Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T14:24:52.410Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 15 - Teaching about Language by Integrating Culture

from Cognition

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 March 2018

Kenneth D. Keith
Affiliation:
University of San Diego
Get access

Summary

Language is a central topic in modern psychological science. In addition to longstanding interest in the psychology of language in its own right, language is an important topic within many subfields of psychology, such as developmental psychology, cognition, perception, neuroscience, comparative psychology, and multicultural psychology. The study of language is also important because of its role in the historical development of psychology. I propose that instructors can integrate cultural concepts into a course without substantially reducing other content about language. An efficient way to integrate culture into the teaching of language is to use cultural examples as a tool for helping students understand language concepts. This approach supports effective teaching of language concepts via the integration of cultural content.
Type
Chapter
Information
Culture across the Curriculum
A Psychology Teacher's Handbook
, pp. 318 - 338
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2018

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Altarriba, J. (2002). Bilingualism: Language, memory and applied issues. Online Readings in Psychology and Culture, 4(2). doi:10.9707/2307-0919.1034Google Scholar
Blasi, D., Wichmann, S., Hammarstrom, H., Stadler, P., & Christiansen, M. (2016). Sound-meaning association biases evidenced across thousands of languages. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113, 1081810823. doi:10.1073/pnas.1605782113CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bransford, J. D., & Johnson, M. K. (1972). Contextual prerequisites for understanding: Some investigations of comprehension and recall. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11, 717726. doi:10.1016/S0022-5371(72)80006-9Google Scholar
Bruce, D. (1994). Lashley and the problem of serial order. American Psychologist, 49, 93103. doi:10.1037/0003-066X.49.2.93CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, D. (2004). Web-based assignments in the psychology of language class. Teaching of Psychology, 31, 204206. www.teachpsych.org/top/index.phpGoogle Scholar
Carroll, D., & Pinnow, E. (2011). Engaging students in the psychology of language. In Miller, R. L., Balcetis, E., Burns, S. R., Daniel, D. B., Saville, B. K., & Woody, W. D. (Eds.), Promoting student engagement (Vol. 2, pp. 92–95). Retrieved from the Society for the Teaching of Psychology web site, http://teachpsych.org/ebooks/pse2011/vol2/index.phpGoogle Scholar
Chiu, C. (2011). Language and culture. Online Readings in Psychology and Culture, 4(2). doi:10.9707/2307-0919.1098Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1959). Verbal behavior [Review]. Language, 35, 2658. doi:10.2307/411334Google Scholar
Cook, V., & Bassetti, B. (2011). Language and bilingual cognition. New York, NY: Psychology Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crago, M. B., Allen, S. E. M., & Hough-Eyamie, W. P. (1997). Exploring innateness through cultural and linguistic variation. In Gopnik, M. (Ed.), The biological basis of language (pp. 7090). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Diekhoff, G. M. (2008). The role of expectancies in the perception of language. In Benjamin, L. T. (Ed.), Favorite activities for the teaching of psychology (pp. 145147). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Dunn, D. S., & Hammer, E. D. (2014). On teaching multicultural psychology. In Leong, F. T. L. (Ed.), APA handbook of multicultural psychology: Vol. 1. Theory and research (pp. 4358). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Everett, D. L. (2005). Cultural constraints on grammar and cognition in Pirahã. Current Anthropology, 46, 621646. doi:10.1086/431525Google Scholar
Fernald, P. S., & Fernald, L. D. (2008). Early motor and verbal development. In Benjamin, L. T. (Ed.), Favorite activities for the teaching of psychology (pp. 180181). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Gass, S. M., & Maronis, E. M. (1991). Miscommunication in nonnative speaker discourse. In Coupland, H. G. N., Giles, H., & Wiemann, J. M. (Eds.),Miscommunication” and problem talk (pp. 121145). Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications.Google Scholar
Gathercole, V. M., & Hoff, E. (2007). Input and the acquisition of language: Three questions. In Hoff, E., Shatz, M., Hoff, E., & Shatz, M. (Eds.), Blackwell handbook of language development (pp. 107127). Oxford: Blackwell. doi:10.1002/9780470757833.ch6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grice, H. P. (1978). Further notes on logic and conversation. In Adler, J. A. & Rips, L. J. (Eds.), Reasoning: Studies of human inference and its foundations (pp. 765773). Cambridge, MA: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Hill, A. A. (1952). A note on primitive languages. International Journal of American Linguistics, 18, 172177. doi:10.1086/464167Google Scholar
Hockett, C. (1966). The problem of universals in language. In Greenberg, J. H. (Ed.), Universals of language (2nd ed., pp. 129). Cambridge, MA: Massachusetts Institute of Technology Press.Google Scholar
Hoff, E. (2009). Language development (4th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.Google Scholar
Holtgraves, T. (1997). Styles of language use: Individual and cultural variability in conversational indirectness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 624637. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.73.3.624Google Scholar
Joseph, J. E., & Newmeyer, F. J. (2012). “All languages are equally complex”: The rise and fall of a consensus. Historiographia Linguistica, 2, 341368. doi:10.1075/hl.39.2-3.08josGoogle Scholar
Kite, M. E., & Whitley, B. E. (2012). Ethnic and nationality stereotypes in everyday language. Teaching of Psychology, 39, 5456. doi:10.1177/0098628311430314Google Scholar
Krumov, K., & Larsen, K. S. (2013). The evolution of language and socio-culture. In Krumov, K. & Larsen, K. S., Cross-cultural psychology: Why culture matters (pp. 109142). Charlotte, NC: Information Age.Google Scholar
Lashley, K. S. (1951). The problem of serial order in behavior. In Jeffress, L. A (Ed.), Cerebral mechanisms in behavior: The Hixon Symposium (pp. 112146). New York, NY: Wiley.Google Scholar
Lee, J. S. (2005). Embracing diversity through the understanding of pragmatics. In Denham, K. & Lobeck, A. (Eds.), Language in the schools: Integrating linguistic knowledge into K-12 teaching (pp. 1727). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Miller, G. A. (1990). The place of language in a scientific psychology. Psychological Science, 1, 714. doi:10.1111/j.1467-9280.1990.tb00059.xGoogle Scholar
Parama, K., Kreiner, D. S., Stark, K., & Schuetz, S. (2017). Monolingual and bilingual perceptions of code-switching: A difference in cognition but not competence. North American Journal of Psychology, 19, 87102. http://najp.us/north-american-journal-of-psychology-indexGoogle Scholar
Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct: How the mind creates language. New York, NY: William Morrow.Google Scholar
Rawson, K., Thomas, R., & Jacoby, L. L. (2015). The power of examples: Illustrative examples enhance conceptual learning of declarative concepts. Educational Psychology Review, 27, 483504. doi:10.1007/s10648-014-9273-3Google Scholar
Roberts, C., Moss, B., Wass, V., Sarangi, S., & Jones, R. (2005). Misunderstandings: A qualitative study of primary care consultations in multilingual settings, and educational implications. Medical Education, 39, 465475. doi:10.1111/j.1365-2929.2005.02121.xGoogle Scholar
Ronkin, M., & Karn, H. E. (1999). Mock Ebonics: Linguistic racism in parodies of Ebonics on the internet. Journal of Sociolinguistics, 3, 360380. doi:10.1111/1467-9481.00083Google Scholar
Sandler, W., Meir, I., Padden, C., & Aronoff, M. (2005). The emergence of grammar: Systematic structure in a new language. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 102, 26612665. doi:10.1073/pnas.0405448102Google Scholar
Scancarelli, J. (1994). Another look at a “primitive language.” International Journal of American Linguistics, 60, 149160. doi:10.1086/466227Google Scholar
Whorf, B. (1950). An American Indian model of the universe. International Journal of American Linguistics, 16, 6772. doi:10.1086/464066CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wong, J. (2010). The “triple articulation” of language. Journal of Pragmatics, 42, 29322944. doi:10.1016/j.pragma.2010.06.013Google Scholar
Zhang, Y., Jin, X., Shen, X., Zhang, J., & Hoff, E. (2008). Correlates of early language development in Chinese children. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 32, 145151. doi:10.1177/0165025407087213Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×