to facial expressions of sadness and anger. Brain, 122(5), 883–93. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/122.5.883Google Scholar
Boesch, C., Marchesi, P., Marchesi, N., Fruth, B., & Joulian, F. (1994). Is nut cracking in wild chimpanzees a cultural behaviour? Journal of Human Evolution, 26(4), 325–38. https://doi.org/10.1006/jhev.1994.1020Google Scholar
Buckholtz, J. W., Asplund, C. L., Dux, P. E., Zald, D. H., Gore, J. C., Jones, O. D., & Marois, R. (2008). The neural correlates of third-party punishment. Neuron, 60(5), 930–40. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.10.016Google Scholar
Buckholtz, J. W., & Marois, R. (2012). The roots of modern justice: Cognitive and neural foundations of social norms and their enforcement. Nature Neuroscience, 15(5), 655–61. https://doi.org/10.1038/nn.3087Google Scholar
Calder, A. J., Lawrence, A. D., & Young, A. W. (2001). Neuropsychology of fear and loathing. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 2(5), 352–63. https://doi.org/10.1038/35072584Google Scholar
Ceballos, N. A., Houston, R. J., Smith, N. D., Bauer, L. O., & Taylor, R. E. (2005). N400 as an index of semantic expectancies: Differential effects of alcohol and cocaine dependence. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry, 29(6), 936–43. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.pnpbp.2005.04.036Google Scholar
Chiao, J. Y. (2009). Cultural neuroscience: A once and future discipline. Progress in Brain Research, 178, 287304. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0079-6123(09)17821-4Google Scholar
Chiao, J. Y., & Blizinsky, K. D. (2010). Culture-gene coevolution of individualism-collectivism and the serotonin transporter gene. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 277(1681), 529–37. http://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2009.1650Google Scholar
Chudek, M., & Henrich, J. (2011). Culture-gene coevolution, norm-psychology and the emergence of human prosociality. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(5), 218–26. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2011.03.003Google Scholar
De Dreu, C. K. W., Greer, L. L., Van Kleef, G. A., Shalvi, S., & Handgraaf, M. J. J. (2011). Oxytocin promotes human ethnocentrism. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(4), 1262–6. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1015316108Google Scholar
de Quervain, D. J.-F., Fischbacher, U., Treyer, V., Schellhammer, M., Schnyder, U., Buck, A., & Fehr, E. (2004). The neural basis of altruistic punishment. Science, 305(5688), 1254–8. http://doi.org/10.1126/science.1100735Google Scholar
Dellinger, K. (2002). Wearing gender and sexuality “on your sleeve”: Dress norms and the importance of occupational and organizational culture at work. Gender Issues, 20(1), 325. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12147-002-0005-5Google Scholar
Dixon, R. B. (1971). Explaining cross-cultural variations in age at marriage and proportions never marrying. Population Studies, 25(2), 215–33. https://doi.org/10.1080/00324728.1971.10405799Google Scholar
Dumas, G., Lachat, F., Martinerie, J., Nadel, J., & George, N. (2011). From social behaviour to brain synchronization: Review and perspectives in hyperscanning. IRBM, 32(1), 4853. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.irbm.2011.01.002Google Scholar
Dumas, G., Nadel, J., Soussignan, R., Martinerie, J., & Garnero, L. (2010). Inter-brain synchronization during social interaction. PLoS ONE, 5(8), e12166. http://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0012166Google Scholar
Ebstein, R. P., Novick, O., Umansky, R., Priel, B., Osher, Y., Blaine, D., Bennett, E. R., Nemanov, L., Katz, M., & Belmaker, R. H. (1996). Dopamine D4 receptor (D4DR) exon III polymorphism associated with the human personality trait of novelty seeking. Nature Genetics, 12(1), 7880. https://doi.org/10.1038/ng0196-78Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., & Zhou, Q. (2000). Regulation from a developmental perspective. Psychological Inquiry, 11(3), 166–71. www.jstor.org/stable/1449796Google Scholar
Elster, J. (1989). The cement of society: A study of social order. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2004a). Social norms and human cooperation. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(4), 185–90. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2004.02.007Google Scholar
Fehr, E., & Fischbacher, U. (2004b). Third-party punishment and social norms. Evolution and Human Behavior, 25(2), 6387. http://doi.org/10.1016/S1090-5138(04)00005-4Google Scholar
Fehr, E., & Rockenbach, B. (2004). Human altruism: Economic, neural, and evolutionary perspectives. Current Opinion in Neurobiology, 14(6), 784–90. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.conb.2004.10.007Google Scholar
Frith, C. D., & Frith, U. (2006). The neural basis of mentalizing. Neuron, 50(4), 531–4. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2006.05.001Google Scholar
Galef, B. G., & Whiskin, E. E. (2008). “Conformity” in Norway rats? Animal Behaviour, 75(6), 2035–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2007.11.012Google Scholar
Gelfand, M. J. (2018). Rule makers, rule breakers: How tight and loose cultures wire our world. Scribner.Google Scholar
Gelfand, M. J., Harrington, J. R., & Jackson, J. C. (2017). The strength of social norms across human groups. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 12(5), 800–9. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691617708631Google Scholar
Gelfand, M. J., Raver, J. L., Nishii, L., Leslie, L. M., Lun, J., Lim, B. C., Duan, L., Almaliach, A., Ang, S., Arnadottir, J., Aycan, Z., Boehnke, K., Boski, P., Cabecinhas, R., Chan, D., Chhokar, J., D’Amato, A., Ferrer, M., Fischlmayr, I. C., … Yamaguchi, S. (2011). Differences between tight and loose cultures: A 33-nation study. Science, 332(6033), 1100–4. http://doi.org/10.1126/science.1197754Google Scholar
Goto, S. G., Ando, Y., Huang, C., Yee, A., & Lewis, R. S. (2009). Cultural differences in the visual processing of meaning: Detecting incongruities between background and foreground objects using the N400. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 5(2–3), 242–53. http://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsp038Google Scholar
Hagoort, P., Hald, L., Bastiaansen, M., & Petersson, K. M. (2004). Integration of word meaning and world knowledge in language comprehension. Science, 304(5669), 438–41. http://doi.org/10.1126/science.1095455Google Scholar
Han, X., Gelfand, M. J., Wu, B., Zhang, T., Li, W., Gao, T., Pang, C., Wu, T., Zhou, Y., Zhou, S., & Wu, X. (2020). A neurobiological association of revenge propensity during intergroup conflict. ELife, 9, e52014. https://doi.org/10.7554/eLife.52014Google Scholar
Han, S., Northoff, G., Vogeley, K., Wexler, B. E., Kitayama, S., & Varnum, M. E. W. (2013). A cultural neuroscience approach to the biosocial nature of the human brain. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 335–59. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-071112-054629Google Scholar
Hariri, A. R., Mattay, V. S., Tessitore, A., Kolachana, B., Fera, F., Goldman, D., Egan, M. F., & Weinberger, D. R. (2002). Serotonin transporter genetic variation and the response of the human amygdala. Science, 297(5580), 400–3. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1071829Google Scholar
Harrington, J. R., & Gelfand, M. J. (2014). Tightness-looseness across the 50 United States. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111(22), 7990–5. http://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1317937111Google Scholar
Harrington, J. R., & Gelfand, M. J. (2020). A world unto themselves: Tightness-looseness and social class [Unpublished manuscript]. Department of Psychology, University of Maryland.Google Scholar
Haun, D. B. M., Rekers, Y., & Tomasello, M. (2014). Children conform to the behavior of peers; other great apes stick with what they know. Psychological Science, 25(12), 2160–7. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614553235Google Scholar
Heeger, D. J., & Ress, D. (2002). What does fMRI tell us about neuronal activity? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 3(2), 142–51. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn730Google Scholar
Higgs, S. (2015). Social norms and their influence on eating behaviours. Appetite, 86, 3844. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.appet.2014.10.021Google Scholar
Hoehl, S., Keupp, S., Schleihauf, H., McGuigan, N., Buttelmann, D., & Whiten, A. (2019). ‘Over-imitation’: A review and appraisal of a decade of research. Developmental Review, 51, 90108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2018.12.002Google Scholar
Hofstede, G. (1980). Culture and organizations. International Studies of Management & Organization, 10(4), 1541. https://doi.org/10.1080/00208825.1980.11656300Google Scholar
Kacen, J. J., & Lee, J. A. (2002). The influence of culture on consumer impulsive buying behavior. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 12(2), 163–76. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327663JCP1202_08Google Scholar
Kanat, M., Heinrichs, M., & Domes, G. (2014). Oxytocin and the social brain: Neural mechanisms and perspectives in human research. Brain Research, 1580, 160–71. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2013.11.003Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., Karasawa, M., & Mesquita, B. (2004). Collective and personal processes in regulating emotions: Emotion and self in Japan and the United States. In Philippot, P. & Friedman, R. S. (Eds.), The regulation of emotion (pp. 251–73). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., King, A., Hsu, M., Liberzon, I., & Yoon, C. (2016). Dopamine-system genes and cultural acquisition: The norm sensitivity hypothesis. Current Opinion in Psychology, 8, 167–74. https://doi.org/10.1016%2Fj.copsyc.2015.11.006Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., King, A., Yoon, C., Tompson, S., Huff, S., & Liberzon, I. (2014). The dopamine D4 receptor gene (DRD4) moderates cultural difference in independent versus interdependent social orientation. Psychological Science, 25(6), 1169–77. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614528338Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., & Uskul, A. K. (2011). Culture, mind, and the brain: Current evidence and future directions. Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 419–49. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120709-145357Google Scholar
Klimesch, W., Sauseng, P., & Hanslmayr, S. (2007). EEG alpha oscillations: The inhibition-timing hypothesis. Brain Research Reviews, 53(1), 6388. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainresrev.2006.06.003Google Scholar
Koenigs, M., & Tranel, D. (2007). Irrational economic decision-making after ventromedial prefrontal damage: Evidence from the Ultimatum Game. Journal of Neuroscience, 27(4), 951–6. http://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4606-06.2007Google Scholar
Krawczyk, D. C. (2002). Contributions of the prefrontal cortex to the neural basis of human decision-making. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 26(6), 631–64. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0149-7634(02)00021-0Google Scholar
Krueger, F., Parasuraman, R., Iyengar, V., Thornburg, M., Weel, J., Lin, M., Clarke, E., McCabe, K., & Lipsky, R. H. (2012). Oxytocin receptor genetic variation promotes human trust behavior. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 6, 4. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2012.00004Google Scholar
Kuhnen, C. M., & Chiao, J. Y. (2009). Genetic determinants of financial risk taking. PLoS ONE, 4(2), e4362. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0004362Google Scholar
Kutas, M., & Federmeier, K. D. (2011). Thirty years and counting: Finding meaning in the N400 component of the event-related brain potential (ERP). Annual Review of Psychology, 62, 621–47. http://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.093008.131123Google Scholar
Laland, K. N., Odling-Smee, J., & Myles, S. (2010). How culture shaped the human genome: Bringing genetics and the human sciences together. Nature Reviews Genetics, 11(2), 137–48. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrg2734Google Scholar
Lesch, K.-P., Bengel, D., Heils, A., Sabol, S. Z., Greenberg, B. D., Petri, S., Benjamin, J., Müller, C. R., Hamer, D. H., & Murphy, D. L. (1996). Association of anxiety-related traits with a polymorphism in the serotonin transporter gene regulatory region. Science, 274(5292), 1527–31. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.274.5292.1527Google Scholar
Luncz, L. V, Mundry, R., & Boesch, C. (2012). Evidence for cultural differences between neighboring chimpanzee communities. Current Biology, 22(10), 922–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2012.03.031Google Scholar
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98(2), 224–53. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.98.2.224Google Scholar
Mathur, V. A., Harada, T., Lipke, T., & Chiao, J. Y. (2010). Neural basis of extraordinary empathy and altruistic motivation. NeuroImage, 51(4), 1468–75. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.03.025Google Scholar
Miller, E. K., & Cohen, J. D. (2001). An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 24, 167202. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167Google Scholar
Montague, P. R., Berns, G. S., Cohen, J. D., McClure, S. M., Pagnoni, G., Dhamala, M., Wiest, M. C., Karpov, I., King, R. D., Apple, N., & Fisher, R. E. (2002). Hyperscanning: Simultaneous fMRI during linked social interactions. NeuroImage, 16(4), 1159–64. http://doi.org/10.1006/nimg.2002.1150Google Scholar
Montague, P. R., & Lohrenz, T. (2007). To detect and correct: Norm violations and their enforcement. Neuron, 56, 1418. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2007.09.020Google Scholar
Mrazek, A. J., Chiao, J. Y., Blizinsky, K. D., Lun, J., & Gelfand, M. J. (2013). The role of culture-gene coevolution in morality judgment: Examining the interplay between tightness-looseness and allelic variation of the serotonin transporter gene. Culture and Brain, 1(2–4), 100–17. https://doi.org/10.1007%2Fs40167-013-0009-xGoogle Scholar
Mu, Y., Guo, C., & Han, S. (2016). Oxytocin enhances inter-brain synchrony during social coordination in male adults. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(2), 1882–93. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsw106Google Scholar
Mu, Y., Han, S., & Gelfand, M. J. (2017). The role of gamma interbrain synchrony in social coordination when humans face territorial threats. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(10), 1614–23. https://doi.org/10.1093%2Fscan%2Fnsx093Google Scholar
Mu, Y., Han, S., & Gelfand, M. J. (2020). An fMRI study on social norm violation detection and punishment [Unpublished manuscript]. Department of Psychology, University of Maryland.Google Scholar
Mu, Y., Kitayama, S., Han, S., & Gelfand, M. J. (2015). How culture gets embrained: Cultural differences in event-related potentials of social norm violations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 112(50), 15348–53. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1509839112Google Scholar
Mu, Y., Kitayama, S., Han, S., & Gelfand, M. J. (2020). Do we rest differently?: Cultural variation in neural markers of self-control [Unpublished manuscript]. Department of Psychology, University of Maryland.Google Scholar
Munafò, M. R., Brown, S. M., & Hariri, A. R. (2008). Serotonin transporter (5-HTTLPR) genotype and amygdala activation: A meta-analysis. Biological Psychiatry, 63(9), 852–7. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2007.08.016Google Scholar
Na, J., & Kitayama, S. (2011). Spontaneous trait inference is culture-specific: Behavioral and neural evidence. Psychological Science, 22(8), 1025–32. http://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611414727Google Scholar
Nitsche, M. A., & Paulus, W. (2000). Excitability changes induced in the human motor cortex by weak transcranial direct current stimulation. Journal of Physiology, 527(3), 633–9. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-7793.2000.t01-1-00633.xGoogle Scholar
Osinsky, R., Reuter, M., Küpper, Y., Schmitz, A., Kozyra, E., Alexander, N., & Hennig, J. (2008). Variation in the serotonin transporter gene modulates selective attention to threat. Emotion, 8(4), 584–8. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0012826Google Scholar
Pelto, P. J. (1968). The differences between “tight” and “loose” societies. Society, 5(5), 3740. https://doi.org/10.1007/BF03180447Google Scholar
Ramnani, N., & Owen, A. M. (2004). Anterior prefrontal cortex: Insights into function from anatomy and neuroimaging. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5(3), 184–94. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn1343Google Scholar
Reher, D. S. (1998). Family ties in Western Europe: Persistent contrasts. Population and Development Review, 24(2), 203–34. https://doi.org/10.2307/2807972Google Scholar
Röder, B., Rösler, F., & Neville, H. J. (2000). Event-related potentials during auditory language processing in congenitally blind and sighted people. Neuropsychologia, 38(11), 1482–502. http://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00057-9Google Scholar
Roos, P., Gelfand, M. J., Nau, D., & Lun, J. (2015). Societal threat and cultural variation in the strength of social norms: An evolutionary basis. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 129, 1423. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.obhdp.2015.01.003Google Scholar
Ruff, C. C., Ugazio, G., & Fehr, E. (2013). Changing social norm compliance with noninvasive brain stimulation. Science, 342(6157), 482–4. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1241399Google Scholar
Salvador, C. E., Mu, Y., Gelfand, M. J., & Kitayama, S. (2020). When norm violations are spontaneously detected: An electrocortical investigation. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 15(3), 319–27. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsaa035Google Scholar
Sanfey, A. G., Rilling, J. K., Aronson, J. A, Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D. (2003). The neural basis of economic decision-making in the Ultimatum Game. Science, 300(5626), 1755–8. http://doi.org/10.1126/science.1082976Google Scholar
Sanz, C., Call, J., & Morgan, D. (2009). Design complexity in termite-fishing tools of chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Biology Letters, 5(3), 293–6. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsbl.2008.0786Google Scholar
Sasaki, J. Y., Kim, H. S., Mojaverian, T., Kelley, L. D. S., Park, I. Y., & Janušonis, S. (2013). Religion priming differentially increases prosocial behavior among variants of the dopamine D4 receptor (DRD4) gene. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 8(2), 209–15. https://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsr089Google Scholar
Sauseng, P., Klimesch, W., Doppelmayr, M., Pecherstorfer, T., Freunberger, R., & Hanslmayr, S. (2005). EEG alpha synchronization and functional coupling during top-down processing in a working memory task. Human Brain Mapping, 26(2), 148–55. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20150Google Scholar
Schwartz, S. H. (1992). Universals in the content and structure of values: Theoretical advances and empirical tests in 20 countries. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 25, 165. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60281-6Google Scholar
Sen, S., Burmeister, M., & Ghosh, D. (2004). Meta-analysis of the association between a serotonin transporter promoter polymorphism (5‐HTTLPR) and anxiety-related personality traits. American Journal of Medical Genetics Part B: Neuropsychiatric Genetics, 127B(1), 85–9. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajmg.b.20158Google Scholar
Shamay-Tsoory, S. G., & Abu-Akel, A. (2016). The social salience hypothesis of oxytocin. Biological Psychiatry, 79(3), 194202. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2015.07.020Google Scholar
Sheese, B. E., Voelker, P. M., Rothbart, M. K., & Posner, M. I. (2007). Parenting quality interacts with genetic variation in dopamine receptor D4 to influence temperament in early childhood. Development and Psychopathology, 19(4), 1039–46. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0954579407000521Google Scholar
Singelis, T. M. (1994). The measurement of independent and interdependent self-construals. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 20(5), 580–91. http://doi.org/10.1177/0146167294205014Google Scholar
Singer, T., Critchley, H. D., & Preuschoff, K. (2009). A common role of insula in feelings, empathy and uncertainty. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(8), 334–40. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.05.001Google Scholar
Spitzer, M., Fischbacher, U., Herrnberger, B., Grön, G., & Fehr, E. (2007). The neural signature of social norm compliance. Neuron, 56(1), 185–96. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2007.09.011Google Scholar
Spring, J. H. (2008). Wheels in the head: Educational philosophies of authority, freedom, and culture from Confucianism to human rights (3rd ed.). Routledge.Google Scholar
Strobel, A., Zimmermann, J., Schmitz, A., Reuter, M., Lis, S., Windmann, S., & Kirsch, P. (2011). Beyond revenge: Neural and genetic bases of altruistic punishment. NeuroImage, 54(1), 671–80. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.07.051Google Scholar
Suitor, J. J., & Carter, R. S. (1999). Jocks, nerds, babes and thugs: A research note on regional differences in adolescent gender norms. Gender Issues, 17(3), 87101. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12147-999-0005-9Google Scholar
Tomasello, M., Kruger, A. C., & Ratner, H. H. (1993). Cultural learning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 16(3), 495511. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X0003123XGoogle Scholar
Tost, H., Kolachana, B., Hakimi, S., Lemaitre, H., Verchinski, B. A., Mattay, V. S., Weinberger, D. R., & Meyer-Lindenberg, A. (2010). A common allele in the oxytocin receptor gene (OXTR) impacts prosocial temperament and human hypothalamic-limbic structure and function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 107(31), 13936–41. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1003296107Google Scholar
Triandis, H. C. (1989). The self and social behavior in differing cultural contexts. Psychological Review, 96(3), 506–20. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.96.3.506.Google Scholar
Triandis, H. C., & Gelfand, M. J. (1998). Converging measurement of horizontal and vertical individualism and collectivism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74(1), 118–28. http://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.1.118Google Scholar
van de Waal, E., Borgeaud, C., & Whiten, A. (2013). Potent social learning and conformity shape a wild primate’s foraging decisions. Science, 340(6131), 483–5. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1232769Google Scholar
Van Tol, H. H. M., Wu, C. M., Guan, H.-C., Ohara, K., Bunzow, J. R., Civelli, O., Kennedy, J., Seeman, P., Niznik, H. B., & Jovanovic, V. (1992). Multiple dopamine D4 receptor variants in the human population. Nature, 358(6382), 149–52. https://doi.org/10.1038/358149a0Google Scholar
Varnum, M. E. W., Na, J., Murata, A., & Kitayama, S. (2012). Social class differences in N400 indicate differences in spontaneous trait inference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(3), 518–26. http://doi.org/10.1037/a0026104Google Scholar
Verbeke, W. J. M. I., Pozharliev, R., Van Strien, J. W., Belschak, F., & Bagozzi, R. P. (2014). “I am resting but rest less well with you.” The moderating effect of anxious attachment style on alpha power during EEG resting state in a social context. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 486. https://doi.org/10.3389%2Ffnhum.2014.00486Google Scholar
Wager, T. D., Davidson, M. L., Hughes, B. L., Lindquist, M. A., & Ochsner, K. N. (2008). Prefrontal-subcortical pathways mediating successful emotion regulation. Neuron, 59(6), 1037–50. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2008.09.006Google Scholar
White, K. R., Crites, S. L. Jr., Taylor, J. H., & Corral, G. (2009). Wait, what? Assessing stereotype incongruities using the N400 ERP component. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4(2), 191–8. http://doi.org/10.1093/scan/nsp004Google Scholar
Whiten, A. (2019). Conformity and over-imitation: An integrative review of variant. In Naguib, M., Barrett, L., Healy, S. D., Podos, J., Simmons, L. W., & Zuk, M. (Eds.), Advances in the study of behavior (Vol. 51, pp. 3175). Elsevier Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.asb.2018.12.003Google Scholar
Whiten, A., Horner, V., & de Waal, F. B. M. (2005). Conformity to cultural norms of tool use in chimpanzees. Nature, 437(7059), 737–40. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature04047Google Scholar
Zhang, Y., & Shrum, L. J. (2009). The influence of self-construal on impulsive consumption. Journal of Consumer Research, 35(5), 838–50. https://doi.org/10.1086/593687.Google Scholar

References

Abrams, D., & Hogg, M. A. (1988). Comments on the motivational status of self-esteem in social identity and intergroup discrimination. European Journal of Social Psychology, 18(4), 317–34. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2420180403Google Scholar
Ahmed, A., & Salas, O. (2013). Religious context and prosociality: An experimental study from Valparaíso, Chile. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 52(3), 627–37. https://doi.org/10.1111/jssr.12045Google Scholar
Allen, M. R. (1967). Male cults and secret initiations in Melanesia. Melbourne University Press.Google Scholar
Anderson, B. (1983). Imagined communities: Reflections on the origin and spread of nationalism. Verso.Google Scholar
Annis, L. V. (1976). Emergency helping and religious behavior. Psychological Reports, 39(1), 151–8. https://doi.org/10.2466%2Fpr0.1976.39.1.151Google Scholar
Arnal, W., & McCutcheon, R. T. (2012). The sacred is the profane: The political nature of “religion.” Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199757114.001.0001Google Scholar
Aronson, E. (1968). Dissonance theory: Progress and problems. In Abelson, R., Aronson, E.McGuire, W.Newcomb, T.Rosenberg, M., & Tannebaum, P. (Eds.), The cognitive consistency theories: A source book (pp. 527). Rand McNally.Google Scholar
Aronson, E. (1999). The power of self-persuasion. American Psychologist, 54(11), 875–84. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0088188Google Scholar
Aronson, E., & Mills, J. (1959). The effect of severity of initiation on liking for a group. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 59(2), 177–81. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0047195Google Scholar
Atkinson, Q. D., & Whitehouse, H. (2011). The cultural morphospace of ritual form: Examining modes of religiosity cross-culturally. Evolution and Human Behavior, 32(1), 5062. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.09.002Google Scholar
Bandura, A., Ross, D., & Ross, S. A. (1961). Transmission of aggression through imitation of aggressive models. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 63(3), 575–82. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0045925Google Scholar
Bastian, B., Jetten, J., & Ferris, L. J. (2014). Pain as social glue: Shared pain increases cooperation. Psychological Science, 25(11), 2079–85. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797614545886Google Scholar
Baumard, N., Hyafil, A., Morris, I., & Boyer, P. (2014). Increased affluence explains the emergence of ascetic wisdoms and moralizing religions. Current Biology, 25(1), 1015. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2014.10.063Google Scholar
Bell, C. (1997). Ritual: Perspectives and dimensions. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bellah, R. N. (2011). Religion in human evolution: From the Paleolithic to the Axial Age. Belknap Press.Google Scholar
Bem, D. J. (1972). Self-perception theory. In Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 6, pp. 162). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Berling, J. A. (1987). Orthopraxy. In Eliade, M. (Ed.), The encyclopedia of religion (Vol. 11, pp. 129–32). Macmillan.Google Scholar
Bloom, P. (2012). Religion, morality, evolution. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 179–99. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-psych-120710-100334Google Scholar
Boyer, P. (2001). Religion explained: The evolutionary origins of religious thought. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Boyer, P. (2011, February 2). Why would (otherwise intelligent) scholars believe in “religion”? [Blog post]. https://cognitionandculture.net/blog/pascals-blog/why-would-otherwise-intelligent-scholars-believe-inGoogle Scholar
Boyer, P., & Liénard, P. (2006). Why ritualized behavior? Precaution systems and action parsing in developmental, pathological and cultural rituals. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29(6), 595613. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X06009332Google Scholar
Bressan, P., & Kramer, P. (2015). Human kin detection. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 6(3), 299311. https://doi.org/10.1002/wcs.1347Google Scholar
Brewer, M. B., & Gardner, W. (1996). Who is this “we”? Levels of collective identity and self representations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(1), 8393. https://doi.org/10.1037//0022-3514.71.1.83Google Scholar
Brooks, A. C. (2006). Who really cares: The surprising truth about compassionate conservatism—America’s charity divide—who gives, who doesn’t, and why it matters. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Brown, J. L., Sheffield, D., Leary, M. R., & Robinson, M. E. (2003). Social support and experimental pain. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65(2), 276–83. https://doi.org/10.1097/01.PSY.0000030388.62434.46Google Scholar
Bulbulia, J., Geertz, A. W., Atkinson, Q. D., Cohen, E., Evans, N., François, P., Gintis, H., Gray, R. D., Henrich, J., Jordon, F. M., Norenzayan, A., Richerson, P. J., Slingerland, E., Turchin, P., Whitehouse, H., Widlok, T., & Wilson, D. S. (2013). The cultural evolution of religion. In Richerson, P. J. & Christiansen, M. H. (Eds.), Cultural evolution: Science, technology, language, and religion (pp. 381404). MIT Press. https://doi.org/10.7551/mitpress/9780262019750.003.0020Google Scholar
Bulbulia, J. A., & Sosis, R. (2011). Signalling theory and the evolution of religious cooperation. Religion, 41(3), 363–88. https://doi.org/10.1080/0048721X.2011.604508Google Scholar
Bushman, B. J., Ridge, R. D., Das, E., Key, C. W., & Busath, G. L. (2007). When God sanctions killing: Effect of scriptural violence on aggressionPsychological Science18(3), 204–7. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01873.xGoogle Scholar
Carpenter, M., Uebel, J., & Tomasello, M. (2013). Being mimicked increases prosocial behavior in 18-month-old infants. Child Development, 84(5), 1511–18. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12083Google Scholar
Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception–behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(6), 893910. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.76.6.893Google Scholar
Cimino, A. (2011). The evolution of hazing: Motivational mechanisms and the abuse of newcomers. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 11(3–4), 241–67. https://doi.org/10.1163/156853711X591242Google Scholar
Cimino, A. (2013). Predictors of hazing motivation in a representative sample of the United States. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(6), 446–52. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2013.08.007Google Scholar
Cirelli, L. K., Einarson, K. M., & Trainor, L. J. (2014a). Interpersonal synchrony increases prosocial behavior in infants. Developmental Science, 17(6), 1003–11. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12193Google Scholar
Cirelli, L. K., Wan, S. J., & Trainor, L. J. (2014b). Fourteen-month-old infants use interpersonal synchrony as a cue to direct helpfulness. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369(1658), 20130400. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2013.0400Google Scholar
Cirelli, L. K., Wan, S. J., & Trainor, L. J. (2016). Social effects of movement synchrony: Increased infant helpfulness only transfers to affiliates of synchronously moving partners. Infancy, 21(6), 807–21. https://doi.org/10.1111/infa.12140Google Scholar
Clark, R. D. III, & Maass, A. (1988). The role of social categorization and perceived source credibility in minority influence. European Journal of Social Psychology, 18(5), 381–94. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2420180502Google Scholar
Clegg, J. M., & Legare, C. H. (2015). Instrumental and conventional interpretations of behavior are associated with distinct outcomes in early childhood. Child Development, 87(2), 527–42. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12472.Google Scholar
Clegg, J. M., & Legare, C. H. (2016). A cross-cultural comparison of children’s imitative flexibilityDevelopmental Psychology52(9), 1435–44. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000131Google Scholar
Coan, J. A., Schaefer, H. S., & Davidson, R. J. (2006). Lending a hand: Social regulation of the neural response to threatPsychological Science17(12), 1032–9. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2006.01832.xGoogle Scholar
Cogan, R., & Spinnato, J. A. (1986). Pain and discomfort thresholds in late pregnancy. Pain, 27(1), 63–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-3959(86)90223-XGoogle Scholar
Cooper, J., & Fazio, R. H. (1984). A new look at dissonance theory. In Berkowitz, L. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 17, pp. 229–66). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60121-5Google Scholar
Cooper, J., & Hogg, M. A. (2007). Feeling the anguish of others: A theory of vicarious dissonance. In Zanna, M. P. (Ed.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 39, 359403). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-2601(06)39007-7Google Scholar
Corriveau, K. H., DiYanni, C. J., Clegg, J. M., Min, G., Chin, J., & Nasrini, J. (2017). Cultural differences in the imitation and transmission of inefficient actionsJournal of Experimental Child Psychology161, 118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.03.002Google Scholar
Darley, J. M., & Batson, C. D. (1973). “ From Jerusalem to Jericho”: A study of situational and dispositional variables in helping behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 27(1), 100–8. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0034449Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (2004). A devil’s chaplain: Reflections on hope, lies, science, and love. Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (2007). The God delusion. Black Swan.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (2017). Science in the soul: Selected writings of a passionate rationalist. (Somerscales, G., Ed.). Penguin Random House.Google Scholar
DeBono, K. G., & Harnish, R. J. (1988). Source expertise, source attractiveness, and the processing of persuasive information: A functional approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 55(4), 541–6. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.55.4.541Google Scholar
DeBruine, L. M. (2005). Trustworthy but not lust-worthy: Context-specific effects of facial resemblance. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 272(1566), 919–22. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2004.3003Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (2006). Breaking the spell: Religion as a natural phenomenon. Viking.Google Scholar
Durkheim, É. (1912/1965). The elementary forms of the religious life (Swain, J.W., Trans.). Free Press. (Original work published 1912)Google Scholar
Fehr, E., & Schmidt, K. M. (1999). A theory of fairness, competition, and cooperationQuarterly Journal of Economics114(3), 817–68. https://doi.org/10.1162/003355399556151Google Scholar
Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Fischer, R., Xygalatas, D., Mitkidis, P., Reddish, P., Tok, P., Konvalinka, I., & Bulbulia, J. A. (2014). The fire-walker’s high: Affect and physiological responses in an extreme collective ritual. PLoS ONE, 9(2), e88355. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0088355Google Scholar
Fischer-Lokou, J., Martin, A., Guéguen, N., & Lamy, L. (2011). Mimicry and propagation of prosocial behavior in a natural setting. Psychological Reports, 108(2), 599605. https://doi.org/10.2466/07.17.21.PR0.108.2.599-605Google Scholar
Fishbein, M., & Ajzen, I. (1975). Belief, attitude, intention, and behavior: An introduction to theory and research. Addison-Wesley Pub. Co.Google Scholar
Flynn, E., & Smith, K. (2012) Investigating the mechanisms of cultural acquisition: How pervasive is overimitation in adults? Social Psychology43(4), 185–95. https://doi.org/10.1027/1864-9335/a000119Google Scholar
Freud, S. (1961). Obsessive actions and religious practices. In Strachey, J. (Ed.), The standard edition of the complete psychological works of Sigmund Freud (Vol. 9, pp. 167–75). Hogarth Press.Google Scholar
Galen, L. W. (2012). Does religious belief promote prosociality? A critical examination. Psychological Bulletin, 138(5), 876906. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028251Google Scholar
Gerard, H. B., & Mathewson, G. C. (1966). The effects of severity of initiation on liking for a group: A replication. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2(3), 278–87. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(66)90084-9Google Scholar
Ginges, J., Hansen, I., & Norenzayan, A. (2009). Religion and support for suicide attacksPsychological Science20(2), 224–30. https://doi.org/10.1111%2Fj.1467-9280.2009.02270.xGoogle Scholar
Gomes, C. M., & McCullough, M. E. (2015). The effects of implicit religious primes on dictator game allocations: A preregistered replication experiment. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 144(6), e94e104. https://doi.org/10.1037/xge0000027Google Scholar
Gómez, Á., Brooks, M. L., Buhrmester, M. D., Vázquez, A., Jetten, J., & Swann, W. B. Jr. (2011). On the nature of identity fusion: Insights into the construct and a new measure. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(5), 918–33. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022642Google Scholar
Graham, J., & Haidt, J. (2010). Beyond beliefs: Religions bind individuals into moral communities. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14(1), 140–50. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868309353415Google Scholar
Graham, J., Haidt, J., Koleva, S., Motyl, M., Iyer, R., Wojcik, S. P., & Ditto, P. H. (2013). Moral foundations theory: The pragmatic validity of moral pluralism. In Devine, P. & Plant, A. (Eds.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 47, pp. 55130). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-407236-7.00002-4Google Scholar
Graham, J., Haidt, J., & Nosek, B. A. (2009). Liberals and conservatives rely on different sets of moral foundations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(5), 1029–46. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0015141Google Scholar
Grossman, P. J., & Parrett, M. B. (2011). Religion and prosocial behaviour: A field test. Applied Economics Letters, 18(6), 523–6. https://doi.org/10.1080/13504851003761798Google Scholar
Guthrie, S. E. (1993). Faces in the clouds: A new theory of religion. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Harris, S. (2006). The end of faith: Religion, terror, and the future of reason. Free Press.Google Scholar
Hautaluoma, J. E., & Spungin, H. (1974). Effects of initiation severity and interest on group attitudes. Journal of Social Psychology, 93(2), 245–9. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224545.1974.9923159Google Scholar
Henrich, J. (2009). The evolution of costly displays, cooperation and religion: Credibility enhancing displays and their implications for cultural evolution. Evolution and Human Behavior, 30(4), 244–60. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2009.03.005Google Scholar
Henrich, J., Ensminger, J., McElreath, R., Barr, A., Barrett, C., Bolyanatz, A., Cardenas, J. C., Gurven, M., Gwako, E., Henrich, N., Lesorogol, C., Marlowe, F., Tracer, D., & Ziker, J. (2010). Markets, religion, community size, and the evolution of fairness and punishment. Science, 327(5972), 1480–4. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1182238Google Scholar
Herrmann, P. A., Legare, C. H., Harris, P. L., & Whitehouse, H. (2013). Stick to the script: The effect of witnessing multiple actors on children’s imitation. Cognition, 129(3), 536–43. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.08.010Google Scholar
Hitchens, C. (2008). God is not great: How religion poisons everything. McClelland & Stewart.Google Scholar
Hoehl, S., Keupp, S., Schleihauf, H., McGuigan, N., Buttelmann, D., & Whiten, A. (2019). ‘Over-imitation’: A review and appraisal of a decade of researchDevelopmental Review51, 90108. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2018.12.002Google Scholar
Hofmann, W., Wisneski, D. C., Brandt, M. J., & Skitka, L. J. (2014). Morality in everyday life. Science, 345(6202), 1340–3. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1251560Google Scholar
Hogg, M. A. (2006). Social identity theory. In Burke, P. J. (Ed.), Contemporary social psychological theories (pp. 111–36). Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Hoppitt, W., & Laland, K. N. (2013). Social learning: An introduction to mechanisms, methods, and models. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Horner, V., & Whiten, A. (2005). Causal knowledge and imitation/emulation switching in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and children (Homo sapiens). Animal Cognition, 8(3), 164–81. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10071-004-0239-6Google Scholar
Hornsey, M. J. (2008). Social identity theory and self-categorization theory: A historical review. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2(1), 204–22. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1751-9004.2007.00066.xGoogle Scholar
Hove, M. J., & Risen, J. L. (2009). It’s all in the timing: Interpersonal synchrony increases affiliation. Social Cognition, 27(6), 949–60. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2009.27.6.949Google Scholar
Hovland, C. I., & Weiss, W. (1951). The influence of source credibility on communication effectiveness. Public Opinion Quarterly, 15(4), 635–50. https://doi.org/10.1086/266350Google Scholar
Humphrey, C., & Laidlaw, J. (1994). The archetypal actions of ritual: A theory of ritual illustrated by the Jain rite of worship. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Iacoboni, M. (2009). Imitation, empathy, and mirror neuronsAnnual Review of Psychology, 60, 653–70. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.60.110707.163604Google Scholar
Iannaccone, L. R. (1994). Why strict churches are strong. American Journal of Sociology, 99(5), 1180–211. https://doi.org/10.1086/230409Google Scholar
Irons, W. (2001). Religion as a hard-to-fake sign of commitment. In Nesse, R. M. (Ed.), Vol. 3 in the Russell SAGE Foundation series on trust. Evolution and the capacity for commitment (pp. 292309). Russell SAGE Foundation.Google Scholar
Jegindø, E.-M. E., Vase, L., Jegindø, J., & Geertz, A. W. (2013). Pain and sacrifice: Experience and modulation of pain in a religious piercing ritual. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 23(3), 171–87. https://doi.org/10.1080/10508619.2012.759065Google Scholar
Johnson, D. (2015). God is watching you: How the fear of God makes us human. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, K. A., Hook, J. N., Davis, D. E., Van Tongeren, D. R., Sandage, S. J., & Crabtree, S. A. (2016). Moral foundation priorities reflect U. S. Christians’ individual differences in religiosity. Personality and Individual Differences, 100, 5661. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2015.12.037Google Scholar
Johnson, M. K., Rowatt, W. C., & LaBouff, J. (2010). Priming Christian religious concepts increases racial prejudiceSocial Psychological and Personality Science1(2), 119–26. https://doi.org/10.1177%2F1948550609357246Google Scholar
Johnson, M. K., Rowatt, W. C., & LaBouff, J. P. (2012). Religiosity and prejudice revisited: In-group favoritism, out-group derogation, or both? Psychology of Religion and Spirituality, 4(2), 154–68. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025107Google Scholar
Jones, S. S. (2009). The development of imitation in infancy. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364(1528), 2325–35. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0045Google Scholar
Jones, J. M., & Jetten, J. (2011). Recovering from strain and enduring pain: Multiple group memberships promote resilience in the face of physical challenges. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 2(3), 239–44. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550610386806Google Scholar
Jong, J. (2015). On (not) defining (non)religion. Science, Religion & Culture, 2(3), 1524. https://doi.org/10.17582/journal.src/2015/2.3.15.24Google Scholar
Jong, J. (2017). “Belief in spiritual beings”: E. B. Tylor’s (primitive) cognitive theory of religion. In Tremlett, P.-F., Harvey, G., & Sutherland, L. T. (Eds.), Edward Burnett Tylor, religion and culture (pp. 4761). Bloomsbury Publishing.Google Scholar
Jong, J., & Halberstadt, J. (2016). Death anxiety and religious belief: An existential psychology of religion. Bloomsbury Academic.Google Scholar
Jong, J., Whitehouse, H., Kavanagh, C. M., & Lane, J. (2015). Shared negative experiences lead to identity fusion via personal reflection. PLoS ONE, 10(12), e0145611. https://doi.org/10.1371%2Fjournal.pone.0145611Google Scholar
Kapitány, R., Kavanagh, C., Buhrmester, M. D., Newson, M., & Whitehouse, H. (2019). Ritual, identity fusion, and the inauguration of president Trump: A pseudo-experiment of ritual modes theory. Self and Identity. Advance online publication. https://doi.org/10.1080/15298868.2019.1578686Google Scholar
Kavanagh, C. (2016a). Individual pain & social gain: The personal and social consequences of collective dysphoric rituals [doctoral dissertation]. University of Oxford. https://ora.ox.ac.uk/objects/uuid:e2e0f4de-ccf1-4962-87fe-4d7fa48faf75Google Scholar
Kavanagh, C. (2016b, September 15) Religion without belief. Aeon. https://aeon.co/essays/can-religion-be-based-on-ritual-practice-without-beliefGoogle Scholar
Kavanagh, C. M., & Jong, J. (2020). Is Japan religious? Journal for the Study of Religion, Nature and Culture, 14(1). https://doi.org/10.31234/osf.io/qyt95Google Scholar
Kavanagh, C. M., Jong, J., McKay, R., & Whitehouse, H. (2018). Positive experiences of high arousal martial arts rituals are linked to identity fusion and costly pro-group actions. European Journal of Social Psychology, 49(3), 461–81. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.2514Google Scholar
Kay, A. C., Gaucher, D., McGregor, I., & Nash, K. (2010). Religious belief as compensatory control. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14(1), 3748. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868309353750Google Scholar
Keating, C. F., Pomerantz, J., Pommer, S. D., Ritt, S. J. H., Miller, L. M., & McCormick, J. (2005). Going to college and unpacking hazing: A functional approach to decrypting initiation practices among undergraduates. Group Dynamics: Theory, Research, and Practice, 9(2), 104–26. https://doi.org/10.1037/1089-2699.9.2.104Google Scholar
Kenward, B., Karlsson, M., & Persson, J. (2010). Over-imitation is better explained by norm learning than by distorted causal learningProceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences278(1709), 1239–46. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.1399Google Scholar
Kirschner, S., & Tomasello, M. (2010). Joint music making promotes prosocial behavior in 4-year-old children. Evolution and Human Behavior, 31(5), 354–64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2010.04.004Google Scholar
Kitayama, S., & Tompson, S. (2015). A biosocial model of affective decision making: Implications for dissonance, motivation, and culture. In Olson, J. M. & Zanna, M. P. (Eds.), Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 52, pp. 71137). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Konvalinka, I., Xygalatas, D., Bulbulia, J., Schjødt, U., Jegindø, E.-M., Wallot, S., Van Orden, G., & Roepstorff, A. (2011). Synchronized arousal between performers and related spectators in a fire-walking ritual. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 108(20), 8514–19. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1016955108Google Scholar
Krupp, D. B., DeBruine, L. M., Jones, B. C., & Lalumière, M. L. (2012). Kin recognition: Evidence that humans can perceive both positive and negative relatedness. Journal of Evolutionary Biology, 25(8), 1472–8. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1420-9101.2012.02553.xGoogle Scholar
LaBouff, J. P., Humphreys, M., & Shen, M. J. (2017). Religiosity and group-binding moral concerns. Archive for the Psychology of Religion, 39(3), 263–82. https://journals.sagepub.com/doi/10.1163/15736121-12341343?icid=int.sj-abstract.similar-articles.2; https://doi.org/10.1163/15736121-12341343Google Scholar
Lakens, D. (2010). Movement synchrony and perceived entitativity. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 46(5), 701–8. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2010.03.015Google Scholar
Lakens, D., & Stel, M. (2011). If they move in sync, they must feel in sync: Movement synchrony leads to attributions of rapport and entitativity. Social Cognition, 29(1), 114. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2011.29.1.1Google Scholar
LaBouff, J. P., Rowatt, W. C., Johnson, M. K., & Finkle, C. (2012). Differences in attitudes toward outgroups in religious and nonreligious contexts in a multinational sample: A situational context priming studyInternational Journal for the Psychology of Religion22(1), 19. https://doi.org/10.1080/10508619.2012.634778Google Scholar
Lanman, J. A., & Buhrmester, M. D. (2017). Religious actions speak louder than words: Exposure to credibility-enhancing displays predicts theism. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 7(1), 316. https://doi.org/10.1080/2153599X.2015.1117011Google Scholar
Legare, C. H., Wen, N. J., Herrmann, P. A., & Whitehouse, H. (2015). Imitative flexibility and the development of cultural learning. Cognition, 142, 351–61. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.05.020Google Scholar
Lidderdale, J. M., & Walsh, J. J. (1998). The effects of social support on cardiovascular reactivity and perinatal outcome. Psychology & Health, 13(6), 1061–70. https://doi.org/10.1080/08870449808407450Google Scholar
Liénard, P., & Boyer, P. (2006). Whence collective rituals? A cultural selection model of ritualized behavior. American Anthropologist, 108(4), 814–27. https://doi.org/10.1525/aa.2006.108.4.814Google Scholar
Lodewijkx, H. F. M., & Syroit, J. E. M. M. (1997). Severity of initiation revisited: Does severity of initiation increase attractiveness in real groups? European Journal of Social Psychology, 27(3), 275300. https://doi.org/10.1002/(SICI)1099-0992(199705)27:3<275::AID-EJSP822>3.0.CO;2-SGoogle Scholar
López-Martínez, A. E., Esteve-Zarazaga, R., & Ramírez-Maestre, C. (2008). Perceived social support and coping responses are independent variables explaining pain adjustment among chronic pain patients. Journal of Pain, 9(4), 373–9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpain.2007.12.002Google Scholar
Lyons, D. E., Damrosch, D. H., Lin, J. K., Macris, D. M., & Keil, F. C. (2011). The scope and limits of overimitation in the transmission of artefact culture. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 366(1567), 1158–67. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2010.0335Google Scholar
Lyons, D. E., Young, A. G., & Keil, F. C. (2007). The hidden structure of overimitation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 104(50), 19751–6. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0704452104Google Scholar
Master, S. L., Eisenberger, N. I., Taylor, S. E., Naliboff, B. D., Shirinyan, D., & Lieberman, M. D. (2009). A picture’s worth: Partner photographs reduce experimentally induced pain. Psychological Science, 20(11), 1316–18. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02444.xGoogle Scholar
McCutcheon, R. T. (1997). Manufacturing religion: The discourse on sui generis religion and the politics of nostalgia. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
McGuigan, N. (2012). The role of transmission biases in the cultural diffusion of irrelevant actionsJournal of Comparative Psychology126(2), 150–60. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025525Google Scholar
McGuigan, N., Makinson, J., & Whiten, A. (2011). From over-imitation to super-copying: Adults imitate causally irrelevant aspects of tool use with higher fidelity than young children. British Journal of Psychology, 102(1), 118. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712610X493115Google Scholar
McKay, R., Efferson, C., Whitehouse, H., & Fehr, E. (2011). Wrath of God: Religious primes and punishment. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 278(1713), 1858–63. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2010.2125Google Scholar
McKay, R., & Whitehouse, H. (2015). Religion and morality. Psychological Bulletin, 141(2), 447–73. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038455Google Scholar
Meltzoff, A. N. (2007). “Like me”: A foundation for social cognition. Developmental Science, 10(1), 126–34. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00574.xGoogle Scholar
Mischel, W. (1968). Personality and assessment. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Mitkidis, P., Ayal, S., Shalvi, S., Heimann, K., Levy, G., Kyselo, M., Wallot, S., Ariely, D., & Roepstorff, A. (2017). The effects of extreme rituals on moral behavior: The performers-observers gap hypothesis. Journal of Economic Psychology, 59, 17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2016.12.007Google Scholar
Miyatake, S., & Higuchi, M. (2017). Does religious priming increase the prosocial behaviour of a Japanese sample in an anonymous economic game? Asian Journal of Social Psychology, 20(1), 54–9. https://doi.org/10.1111/ajsp.12164Google Scholar
Mogan, R., Fischer, R., & Bulbulia, J. A. (2017). To be in synchrony or not? A meta-analysis of synchrony’s effects on behavior, perception, cognition and affect. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 72, 1320. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2017.03.009Google Scholar
Nielsen, M., & Blank, C. (2011). Imitation in young children: When who gets copied is more important than what gets copiedDevelopmental Psychology47(4), 1050–3. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0023866Google Scholar
Nielsen, M., Mushin, I., Tomaselli, K., & Whiten, A. (2014). Where culture takes hold:”Overimitation” and its flexible deployment in Western, Aboriginal, and Bushmen childrenChild Development85(6), 2169–84. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12265Google Scholar
Nielsen, M., & Tomaselli, K. (2010) Overimitation in Kalahari Bushman children and the origins of human cultural cognition. Psychological Science21(5), 729–36. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610368808Google Scholar
Norenzayan, A. (2013). Big gods: How religion transformed cooperation and conflict. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Norenzayan, A., Shariff, A. F., Gervais, W. M.Willard, A. K., McNamara, R. A., Slingerland, E., & Henrich, J. (2016). The cultural evolution of prosocial religionsBehavioral and Brain Sciences39, e1https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X14001356Google Scholar
Norton, A. R. (2005). Ritual, blood, and Shiite identity: Ashura in Nabatiyya, Lebanon. TDR/The Drama Review, 49(4), 140–55. https://doi.org/10.1162/105420405774762880Google Scholar
Norton, M. I., & Gino, F. (2014). Rituals alleviate grieving for loved ones, lovers, and lotteries. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(1), 266–72. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0031772Google Scholar
Nosek, B. A., Alter, G., Banks, G. C., Borsboom, D., Bowman, S. D., Breckler, S. J., Buck, S., Chambers, C. D., Chin, G., Christensen, G., Contestabile, M., Dafoe, A., Eich, E., Freese, J., Glennerster, R., Goroff, D., Green, D. P., Hesse, B., Humphreys, M., Ishiyama, J., … Yarkoni, T. (2015). Promoting an open research culture. Science, 348(6242), 1422–5. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aab2374Google Scholar
Open Science Collaboration. (2015). Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science, 349(6251), aac4716. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aac4716Google Scholar
Over, H., & Carpenter, M. (2009). Priming third-party ostracism increases affiliative imitation in childrenDevelopmental Science12(3), F1F8. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00820.xGoogle Scholar
Páez, D., Rimé, B., Basabe, N., Wlodarczyk, A., & Zumeta, L. (2015). Psychosocial effects of perceived emotional synchrony in collective gatherings. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108(5), 711–29. https://doi.org/10.1037/pspi0000014Google Scholar
Park, C. L. (2005). Religion as a meaning-making framework in coping with life stressJournal of Social Issues61(4), 707–29https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-4560.2005.00428.xGoogle Scholar
Park, C. L. (2011). Meaning and growth within positive psychology: Toward a more complete understanding. In Sheldon, K. M., Kashdan, T. B., & Steger, M. F. (Eds.), Designing positive psychology: Taking stock and moving forward (pp. 324–34). Oxford University Presshttps://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195373585.003.0021Google Scholar
Patzer, G. L. (1983). Source credibility as a function of communicator physical attractiveness. Journal of Business Research, 11(2), 229–41. https://doi.org/10.1016/0148-2963(83)90030-9Google Scholar
Piazza, J., Bering, J. M., & Ingram, G. (2011). “Princess Alice is watching you”: Children’s belief in an invisible person inhibits cheating. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 109(3), 311–20. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2011.02.003Google Scholar
Platow, M. J., Voudouris, N. J., Coulson, M., Gilford, N., Jamieson, R., Najdovski, L., Papaleo, H., Pollard, C., & Terry, L. (2007). In-group reassurance in a pain setting produces lower levels of physiological arousal: Direct support for a self-categorization analysis of social influence. European Journal of Social Psychology, 37(4), 649–60. https://doi.org/10.1002/ejsp.381Google Scholar
Pornpitakpan, C. (2004). The persuasiveness of source credibility: A critical review of five decades’ evidence. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 34(2), 243–81. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.2004.tb02547.xGoogle Scholar
Purzycki, B. G. (2013). The minds of gods: A comparative study of supernatural agency. Cognition, 129(1), 163–79. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2013.06.010Google Scholar
Purzycki, B. G., Apicella, C., Atkinson, Q. D., Cohen, E., McNamara, R. A., Willard, A. K., Xygalatas, D., Norenzayan, A., & Henrich, J. (2016). Moralistic gods, supernatural punishment and the expansion of human sociality. Nature, 530(7590), 327–30. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature16980Google Scholar
Purzycki, B. G., Henrich, J., Apicella, C., Atkinson, Q. D., Baimel, A., Cohen, E., McNamara, R. A., Willard, A. K., Xygalatas, D., & Norenzayan, A. (2017). The evolution of religion and morality: A synthesis of ethnographic and experimental evidence from eight societies. Religion, Brain and Behavior, 8(2), 101–32. https://doi.org/10.1080/2153599X.2016.1267027Google Scholar
Purzycki, B. G., & Sosis, R. (2011). Our gods: Variation in supernatural minds. In Frey, U. J., Störmer, C., & Willführ, K. P. (Eds.), Essential building blocks of human nature (pp. 7793). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-642-13968-0_5Google Scholar
Putnam, R. D., & Campbell, D. E. (2012). American grace: How religion divides and unites us. Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Pyysiäinen, I. (2003). Buddhism, religion and the concept of “God.” Numen International Review for the History of Religions, 50(2), 147–71. https://doi.org/10.1163/156852703321506141Google Scholar
Pyysiäinen, I. (2009). Supernatural agents: Why we believe in souls, gods, and Buddhas. Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195380026.001.0001Google Scholar
Rabinowitch, T. C., & Knafo-Noam, A. (2015). Synchronous rhythmic interaction enhances children’s perceived similarity and closeness towards each other. PLoS ONE, 10(4), e0120878 https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0120878Google Scholar
Randolph-Seng, B., & Nielsen, M. E. (2007). Honesty: One effect of primed religious representations. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 17(4), 303–15. https://doi.org/10.1080/10508610701572812Google Scholar
Rappaport, R. A. (1999). Ritual and religion in the making of humanity. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Reader, I., & Tanabe, G. J. Jr. (1998). Practically religious: Worldly benefits and the common religion of Japan. University of Hawai’i Press.Google Scholar
Reddish, P., Bulbulia, J. A., & Fischer, R. (2014). Does synchrony promote generalized prosociality? Religion, Brain and Behavior, 4(1), 319. https://doi.org/10.1080/2153599x.2013.764545Google Scholar
Reddish, P., Fischer, R., & Bulbulia, J. A. (2013). Let’s dance together: Synchrony, shared intentionality and cooperation. PLoS ONE, 8(8), e71182. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0071182Google Scholar
Reddish, P., Tong, E. M. W., Jong, J., Lanman, J. A., & Whitehouse, H. (2016). Collective synchrony increases prosociality towards non-performers and outgroup members. British Journal of Social Psychology, 55(4), 722–38. https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12165Google Scholar
Rennung, M., & Göritz, A. S. (2016). Prosocial consequences of interpersonal synchrony: A meta-analysis. Zeitschrift für Psychologie/Journal of Psychology, 224, 168–89. https://doi.org/10.1027/2151-2604/a000252Google Scholar
Ross, L., & Nisbett, R. (1991). McGraw-Hill series in social psychology. The person and the situation. McGraw-Hill Book Company.Google Scholar
Rossano, M. J. (2012). The essential role of ritual in the transmission and reinforcement of social norms. Psychological Bulletin, 138(3), 529–49. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0027038Google Scholar
Ruffle, B., & Sosis, R. (2010). Do religious contexts elicit more trust and altruism? An experiment on Facebook. Discussion Paper No. 10–02, Department of Economics: Ben-Gurion University, (860), 1–30. https://doi.org/10.2139/ssrn.1566123Google Scholar
Saroglou, V. (2006). Religion’s role in prosocial behavior: Myth or reality? Psychology of Religion Newsletter: American Psychological Association Division 36, 31(2), 18. https://cdn.uclouvain.be/public/Exports reddot/psyreli/documents/Newsletter.pdfGoogle Scholar
Saroglou, V., Corneille, O., & Van Cappellen, P. (2009). “Speak, Lord, your servant is listening”: Religious priming activates submissive thoughts and behaviors. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 19(3), 143–54. https://doi.org/10.1080/10508610902880063Google Scholar
Scheepers, P., Gijsberts, M., & Hello, E. (2002). Religiosity and prejudice against ethnic minorities in Europe: Cross-national tests on a controversial relationship. Review of Religious Research, 43(3), 242–65. https://doi.org/10.2307/3512331Google Scholar
Schleihauf, H., Graetz, S., Pauen, S., & Hoehl, S. (2018). Contrasting social and cognitive accounts on overimitation: The role of causal transparency and prior experiencesChild Development89(3), 1039–55. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12780Google Scholar
Shariff, A. F., & Norenzayan, A. (2007). God is watching you: Priming God concepts increases prosocial behavior in an anonymous economic game. Psychological Science, 18(9), 803–9. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01983.xGoogle Scholar
Shariff, A. F., Willard, A. K., Andersen, T., & Norenzayan, A. (2015). Religious priming: A meta-analysis with a focus on prosociality. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 20(1), 2748. https://doi.org/10.1177/1088868314568811Google Scholar
Siegman, A. W. (1962). Personality and socio-cultural variables associated with religious behavior. Archiv für Religionspsychologie/Archive for the Psychology of Religion, 7(1), 96104. www.jstor.org/stable/23919300Google Scholar
Simmons, J. P., Nelson, L. D., & Simonsohn, U. (2011). False-positive psychology: Undisclosed flexibility in data collection and analysis allows presenting anything as significant. Psychological Science, 22(11), 1359–66. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611417632Google Scholar
Simons, H. W., Berkowitz, N. N., & Moyer, R. J. (1970). Similarity, credibility, and attitude change: A review and a theory. Psychological Bulletin, 73(1), 116. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0028429Google Scholar
Smith, R. E., Wheeler, G., & Diener, E. (1975). Faith without works: Jesus people, resistance to temptation, and altruism. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 5(4), 320–30. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1559-1816.1975.tb00684.xGoogle Scholar
Sosis, R. (2003). Why aren’t we all Hutterites? Costly signaling theory and religious behavior. Human Nature, 14(2), 91127. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-003-1000-6Google Scholar
Sosis, R., & Alcorta, C. S. (2003). Signalling, solidarity, and the sacred: The evolution of religious behavior. Evolutionary Anthropology, 12(6), 264–74. https://doi.org/10.1002/evan.10120Google Scholar
Sosis, R., & Bressler, E. R. (2003). Cooperation and commune longevity: A test of the costly signaling theory of religion. Cross-Cultural Research, 37(2), 211–39. https://doi.org/10.1177/1069397103037002003Google Scholar
Stel, M., & Vonk, R. (2010). Mimicry in social interaction: Benefits for mimickers, mimickees, and their interaction. British Journal of Psychology, 101(2), 311–23. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712609X465424Google Scholar
Strathern, A. (1970). Male initiation in New Guinea highlands societies. Ethnology, 9(4), 373–9. https://doi.org/10.2307/3773043Google Scholar
Strenski, I. (2006). Thinking about religion: An historical introduction to theories of religion. Blackwell Publishing.Google Scholar
Sutherland, L. (2012). Tylor and Neo-Tylorian approaches to the study of religion: Re-evaluating an important lineage in the theorisation of religion. Paranthropology: Journal of Anthropological Approaches to the Paranormal, 3(3), 4757.Google Scholar
Swann, W. B. Jr., Buhrmester, M. D., Gómez, A., Jetten, J., Bastian, B., Vázquez, A., Ariyanto, A., Besta, T., Christ, O., Cui, L., Finchilescu, G., González, R., Goto, N., Hornsey, M., Sharma, S., Susianto, H., & Zhang, A. (2014). What makes a group worth dying for? Identity fusion fosters perception of familial ties, promoting self-sacrifice. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106(6), 912–26. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0036089Google Scholar
Swann, W. B. Jr., Gómez, Á., Huici, C., Morales, J. F., & Hixon, J. G. (2010). Identity fusion and self-sacrifice: Arousal as a catalyst of pro-group fighting, dying, and helping behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 99(5), 824–41. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0020014Google Scholar
Swann, W. B. Jr., Gómez, Á., Seyle, D. C., Morales, J. F., & Huici, C. (2009). Identity fusion: The interplay of personal and social identities in extreme group behavior. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(5), 9951011. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013668Google Scholar
Swann, W. B. Jr., Jetten, J., Gómez, Á., Whitehouse, H., Bastian, B., Gómez, A., & Bastian, B. (2012). When group membership gets personal: A theory of identity fusion. Psychological Review, 119(3), 441–56. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0028589Google Scholar
Szerszynski, B. (2002). Ecological rites: Ritual action in environmental protest events. Theory, Culture & Society, 19(3), 5169. https://doi.org/10.1177/026327602401081521Google Scholar
Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (1985). The social identity theory of intergroup behavior. In Worchel, S. & Austin, W. G. (Eds.), Psychology of intergroup relations (2nd ed., Vol. 2, pp. 724). Nelson-Hall.Google Scholar
Taniguchi, Y., & Sanefuji, W. (2017). The boundaries of overimitation in preschool children: Effects of target and tool use on imitation of irrelevant actionsJournal of Experimental Child Psychology159, 8395. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2017.01.014Google Scholar
Tunçgenç, B., & Cohen, E. (2016). Interpersonal movement synchrony facilitates pro-social behavior in children’s peer-play. Developmental Science, 21(1), e12505. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12505Google Scholar
Tunçgenç, B., Cohen, E., & Fawcett, C. (2015). Rock with me: The role of movement synchrony in infants’ social and nonsocial choices. Child Development, 86(3), 976–84. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12354Google Scholar
Turchin, P., Brennan, R., Currie, T. E., Feeney, K. C., François, P., Hoyer, D., Manning, J. G., Marciniak, A., Mullins, D., Palmisano, A., Peregrine, P., Turner, E. A. L., & Whitehouse, H. (2015). Seshat: The global history databankCliodynamics6, 77107.Google Scholar
Turchin, P., Whitehouse, H., François, P., Hoyer, D., Nugent, S., Larson, J., Covey, A., Altaweel, M., Peregrine, P., Carballo, D., Feinman, G., Wallace, V., Bol, P., Korotayev, A., Kradin, N., Anderson, E., Savage, P., Cioni, E., Levine, J., … Brandl, E. (2019, November 20). Explaining the rise of moralizing religions: A test of competing hypotheses using the Seshat Databank. SocArXiv. https://doi.org/10.31235/osf.io/2v59jGoogle Scholar
Turner, V. (1969). The ritual process: Structure and anti-structure. Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
Turner, V. (1985). Betwixt and between: The liminal period in rites de passage. In Lehmann, A. C. & Myers, J. E. (Eds.), Magic, witchcraft, and religion: An anthropological study of the supernatural (pp. 4655). Mayfield Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Tylor, E. B. (1871). Primitive culture: Researches into the development of mythology, philosophy, religion, language, art, and custom. John Murray.Google Scholar
Valdesolo, P., & DeSteno, D. (2011). Synchrony and the social tuning of compassion. Emotion, 11(2), 262–66. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021302Google Scholar
van Baaren, R. B., Holland, R. W., Kawakami, K., & van Knippenberg, A. (2004). Mimicry and prosocial behavior. Psychological Science, 15(1), 71–4. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01501012.xGoogle Scholar
Van Elk, M., Matzke, D., Gronau, Q. F., Guan, M., Vandekerckhove, J., & Wagenmakers, E.-J. (2015). Meta-analyses are no substitute for registered replications: A skeptical perspective on religious priming. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1365. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01365Google Scholar
Van Gennep, A. (1960). The rites of passage (Vizedom, M. B. & Caffee, G. L., Trans.). University of Chicago Press. (Original work published 1909)Google Scholar
Van Raalte, J. L., Cornelius, A. E., Linder, D. E., & Brewer, B. W. (2007). The relationship between hazing and team cohesion. Journal of Sport Behavior, 30(4), 491507.Google Scholar
Van Tongeren, D. R., Raad, J. M., McIntosh, D. N., & Pae, J. (2013). The existential function of intrinsic religiousness: Moderation of effects of priming religion on intercultural tolerance and afterlife anxietyJournal for the Scientific Study of Religion52(3), 508–23. https://doi.org/10.1111/jssr.12053Google Scholar
Verkaaik, O. (2010). The cachet dilemma: Ritual and agency in new Dutch nationalismAmerican Ethnologist37(1), 6982. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1548-1425.2010.01242.xGoogle Scholar
Walster, E., Aronson, E., & Abrahams, D. (1966). On increasing the persuasiveness of a low prestige communicator. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 2(4), 325–42. https://doi.org/10.1016/0022-1031(66)90026-6Google Scholar
Watson-Jones, R. E., Legare, C. H., Whitehouse, H., & Clegg, J. M. (2014). Task-specific effects of ostracism on imitative fidelity in early childhood. Evolution and Human Behavior, 35(3), 204–10. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2014.01.004Google Scholar
Watson-Jones, R. E., Whitehouse, H., & Legare, C. H. (2016). In-group ostracism increases high-fidelity imitation in early childhood. Psychological Science, 27(1), 3442. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797615607205Google Scholar
Watts, J. A., Greenhill, S. J., Atkinson, Q. D., Currie, T. E., Bulbulia, J. A., & Gray, R. D. (2015). Broad supernatural punishment but not moralizing high gods precede the evolution of political complexity in Austronesia. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 282(1804), 20142556. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2014.2556Google Scholar
Weber, M. (1930). The Protestant work ethic and the spirit of capitalism. George Allen and Unwin.Google Scholar
West, S. A., Gardner, A., Shuker, D. M., Reynolds, T., Burton-Chellow, M., Sykes, E. M., Guinnee, M. A., & Griffin, A. S. (2006). Cooperation and the scale of competition in humans. Current Biology, 16(11), 1103–6. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2006.03.069Google Scholar
Whitehouse, H. (1995). Inside the cult: Religious innovation and transmission in Papua New Guinea. Oxford studies in social and cultural anthropology. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Whitehouse, H. (2000). Arguments and icons: Divergent modes of religiosity. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Whitehouse, H. (2002). Religious reflexivity and transmissive frequency. Social Anthropology, 10(1), 91103. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1469-8676.2002.tb00048.xGoogle Scholar
Whitehouse, H. (2004). Modes of religiosity: A cognitive theory of religious transmission. AltaMira Press.Google Scholar
Whitehouse, H. (2011). The coexistence problem in psychology, anthropology, and evolutionary theory: Commentary on Evans & Lane, Harris, Legare & Visala, and Subbotsky. Human Development, 54(3), 191–9. https://doi.org/10.1159/000329149Google Scholar
Whitehouse, H. (2012). Ritual, cognition, and evolution. In Sun, R. (Ed.), Grounding the social sciences in the cognitive sciences (pp. 265–84). MIT Press.Google Scholar
Whitehouse, H. (2013). Religion, cohesion and hostility. In Clarke, S., Powell, R., & Savulescu, J. (Eds.), Religion, intolerance and conflict: A scientific and conceptual investigation (pp. 3647). Oxford University Press. https://doi.org/10.1093/acprof:oso/9780199640911.003.0002Google Scholar
Whitehouse, H., François, P., Savage, P. E., Currie, T. E., Feeney, K. C., Cioni, E., Purcell, R., Ross, R. M., Larson, J., Baines, J., ter Haar, B., Covey, A., & Turchin, P. (2019). Complex societies precede moralizing gods throughout world history. Nature, 568, 226–229.Google Scholar
Whitehouse, H., Jong, J., Buhrmester, M. D., Gómez, Á., Bastian, B., Kavanagh, C. M., Newson, M., Matthews, M., Lanman, J. A., McKay, R., & Gavrilets, S. (2017). The evolution of extreme cooperation via shared dysphoric experiences. Scientific Reports, 7, 44292. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep44292Google Scholar
Whitehouse, H., & Laidlaw, J. (Eds.). (2004). Ritual and memory: Towards a comparative anthropology of religion. AltaMira Press.Google Scholar
Whitehouse, H., & Laidlaw, J. (Eds.). (2007). Religion, anthropology and cognitive science. Carolina Academic Press.Google Scholar
Whitehouse, H., & Lanman, J. A. (2014). The ties that bind us: Ritual, fusion, and identification. Current Anthropology, 55(6), 674–95. https://doi.org/10.1086/678698Google Scholar
Whitehouse, H., & McCauley, R. N. (Eds.). (2005). Mind and religion: Psychological and cognitive foundations of religiosity. AltaMira Press.Google Scholar
Whitehouse, H., McQuinn, B., Buhrmester, M. D., & Swann, W. B. Jr. (2014). Brothers in arms: Libyan revolutionaries bond like family. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 111(50), 17783–5. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1416284111Google Scholar
Whiten, A., McGuigan, N., Marshall-Pescini, S., & Hopper, L. M. (2009). Emulation, imitation, over-imitation and the scope of culture for child and chimpanzee. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 3641(1528), 2417–28. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2009.0069Google Scholar
Whiten, A., Allan, G., Devlin, S., Kseib, N., Raw, N., & McGuigan, N. (2016). Social learning in the real-world: ‘Over-imitation’ occurs in both children and adults unaware of participation in an experiment and independently of social interaction. PLoS ONE, 11(7), e0159920. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0159920Google Scholar
Whitley, R. (2012). “Thank you God”: Religion and recovery from dual diagnosis among low-income African Americans. Transcultural Psychiatry, 49(1), 87104. https://doi.org/10.1177/1363461511425099Google Scholar
Wiech, K., Farias, M., Kahane, G., Shackel, N., Tiede, W., & Tracey, I. (2008). An fMRI study measuring analgesia enhanced by religion as a belief system. Pain, 139(2), 467–76. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pain.2008.07.030Google Scholar
Willard, A. K., & Cingl, L. (2017). Testing theories of secularization and religious belief in the Czech Republic and Slovakia. Evolution and Human Behavior, 38(5), 604–15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.01.002Google Scholar
Willard, A. K., Henrich, J., & Norenzayan, A. (2016). Memory and belief in the transmission of counterintuitive content. Human Nature, 27(3), 221–43. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-016-9259-6Google Scholar
Williams, K. D., & Jarvis, B. (2006). Cyberball: A program for use in research on interpersonal ostracism and acceptanceBehavior Research Methods38(1), 174–80. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03192765Google Scholar
Wilson, D. S. (2010). Darwin’s cathedral: Evolution, religion, and the nature of society. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Wiltermuth, S. S., & Heath, C. (2009). Synchrony and cooperation. Psychological Science, 20(1), 15. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02253.xGoogle Scholar
Wood, L. A., Harrison, R. A., Lucas, A. J., McGuigan, N., Burdett, E. R. R., & Whiten, A. (2016). “Model age-based” and “copy when uncertain” biases in children’s social learning of a novel taskJournal of Experimental Child Psychology150, 272–84. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2016.06.005Google Scholar
Xygalatas, D., Klocová, E. K., Cigán, J., Kundt, R., Maňo, P., Kotherová, S., Mitkidis, P., Wallot, S., & Kanovsky, M. (2016). Location, location, location: Effects of cross-religious primes on prosocial behavior. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 26(4), 304–19. https://doi.org/10.1080/10508619.2015.1097287Google Scholar
Xygalatas, D., Mitkidis, P., Fischer, R., Reddish, P., Skewes, J., Geertz, A. W., Roepstorff, A., & Bulbulia, J. (2013). Extreme rituals promote prosociality. Psychological Science, 24(8), 1602–5. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797612472910Google Scholar
Young, F. W. (1965). Initiation ceremonies: A cross-cultural study of status dramatization. Bobbs-Merrill.Google Scholar
Younger, J., Aron, A., Parke, S., Chatterjee, N., & Mackey, S. (2010). Viewing pictures of a romantic partner reduces experimental pain: Involvement of neural reward systems. PLoS ONE, 5(10), e13309. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0013309Google Scholar
Zahavi, A., & Zahavi, A. (1997). The handicap principle: A missing piece of Darwin’s puzzle. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×