Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T20:30:13.119Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

References

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  02 June 2022

Patrick McNamara
Affiliation:
Boston University
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
The Cognitive Neuroscience of Religious Experience
Decentering and the Self
, pp. 238 - 254
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2022

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abramowitz, J. S., & Jacoby, R. J. (2014). Scrupulosity: A cognitive–behavioral analysis and implications for treatment. Journal of Obsessive-Compulsive and Related Disorders, 3, 140–149.Google Scholar
Ackermann, S., & Rasch, B. (2014). Differential effects of non-REM and REM sleep on memory consolidation? Current Neurology and Neuroscience Reports, 14, 430–438. https://doi.org/10.1007/s11910-013-0430-8Google Scholar
Andersen, M. (2019). Predictive coding in agency detection. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 9(1), 65–84. https://doi.org/10.1080/2153599X.2017.1387170Google Scholar
Arzy, S., & Idel, M. (2015). Kabbala: A neurocognitive approach to mystical experiences. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Asma, S. (2017). The evolution of imagination. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Atasoy, S., Donnelly, I., & Pearson, J. (2016). Human brain networks function in connectome-specific harmonic waves. Nature Communications, 7, 1–10. https://doi.org/10.1038/ncomms10340Google Scholar
Atasoy, S., Roseman, L., Kaelen, M., et al. (2017). Connectome-harmonic decomposition of human brain activity reveals dynamical repertoire re-organization under LSD. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 1–18. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-17546-0Google Scholar
Azari, N. P., Nickel, J. P., Wunderlich, G., et al. (2001). Neural correlates of religious experience. European Journal of Neuroscience, 13, 1649–1652.Google Scholar
Aznar, S., & Hervig, M. E.-S. (2016). The 5-HT2A serotonin receptor in executive function: Implications for neuropsychiatric and neurodegenerative diseases. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 64, 63–82.Google Scholar
Barrett, F. S., Doss, M. K., Sepeda, N. D., et al. (2020). Emotions and brain function are altered up to one month after a single high dose of psilocybin. Scientific Reports, 10(1), 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-59282-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barrett, F. S., & Griffiths, R. R. (2018). Classic hallucinogens and mystical experiences: Phenomenology and neural correlates. Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences, 36, 393–430. https://doi.org/10.1007/7854_2017_474Google Scholar
Barrett, F. S., Johnson, M. W., & Griffiths, R. R. (2015). Validation of the revised Mystical Experience Questionnaire in experimental sessions with psilocybin. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 29(11), 1182–1190.Google Scholar
Bauer, J. J. (2021). The transformative self: Personal growth, narrative identity and the good life. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bear, D. M., & Fedio, P. (1977). Quantitative analysis of interictal behavior in temporal lobe epilepsy. Archives of Neurology, 34(8), 454–467.Google Scholar
Beauregard, M., & O’Leary, D. (2007). The spiritual brain: A neuroscientist’s case for the existence of the soul. San Francisco: HarperCollins.Google Scholar
Beauregard, M., & Paquette, V. (2006). Neural correlates of a mystical experience in Carmelite nuns. Neuroscience Letters, 405(3), 186–190. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2006.06.060Google Scholar
Bell, C. (1997). Ritual: Perspectives and dimensions. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bernstein, A., Hadash, Y., Lichtash, Y., et al. (2015). Decentering and related constructs: A critical review and metacognitive processes model. Perspectives in Psychological Science, 10(5), 599–617.Google Scholar
Blanke, O. (2004). Out of body experiences and their neural basis. BMJ, 329(7480), 1414–1415. https://doi.org/10.1136/bmj.329.7480.1414Google Scholar
Blanke, O., & Dieguez, S. (2009). Leaving body and life behind: Out-of-body and near-death experience. In Laureys, S. & Tononi, G. (Eds.), The neurology of consciousness (pp. 303–325). London: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Block, N., & Miller, B. (2020). Religion and frontotemporal dementia. In Coles, A. & Collicult, J. (Eds.), Neurology and religion (pp. 161–170). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Boyer, P. (2001). Religion explained: The evolutionary origins of religious thought. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Boyer, P., & Liénard, P. (2020). Ingredients of ‘rituals’ and their cognitive underpinnings. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 375(1805), 20190439. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2019.0439Google Scholar
Bradbury, J. W., & Vehrenkamp, S. L. (1998). Principles of animal communication. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Associates.Google Scholar
Braun, A. R., Balkin, T. J., Wesenten, N. J., Carson, R. E., Varga, M., Baldwin, P., Selbie, S., Belenky, G., & Herscovitch, P. (1997). Regional cerebral blood flow throughout the sleep-wake cycle. An H2(15)O PET study. Brain, 120(7), 1173–1197. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/120.7.1173Google Scholar
Brouwer, A., & Carhart-Harris, R. L. (2021). Pivotal mental states. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 35(4), 319–352. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881120959637Google Scholar
Brüne, M. (2004). Schizophrenia – an evolutionary enigma? Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 28(1), 41–53.Google Scholar
Bruner, E., Fedato, A., Silva-Gago, M., et al. (2018). Cognitive archeology, body cognition, and hand-tool interaction. Progress in Brain Research, 238, 325–345. https://doi.org/10.1016/bs.pbr.2018.06.013. Epub 2018 Jul 14. PMID: 30097198 Review.Google Scholar
Bulbulia, J., Geertz, A. W., Atkinson, Q. D., et al. (2013). The cultural evolution of religion. In Richerson, P. J. & Christiansen, M. H. (Eds.), Cultural evolution (pp. 381–404). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Bulkeley, K. (2016). Big dreams: The science of dreaming and the origins of religion. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Bullmore, E., & Sporns, O. (2009). Complex brain networks: Graph theoretical analysis of structural and functional systems. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(3), 186–198. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2575Google Scholar
Burkert, W., Girard, R., Smith, J. Z., & Hamerton-Kelly, R. (1987). Violent origins: Walter Burkert, René Girard and Jonathan Z. Smith on ritual killing and cultural formation. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Butler, P. M., McNamara, P., & Durso, R. (2010). Deficits in the automatic activation of religious concepts in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society, 16(2), 252–261. PMID: 19958570.Google Scholar
Butler, P. M., McNamara, P., & Durso, R. (2011a). Side of onset in Parkinson’s disease and alterations in religiosity: Novel behavioral phenotypes. Behavioural Neurology, 24(2), 133–141. https://doi.org/10.3233/BEN-2011-0282Google Scholar
Butler, P. M., McNamara, P., Ghofrani, J., & Durso, R. (2011b). Disease-associated differences in religious cognition in patients with Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 33(8), 917–928. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803395.2011.575768Google Scholar
Buzsáki, G. (1996). The hippocampo-neocortical dialogue. Cerebral Cortex, 6, 81–92. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/6.2.81Google Scholar
Call, J., & Tomasello, M. (2007). Intentional communication in nonhuman primates. In Call, J. & Tomasello, M. (Eds.), The gestural communication of apes and monkeys (pp. 1–15). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Carhart-Harris, R. L., Erritzoe, D., Williams, T., et al. (2012). Neural correlates of the psychedelic state as determined by fMRI studies with psilocybin. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(6), 2138–2143.Google Scholar
Carhart-Harris, R. L., & Friston, K. J. (2019). REBUS and the anarchic brain: Toward a unified model of the brain action of psychedelics. Pharmacological Review, 71(3), 316–344. https://doi.org/10.1124/pr.118.017160. PMID: 31221820.Google Scholar
Carhart-Harris, R. L., Leech, R., Hellyer, P. J., et al. (2014). The entropic brain: A theory of conscious states informed by neuroimaging research with psychedelic drugs. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 1–22. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00020Google Scholar
Carhart-Harris, R., & Nutt, D. (2014). Was it a vision or a waking dream? Frontiers in Psychology, 5, 255. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00255. PMID: 24772095.Google Scholar
Carhart-Harris, R. L., Muthukumaraswamy, S., Roseman, L., et al. (2016). Neural correlates of the LSD experience revealed by multimodal neuroimaging. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(17), 4853–4858.Google Scholar
Cauda, F., Geminiani, G. C., & Vercelli, A. (2014). Evolutionary appearance of von Economo’s neurons in the mammalian cerebral cortex. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 104. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00104. PMID: 24672457.Google Scholar
Chan, D., Anderson, V., Pijnenburg, Y., et al. (2009). The clinical profile of right temporal lobe atrophy. Brain, 132, 1287–1298. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awp037Google Scholar
Charland-Verville, V., Ribeiro de Paula, D., Martial, C., et al. (2020). Characterization of near death experiences using text mining analyses: A preliminary study. PLoS One, 15(1), e0227402. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0227402. eCollection 2020. PMID: 31999716.Google Scholar
Chilton, P., & Kopytowska, M. (Eds.) (2018). Religion, language, and the human mind. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cho, A., & Forster, D., (2017). The religious linguistic characteristics of the presence of the Kingdom in the light of Speech Act Theory: Christian ethical implications. Scriptura, 116(1), 1–12. https://doi.org/10.7833/116-1-1223Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2015). Surfing uncertainty: Prediction, action, and the embodied mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Cooper, R., & Shallice, T. (2000). Contention scheduling and the control of routine activities. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 17(4), 297–338. https://doi.org/10.1080/026432900380427. PMID: 20945185.Google Scholar
Craig, A. D. (2009). How do you feel – now? The anterior insula and human awareness. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(1), 59–70. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2555. PMID: 19096369 Review.Google Scholar
Crescentini, C., Aglioti, S. M., Fabbro, F., & Urgesi, C. (2014). Virtual lesions of the inferior parietal cortex induce fast changes of implicit religiousness/spirituality. Cortex, 54, 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2014.01.023Google Scholar
Crespi, B., & Badcock, C. (2008). Psychosis and autism as diametrical disorders of the social brain. Behavioral Brain Sciences, 31(3), 241–261; discussion 261–320. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X08004214. PMID: 18578904.Google Scholar
Crespi, B, Summers, K., & Dorus, S. (2007). Adaptive evolution of genes underlying schizophrenia. Proceedings of the Royal Society: Biological Science, 274(1627), 2801–2810. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2007.0876Google Scholar
Cristofori, I., Bulbulia, J., Shaver, J. H., et al. (2016). Neural correlates of mystical experience. Neuropsychologia, 80, 212–220. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2015.11.021Google Scholar
Crow, T. J. (1993). Sexual selection, Machiavellian intelligence, and the origins of psychosis. Lancet, 342, 594–598.Google Scholar
Crow, T. J. (1997). Is schizophrenia the price that Homo sapiens pays for language? Schizophrenia Research, 28, 127–141.Google Scholar
Crow, T. J. (2000a). Schizophrenia as the price that Homo sapiens pays for language: A resolution of the central paradox in the origin of the species. Brain Research Reviews, 31, 118–129.Google Scholar
Crow, T. J. (2000b). Sexual selection, timing and an X-Y homologous gene: Did Homo sapiens speciate on the Y chromosome? Psycholoquy, 11, 1–18.Google Scholar
Crow, T. J. (Ed.) (2002). The speciation of modern homo sapiens. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Crow, T. (2008). The ‘big bang’ theory of the origin of psychosis and the faculty of language. Schizophrenia Research, 102, 31–52.Google Scholar
d’Aquili, E., & Newberg, A. (1993). Religious and mystical states: A neuropsychological model. Zygon, 28(2), 177–200.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. (1991). Somatic markers and the guidance of behavior. New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dannemann, M., & Kelso, J. (2017). The contribution of Neanderthals to phenotypic variation in modern humans. American Journal of Human Genetics, 101(4), 578–589. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajhg.2017.09.010. PMID: 28985494.Google Scholar
David, N., Newen, A., & Vogeley, K. (2008). The “sense of agency” and its underlying cognitive and neural mechanisms. Consciousness and Cognition, 17(2), 523–534.Google Scholar
Davie, M. R. (1929). The evolution of war: A study of its role in early society. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Davis, A. K., Clifton, J. M., Weaver, E. G., et al. (2020). Survey of entity encounter experiences occasioned by inhaled N,N-dimethyltryptamine: Phenomenology, interpretation, and enduring effects. Psychopharmacology, 34(9), 1008–1020. https://doi.org/10.1177/0269881120916143. Epub 2020 Apr 28. PMID: 32345112.Google Scholar
Davis, E. B., Moriarty, G. L., & Mauch, J. C. (2013). God images and god concepts: Definitions, development, and dynamics. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality, 5(1), 51–60.Google Scholar
Davis, E. B., Granqvist, P., & Sharp, C. (2021). Theistic relational spirituality: Development, dynamics, health, and transformation. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality, 13(4), 401–415. https://doi.org/10.1037/rel0000219Google Scholar
Denis, D., French, C. C., & Gregory, A. M. (2018). A systematic review of variables associated with sleep paralysis. Sleep Medical Review, 38, 141–157. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.smrv.2017.05.005Google Scholar
Devinsky, O., & Lai, G. (2008). Spirituality and religion in epilepsy. Epilepsy & Behavior, 12(4), 636–643. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yebeh.2007.11.011Google Scholar
Dewhurst, K., & Beard, A. W. (1970). Sudden religious conversions in temporal lobe epilepsy. The British Journal of Psychiatry: The Journal of Mental Science, 117, 497–507.Google Scholar
Dissanayake, E. (2006). Ritual and ritualization: Musical means of conveying and shaping emotion in humans and other animals. In Brown, S. & Volgsten, U. (Eds.), Music and manipulation: On the social uses and social control of music (pp. 31–56). Oxford and New York: Berghahn.Google Scholar
Doblin, R. (1991). Pahnke’s Good Friday experiment: A long term follow up and methodological critique. Journal of Transpersonal Psychology, 23(1), 1–28.Google Scholar
Dolgoff-Kaspar, R., Ettinger, A. B., Golub, S. A., Perrine, K., Harden, C., & Croll, S. D. (2011). Numinous-like auras and spirituality in persons with partial seizures. Epilepsia, 52(3), 640–644.Google Scholar
Downes, W. (2011). Language and religion. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dressler, W. W., Balieiro, M. C., Ribeiro, R. P., & Santos, J. E. D. (2009). Cultural consonance, a 5HT2A receptor polymorphism, and depressive symptoms: A longitudinal study of 42 gene × culture interaction in urban Brazil. American Journal of Human Biology: The Official Journal of the Human Biology Association, 21(1), 91–97.Google Scholar
Du Bois, J. W. (1986). Self-evidence and ritual speech. In Chafe, W. & Nichols, J. (Eds.), Evidentiality: The linguistic coding of epistemology (pp. 313–336). Norwood, NJ: Ablex.Google Scholar
Dunbar, R. I. (2009). The social brain hypothesis and its implications for social evolution. Annals of Human Biology, 36(5), 562–572. https://doi.org/10.1080/03014460902960289Google Scholar
Duranti, A. (2005). Agency in language. In Duranti, A. (Ed.), A companion to linguistic anthropology (pp. 451–473). London: Blackwell.Google Scholar
Eisen, J. L., Goodman, W. K., Keller, M. B., et al. (1999). Patterns of remission and relapse in obsessive-compulsive disorder: A 2-year prospective study. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 60, 346–351.Google Scholar
Epley, N., Converse, B. A., Delbosc, A., Monteleone, G. A., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2009). Believers’ estimates of God’s beliefs are more egocentric than estimates of other people’s beliefs. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(51), 21533–21538. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0908374106Google Scholar
Farias, M., van Mulukom, V., Kahane, G., et al. (2017). Supernatural belief is not modulated by intuitive thinking style or cognitive inhibition. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 15100. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-017-14090-9Google Scholar
Feinberg, T. E., & Keenan, J. P. (2005). Where in the brain is the self? Consciousness and Cognition, 14(4), 661–678.Google Scholar
Ferguson, M. A., Nielsen, J. A., King, J. B., et al. (2018). Reward, salience, and attentional networks are activated by religious experience in devout Mormons. Social Neuroscience, 13(1), 104–116. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470919.2016.1257437Google Scholar
Ferguson, M. A., Schaper, F. L. W. V. J., Cohen, A., et al. (2022). A neural circuit for spirituality and religiosity derived from patients with brain lesions. Biological Psychiatry, 91(4), 380–388. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsych.2021.06.016Google Scholar
Fox, K. C. R., Nijeboer, S., Solomonova, E., Domhoff, G. W., & Christoff, K. (2013). Dreaming as mind wandering: Evidence from functional neuroimaging and first-person content reports. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, 412.Google Scholar
Friston, K. (2009). The free-energy principle: A rough guide to the brain? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(7), 293–301. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2009.04.005Google Scholar
Friston, K. (2010). The free-energy principle: A unified brain theory? Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(2), 127–138. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2787Google Scholar
Friston, K., FitzGerald, T., Rigoli, F., Schwartenbeck, P., & Pezzulo, G. (2017). Active inference: A process theory. Neural Computation, 29(1), 1–49.Google Scholar
Friston, K., Mattout, J., & Kilner, J. (2011). Action understanding and active inference. Biological Cybernetics, 104(1–2), 137–160.Google Scholar
Gastaut, H. (1954). Epilepsies. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas Publisher.Google Scholar
Gavrilets, S., & Rice, W. (2014). The genetics and biology of sexual conflict (Cold Spring Harbor perspectives in biology) Cold Spring Harbor, MA: Cold Spring Harbour Press.Google Scholar
Gearing, R. E., Alonzo, D., Smolak, A., et al. (2011). Association of religion with delusions and hallucinations in the context of schizophrenia: Implications for engagement and adherence. Schizophrenia Research, 126(1–3), 150–163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geschwind, N. (1979). Behavioural changes in temporal lobe epilepsy. Psychological Medicine, 9, 217–219.Google Scholar
Geschwind, M., & Picard, F. (2016). Ecstatic epileptic seizures: A glimpse into the multiple roles of the insula. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnbeh.2016.00021Google Scholar
Giaquinto, S., Bruti, L., Dall’Armi, V., Palma, E., & Spiridigliozzi, C. (2011). Religious and spiritual beliefs in outpatients suffering from Parkinson disease. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 26(9), 916–922. https://doi.org/10.1002/gps.2624Google Scholar
Girard, R. (1977). Violence and the sacred. Baltimore, MD: John Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
Gómez Emilsson, A. (2016). “Consciousness research,” Qualia Computing. https://qualiacomputing.com/category/consciousness-research/page/2/Google Scholar
Granqvist, P. (2020). Attachment in religion and spirituality: A wider view. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Greenberg, D., Witztum, E., & Pisante, J. (1987). Scrupulosity: Religious attitudes and clinical presentations. British Journal of Medical Psychology, 60, 29–37.Google Scholar
Griffiths, R. R., Hurwitz, E. S., Davis, A. K., Johnson, M. W., & Jesse, R. (2019). Survey of subjective ‘God encounter experiences’: Comparisons among naturally occurring experiences and those occasioned by the classic psychedelics psilocybin, LSD, ayahuasca, or DMT. PLoS One, 14(4), e0214377.Google Scholar
Griffiths, R. R., Johnson, M. W., Richards, W. A., et al. (2011). Psilocybin occasioned mystical-type experiences: Immediate and persisting dose-related effects. Psychopharmacology, 218(4), 649–665.Google Scholar
Griffiths, R. R., Richards, W. A., McCann, U., & Jesse, R. (2006). Psilocybin can occasion mystical-type experiences having substantial and sustained personal meaning and spiritual significance. Psychopharmacology, 187(3), 268–283.Google Scholar
Hagmann, P., Cammoun, L., Gigandet, X., et al. (2008). Mapping the structural core of human cerebral cortex. PLoS Biology, 6(7), e159. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.0060159Google Scholar
Haig, D. (2000). The kinship theory of genomic imprinting. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 31, 9–32.Google Scholar
Haig, D. (2004). Evolutionary conflicts in pregnancy and calcium metabolism. Placenta, 25(Suppl. A), S10–S15.Google Scholar
Haig, D. (2006). Intrapersonal conflict. In Jones, M. & Fabian, A. C. (Eds.), Conflict (pp. 8–22). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Halgren, E., Walter, R. D., Cherlow, D. G., & Crandall, P. H. (1978). Mental phenomena evoked by electrical stimulation of the human hippocampal formation and amygdala. Brain, 101(1), 83–117. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/101.1.83Google Scholar
Han, S., Mao, L., Gu, X., et al. (2008). Neural consequences of religious belief on self-referential processing. Social Neuroscience, 3(1), 1–15. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470910701469681Google Scholar
Harris, S., Kaplan, J. T., Curiel, A., et al. (2009). The neural correlates of religious and nonreligious belief. PLoS One, 4(10), e7272. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0007272Google Scholar
Hayden, B., (2018). The power of ritual in prehistory: Secret societies and the origins of social complexity. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Herbet, G., Lafargue, G., de Champfleur, N. M., et al. (2014). Disrupting posterior cingulate connectivity disconnects consciousness from the external environment. Neuropsychologia, 56, 239–244. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.01.020Google Scholar
Hermans, H. J. M., & Kempen, H. J. G. (1993). The dialogical self: Meaning as movement. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Hermans, H. J. M., Kempen, H. J. G., & Van Loon, R. J. (1992). The dialogical self: Beyond individualism and rationalism. American Psychologist, 47(1), 23–33.Google Scholar
Hermans, H. J. M., Konopka, A., Oosterwegel, A., & Zomer, P. (2017). Fields of tension in a boundary-crossing world: Towards a democratic organization of the self. Integral Psychology of Behavioral Science, 51(4), 505–535. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-016-9370-6Google Scholar
Hill, K., Wood, B., Baggio, J., Magdalena Hurtado, A., & Boyd, R. (2014). Hunter-gatherer inter-band interaction rates: Implications for cumulative culture. PLoS One, 9(7), e102806. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0102806Google Scholar
Hocart, A. M. (1927). Kingship. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hogg, M. A., Sherman, D. K., Dierselhuis, J., Maitner, A. T., & Moffitt, G. (2007). Uncertainty, entitativity, and group identification. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43, 135–142.Google Scholar
Hohwy, J. (2013). The predictive mind. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Holbrook, C., Izuma, K., Deblieck, C., Fessler, D. M., & Iacoboni, M. (2016). Neuromodulation of group prejudice and religious belief. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(3), 387–394.Google Scholar
Holtgraves, T., & McNamara, P. (2010). Pragmatic comprehension deficit in Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 32(4), 388–397. https://doi.org/10.1080/13803390903130729Google Scholar
Holtgraves, T., McNamara, P., Cappaert, K., & Durso, R. (2010). Linguistic correlates of asymmetric motor symptom severity in Parkinson’s disease. Brain and Cognition, 72(2), 189–196. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2009.08.004Google Scholar
Hood, R. W., Jr. (2006). The common core thesis in the study of mysticism. In Patrick McNamara, P. (Ed.), Where God and science meet: How brain and evolutionary studies alter our understanding of religion (Vol. 3, pp. 119–138). Westport, CT: Praeger.Google Scholar
Huguelet, P., Mohr, S., Borras, L., Gilliéron, C., & Brandt, P. Y. (2006). Spirituality and religious practices among outpatients with schizophrenia and their clinicians. Psychiatric Services, 57(3), 366–372.Google Scholar
Humphrey, C., & Laidlaw, J. (1993). Archetypal actions: A theory of ritual as a mode of action and the case of the Jain Puja. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Iemmola, I., & Camperio Ciani, C., (2009). New evidence of genetic factors influencing sexual orientation in men: Female fecundity increase in the maternal line. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 38, 393–399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
James, E., Robertshaw, T. L., Hoskins, M., & Sessa, B. (2020). Psilocybin occasioned mystical-type experiences. Human Psychopharmacology, 35(5). https://doi.org/10.1002/hup.2742Google Scholar
Jaynes, J. (1978). The origins of consciousness in the breakdown of the bicameral mind. NY: Plenum Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, K. A., Okun, M. A., Cohen, A. B., Sharp, C. A., & Hook, J. N. (2018). Development and validation of the five-factor LAMBI measure of God representations. Psychology of Religion and Spirituality.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. W., Hendricks, P. S., Barrett, F. S., & Griffiths, R. R. (2019). Classic psychedelics: An integrative review of epidemiology, therapeutics, mystical experience, and brain network function. Pharmacology & Therapeutics, 197, 83–102.Google Scholar
Johnstone, B., Bodling, A., Cohen, D., Christ, S. E., & Wegrzyn, A. (2012). Right parietal lobe-related “selflessness” as the neuropsychological basis of spiritual transcendence. International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 22(4), 267–284.Google Scholar
Johnstone, B., Hanks, R., Bhushan, B., et al. (2017). Selflessness as a universal neuropsychological foundation of spiritual transcendence: Validation with Christian, Hindu, and Muslim traditions. Mental Health, Religion & Culture, 20(2), 175–187. https://doi.org/10.1080/13674676.2017.1333090Google Scholar
Kapitány, R., Kavanagh, C., & Whitehouse, H. (2020). Ritual morphospace revisited: The form, function and factor structure of ritual practice. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 375(1805), 20190436. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2019.0436Google Scholar
Kaplan, J. T., Gimbel, S. I., Dehghani, M., et al. (2017). Processing narratives concerning protected values: A cross-cultural investigation of neural correlates. Cerebral Cortex, 27(2), 1428–1438. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhv325Google Scholar
Kapogiannis, D., Barbey, A. K., Su, M., Krueger, F., & Grafman, J. (2009). Neuroanatomical variability of religiosity. PLoS One, 4(9), e7180. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0007180Google Scholar
Kapogiannis, D., Deshpande, G., Krueger, F., Thornburg, M. P., & Grafman, J. H. (2014). Brain networks shaping religious belief. Brain Connectivity, 4(1), 70–79.Google Scholar
Keane, W. (1997). Religious language. Annual Review of Anthropology, 26, 47–71.Google Scholar
Keenan, J. P., Nelson, A., O’Connor, M., & Pascual-Leone, A. (2001). Self-recognition and the right hemisphere. Nature, 409, 305.Google Scholar
Kelemen, D. (2004). Are children “intuitive theists”? Reasoning about purpose and design in nature. Psychological Science, 15(5), 295–301.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keller, P. S. (2011). Sleep and attachment. Sleep and Development: Familial and Socio-Cultural Considerations, 49–77.Google Scholar
Keshavan, M. S., Collin, G., Guimond, S., et al. (2020). Neuroimaging in schizophrenia. Neuroimaging Clinics of North America, 30(1), 73–83. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nic.2019.09.007Google Scholar
Kierkegaard, S. (1846/1989). The sickness unto death. London: Penguin Classics.Google Scholar
Kirkpatrick, L. A. (2005). Attachment, evolution, and the psychology of religion. New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Kogo, N., & Trengove, C.. (2015). Is predictive coding theory articulated enough to be testable? Frontiers in Computational Neuroscience, 9, 111.Google Scholar
Kussé, C., Muto, V., Mascetti, L., et al. (2010). Neuroimaging of dreaming: State of the art and limitations. International Review of Neurobiology, 92, 87–99. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0074-7742(10)92005-9Google Scholar
Lacaux, C., Izabelle, C., Santantonio, G., et al. (2019). Increased creative thinking in narcolepsy. Brain, 142(7), 1988–1999. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awz137Google Scholar
Lebedev, A. V., Kaelen, M., Lövdén, M., et al. (2016). LSD-induced entropic brain activity predicts subsequent personality change. Human Brain Mapping, 37(9), 3203–3213. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.23234Google Scholar
Lebedev, A. V., Lövdén, M., Rosenthal, G., et al. (2015). Finding the self by losing the self: Neural correlates of ego-dissolution under psilocybin. Human Brain Mapping. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22833Google Scholar
Leeson, P. (2014). Human sacrifice. Review of Behavioral Economics, 1(1–2), 137–165.Google Scholar
Li, W., Ma, L., Yang, G., & Gan, W.-B. (2017). REM sleep selectively prunes and maintains new synapses in development and learning. Nature Neuroscience, 20(3), 427–437.Google Scholar
Liénard, P., & Boyer, P. (2006). Why ritualized behavior? Precaution systems and action parsing in developmental, pathological and cultural rituals. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 29, 1–56.Google Scholar
Lin, K., Marx, C., Caboclo, L. O., Centeno, R. S., Sakamoto, A. C., & Yacubian, E. M. (2009). Sign of the Cross (Signum Crucis): Observation of an uncommon ictal manifestation of mesial temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy Behaviour, 14(2), 400–403.Google Scholar
List, C., & Pettit, P. (2011). Group agency: The possibility, design and status of group agents. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Lord, L. D., Expert, P., Atasoy, S., et al. (2019). Dynamical exploration of the repertoire of brain networks at rest is modulated by psilocybin. NeuroImage, 199, 127–142. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.05.060Google Scholar
Lutkajtis, A. (2020). Entity encounters and the therapeutic effect of the psychedelic mystical experience. Journal of Psychedelic Studies, 4(3), 171–178. https://doi.org/10.1556/2054.2020.00143Google Scholar
Luyten, P., Campbell, C., Allison, E., & Fonagy, P. (2020). The mentalizing approach to psychopathology: State of the art and future directions. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 16, 297–325. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-071919-015355Google Scholar
Lysaker, P. H., & Lysaker, J. T. (2008). Schizophrenia and the fate of the self. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Maquet, P., & Franck, G. (1997). REM sleep and amygdala. Molecular Psychiatry, 2(3), 195–196. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.mp.4000239. PMID: 9152982.Google Scholar
Marean, C. W. (2015). An evolutionary anthropological perspective on modern human origins. Annual Review of Anthropology, 44, 533–556.Google Scholar
Markus, H., & Nurius, P. (1986). Possible selves. American Psychologist, 41, 954–969.Google Scholar
Mataix-Cols, D., Rauch, S. L., Manzo, P. A., Jenike, M. A., & Baer, L. (1999). Use of factor-analyzed symptom dimensions to predict outcome with serotonin reuptake inhibitors and placebo in the treatment of obsessive-compulsive disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry, 156, 1409–1416.Google Scholar
McCauley, R. N., & Lawson, E. T. (2002). Bringing ritual to mind: Psychological foundations of religious forms. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
McCauley, R. N., & Graham, G. (2019). Theory of mind, religiosity, and autistic spectrum disorder: A review of empirical evidence bearing on three hypotheses; Journal of Cognition and Culture, 19(5), 411–431.Google Scholar
McClenon, J. (2006). The ritual healing theory: Therapeutic suggestion and the origin of religion. In McNamara, P. (Ed.), Where God and science meet: How brain and evolutionary studies alter our understanding of religion: Vol. 1: Evolution, genes, and the religious brain (pp. 135–158). Westport, CT and London: Praeger Press.Google Scholar
McNamara, K. J. (1997). Shapes of time: The evolution of growth and development. Baltimore, MD: Johns Hopkins University Press.Google Scholar
McNamara, P. (2000). Counterfactual thought in dreams. Dreaming, 10(4), 232–245.Google Scholar
McNamara, P. (2004a). An evolutionary psychology of sleep and dreams. Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers/Greenwood Publishing Group.Google Scholar
McNamara, P. (2004b). Genomic imprinting and neurodevelopmental disorders of sleep. Sleep and Hypnosis, 6(2), 100–108.Google Scholar
McNamara, P. (2009). The neuroscience of religious experience. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
McNamara, P. (2015). Do you need cognitive neuroscience to understand religious cognition, experiences and texts? Journal of Cognitive Historiagraphia, 66–74. https://doi.org/10.1558/jch.v2i1.30988Google Scholar
McNamara, P. (2016). Dreams and visions: How religious ideas emerge in sleep and dreams. San Diego, CA: ABC-CLIO.Google Scholar
McNamara, P. (2019). Neuroscience of sleep and dreams. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
McNamara, P., Andresen, J., Clark, J., Zborowski, M., & Duffy, C. A. (2001). Impact of attachment styles on dream recall and dream content: A test of the attachment hypothesis of REM sleep. Journal of Sleep Research, 10(2), 117–127.Google Scholar
McNamara, P., Ayala, R., & Minsky, A. (2014). REM sleep, dreams, and attachment themes across a single night of sleep: A pilot study. Dreaming, 24(4), 290.Google Scholar
McNamara, P., Minsky, A., Pae, V., & Gusev, A. (2015). Cognitive phenomenology of religious experience in religious narratives, dreams, and nightmares. Archive for the Psychology of Religion, 37(3), 343–357. https://doi.org/10.1163/15736121-12341311Google Scholar
McNamara, P., Holtgraves, T., Durso, R., & Harris, E. (2010). Social cognition of indirect speech: Evidence from Parkinson’s disease. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 23(2), 162–171. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2009.12.003Google Scholar
McNamara, P., Teed, B., Pae, V., Sebastian, A., & Chukwumerije, C. (2018). Supernatural Agent Cognitions in Dreams. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 18(3–4), 428–450. https://doi.org/10.1163/15685373-12340038Google Scholar
Mikulincer, M., Shaver, P. R., Sapir-Lavid, Y., – Avihou-Kanza, N. (2009). What’s inside the minds of securely and insecurely attached people? The secure-base script and its associations with attachment-style dimensions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(4), 615.Google Scholar
Miller, B., Seeley, W. W., Mychack, P., et al. (2001). Neuroanatomy of the self: Evidence from patients with frontotemporal dementia. Neurology, 57(1), 817–821.Google Scholar
Modestino, E. J., O’Toole, P., & Reinhofer, A. (2016). Experiential and Doctrinal Religious Knowledge Categorization in Parkinson’s Disease: Behavioral and Brain Correlates. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2016.00113Google Scholar
Mohr, S., Brandt, P. Y., Borras, L., Gilliéron, C., & Huguelet, P. (2006). Toward an integration of spirituality and religiousness into the psychosocial dimension of schizophrenia. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163(11), 1952–1959.Google Scholar
Moon, J. W. (2021). Why are world religions so concerned with sexual behavior? Current Opinion in Psychology, 40, 15–19. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.copsyc.2020.07.030Google Scholar
Morgan, J., Clark, D., Tripodis, Y., et al. (2016). Impacts of religious semantic priming on an intertemporal discounting task: Response time effects and neural correlates. Neuropsychologia, 89, 403–413.Google Scholar
Miyauchi, S., Misaki, M., Kan, S., et al. (2009). Human brain activity time-locked to rapid eye movements during REM sleep; Experimental Brain Research, 192(4), 657–667. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-008-1579-2Google Scholar
Mueller, A. D., Meerlo, P., McGinty, D., & Mistlberger, R. E. (2015). Sleep and adult neurogenesis: Implications for cognition and mood. Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences, 25, 151–181. https://doi.org/10.1007/7854_2013_251Google Scholar
Murai, T., Hanakawa, T., Sengoku, A., et al. (1998). Temporal lobe epilepsy in a genius of natural history: MRI volumetric study of postmortem brain. Neurology, 50(5), 1373–1376. https://doi.org/10.1212/wnl.50.5.1373. PMID: 9595989.Google Scholar
Murdock, G. P. (1967). Ethnographic atlas: A summary. Pittsburgh, PA: University of Pittsburgh Press.Google Scholar
Murdock, G. P., & White, D. R. (1969). Standard cross-cultural sample. Ethnology, 8, 329–369.Google Scholar
Muthukumaraswamy, S. D., Carhart-Harris, R. L., Moran, R. J., et al. (2013). Broadband cortical desynchronization underlies the human psychedelic state. Journal of Neuroscience, 33(38), 15171–15183. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2063-13.2013Google Scholar
National Academies of Sciences, Engineering, and Medicine (NASEM). 2021. Sex differences in brain disorders: Emerging transcriptomic evidence: Proceedings of a workshop. Washington, DC: The National Academies Press. https://doi.org/10.17226/26058Google Scholar
Nelson, K. (2015). Near-death experiences--Neuroscience perspectives on near-death experiences. Missouri Medicine, 112(2), 92–98.Google Scholar
Neubauer, R. L. (2014). Prayer as an interpersonal relationship: A neuroimaging study. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 4(2), 92–103. https://doi.org/10.1080/2153599X.2013.768288Google Scholar
Newberg, A., Alavi, A., Baime, M., et al. (2001). The measurement of regional cerebral blood flow during the complex cognitive task of meditation: A preliminary SPECT study. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 106(2), 113–122.Google Scholar
Newberg, A. B., Wintering, N. A., Morgan, D., & Waldman, M. R. (2006). The measurement of regional cerebral blood flow during glossolalia: A preliminary SPECT study. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 148, 67–71.Google Scholar
Nichols, D. E. (2016). Psychedelics. Pharmacological Reviews, 68(2), 264–355.Google Scholar
Nordin, A., & Bjälkebring, P. (2019). Measuring Counterintuitiveness in Supernatural Agent Dream Imagery. Frontiers in Psychology, 10. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01728Google Scholar
Northoff, G., Heinzel, A., de Greck, M., et al. (2006). Self-referential processing in our brain – A meta-analysis of imaging studies on the self. NeuroImage, 31(1), 440–457. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2005.12.002Google Scholar
Nunn, C. L., & Samson, D. R. (2018). Sleep in a comparative context: Investigating how human sleep differs from sleep in other primates. American Journal of Physical Anthropology, 166(3), 601–612. https://doi.org/10.1002/ajpa.23427Google Scholar
Oakeshott, M. (1990). On human conduct. Oxford: Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Ogata, A., & Miyakawa, T. (1998). Religious experiences in epileptic patients with a focus on ictus-related episodes. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 52, 321–325.Google Scholar
Owen, A., Hayward, R. D., Koenig, H. G., Steffens, D. C., & Payne, M. E. (2011). Religious factors and hippocampal atrophy in late life. PLoS One, 6(3), e17006. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0017006Google Scholar
Oyserman, D., Bybee, D., Terry, K., & Hart-Johnson, T. (2004). Possible selves as roadmaps. Journal of Research in Personality, 38, 130–149.Google Scholar
Pahnke, W. N. (1967). LSD and religious experience. In DeBold, R. C. & Leaf, R. C. (Eds.), LSD, man and society (p. 74). Middletown, PA: Wesleyan University Press.Google Scholar
Paloutzian, R. F., Murken, S., Streib, H., & Rößler-Namini, S. (2013). Conversion, deconversion, and spiritual transformation: A multilevel interdisciplinary view. In Paloutzian, R. F. & Park, C. L. (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of religion and spirituality (pp. 399–421). New York: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Pardiñas, A. F., Holmans, P., Pocklington, A. J., et al. (2018). Common schizophrenia alleles are enriched in mutation-intolerant genes and in regions under strong background selection. Nature Genetics, 50(3), 381–389. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41588-018-0059-2Google Scholar
Paul, L. A. (2014). Transformative experience. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Peoples, R., Duda, P., & Marlowe, F. W. (2016). Phylogeny of religion. Human Nature, 27, 261–282. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12110-016-9260-0Google Scholar
Persinger, M. A. (1983). Religious and mystical experiences as artifacts of temporal lobe function: A general hypothesis. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 57, 1255–1262.Google Scholar
Persinger, M. A. (1987). Neuropsychological bases of God beliefs. New York: Praeger.Google Scholar
Péter, Z., Oliphant, M. E., & Fernandez, T. V. (2017). Motor stereotypies: A pathophysiological review. Frontiers of Neuroscience, 11, 171. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2017.00171Google Scholar
Picard, F. (2013). State of belief, subjective certainty and bliss as a product of cortical dysfunction. Cortex, 49(9), 2494–2500. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2013.01.006Google Scholar
Polimeni, J., & Reiss, J. P. (2003). Evolutionary perspectives on schizophrenia. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 48, 34–39.Google Scholar
Preller, K. H., & Vollenweider, F. X. (2018). Phenomenology, structure, and dynamic of psychedelic states. In Halberstadt, A. L., Vollenweider, F. X., & Nichols, D. E. (Eds.), Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences (pp. 221–256). Berlin: Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/7854_2016_459Google Scholar
Preller, K. H., Razi, A., Zeidman, P., et al. (2019). Effective connectivity changes in LSD-induced altered states of consciousness in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(7), 2743–2748.Google Scholar
Puri, B. K., Lekh, S. K., Nijran, K. S., Bagary, M. S., & Richardson, A. J. (2001). Spect neuroimaging in schizophrenia with religious delusions. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 40, 143–148.Google Scholar
Qin, P., Wang, M., & Northoff, G. (2020). Linking bodily, environmental and mental states in the self-A three-level model based on a meta-analysis. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 115, 77–95. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neubiorev.2020.05.004Google Scholar
Ramachandran, V. S., & Blakeslee, S. (1998). Phantoms in the brain: Probing the mysteries of the human mind. New York: William Morrow.Google Scholar
Rappaport, R. A. (1999). Ritual and religion in the making of humanity. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Richerson, P. J., & Boyd, R. (2005). Not by genes alone: How culture transformed human evolution. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Reisenzein, R., Horstmann, G., & Schutzwohlc, A. (2019). The cognitive-evolutionary model of surprise: A review of the evidence. Topics in Cognitive Science, 11(1), 50–74.Google Scholar
Ritchie, S. J., Cox, S. R., Shen, X., et al. (2018). Sex differences in the adult human brain: Evidence from 5216 UK biobank participants. Cerebral Cortex, 28(8), 2959–2975. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhy109Google Scholar
Roberts, J. K. A., & Guberman, A. (1989). Religion and epilepsy. Psychiatric Journal of the University of Ottawa: Revue de psychiatrie de l’Universitie d’Ottawa, 14(1), 282–286.Google Scholar
Romain, W. F. (2009). Shamans of the lost world. (2009). A cognitive approach to the prehistoric religion of the Ohio Hopewell. Plymouth, UK: Altamira Press; Rowman and Littlefield Publishers, Inc.Google Scholar
Sanz, C., Zamberlan, F., Erowid, E., Erowid, F., & Tagliazucchi, E. (2018). The experience elicited by hallucinogens presents the highest similarity to dreaming within a large database of psychoactive substance reports. Frontiers of Neuroscience, 12(7). https://doi.org/10.3389/fnins.2018.00007Google Scholar
Sass, L. A. (2014). Self-disturbance and schizophrenia: Structure, specificity, pathogenesis (Current issues, New directions). Schizophrenia Research, 152(1), 5–11. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.schres.2013.05.017Google Scholar
Schjødt, U., Stødkilde-Jørgensen, H., Geertz, A. W., & Roepstorff, A. (2008). Rewarding prayers. Neuroscience Letters, 443(3), 165–168. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neulet.2008.07.068Google Scholar
Schjoedt, U. (2009). The religious brain: A general introduction to the experimental neuroscience of religion. Method & Theory in the Study of Religion, 21(3), 310–339.Google Scholar
Schjoedt, U., & Andersen, M. (2017). How does religious experience work in predictive minds? Religion, Brain & Behavior, 7(4), 320–323.Google Scholar
Schjoedt, U., Stødkilde-Jørgensen, H., Geertz, A. W., & Roepstorff, A. (2009). Highly religious participants recruit areas of social cognition in personal prayer. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 4(2), 199–207.Google Scholar
Searle, J. R. (2007). What is language: Some preliminary remarks. In Tsohatzidis, S. L. (Ed.), John Searle’s Philosophy of Language: Force, meaning and mind (pp. 15–48). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Searle, J. R., & Vanderveken, D. (1985). Foundations of illocutionary logic. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Selterman, D., Apetroaia, A. I., Riela, S., & Aron, A. (2014). Dreaming of you: Behavior and emotion in dreams of significant others predict subsequent relational behavior. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 5(1), 111–118.Google Scholar
Seth, A. K. (2013). Interoceptive inference, emotion, and the embodied self. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(11), 565–573.Google Scholar
Shanon, B. (2002). The antipodes of the mind: Charting the phenomenology of the Ayahuasca experience. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Shtulman, A., & Rattner, M. (2018). Theories of god: Explanatory coherence in religious cognition. PLoS One, 13(12), e0209758.Google Scholar
Siddle, R., Haddock, G., Tarrier, N., & Garagher, E. B. (2002). Religious delusions in patients admitted to hospital with schizophrenia. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology, 37(3), 130–138.Google Scholar
Slater, E., & Beard, A. W. (1963). The schizophrenia like psychoses of epilepsy. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 109(458), 95–112. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.109.458.95Google Scholar
Stace, W. T. (1960). Mysticism and philosophy. Philadelphia, PA: Lippincott.Google Scholar
Stevens, A., & Price, J. (2015). Evolutionary psychiatry: A new beginning. London: Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315740577CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stickgold, R., Scott, L., Rittenhouse, C., & Hobson, J. A. (1999). Sleep-induced changes in associative memory. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 11(2), 182–193. https://doi.org/10.1162/089892999563319Google Scholar
Stromberg, P. G. (2012). Collective excitement and lapse in agency: Fostering an appetite for cigarettes. In Lende, D. H. & Downey, G. (Eds.), The encultured brain: An introduction to neuroanthropology (pp. 315–338). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Sullivan, P. F., Kendler, K. S., & Neale, M. C. (2003). Schizophrenia as a complex trait: Evidence from a meta-analysis of twin studies. Archives of General Psychiatry, 60, 1187–1192.Google Scholar
Sviri, S. (2002). The self and its transformation in Sūfīsm. In Shulman, D. & Stroumsa, G. G. (Eds.), Self and self-transformation in the history of religions (pp. 195–215). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Synofzik, M., Vosgerau, G., & Voss, M. (2013). The experience of agency: An interplay between prediction and postdiction. Frontiers of Psychology, 4, 127. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00127Google Scholar
Tagliazucchi, E., Carhart-Harris, R., Leech, R., et al. (2014). Enhanced repertoire of brain dynamical states during the psychedelic experience. Human Brain Mapping, 35(11), 5442–5456. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.22562Google Scholar
Tagliazucchi, E., Roseman, L., Kaelen, M., et al. (2016). Increased global functional connectivity correlates with lsd-induced ego dissolution. Current Biology 26(8), 1043–1050. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2016.02.010Google Scholar
Tajfel, H. (1981). Human groups and social categories. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Tambiah, S. (1979). A performative approach to ritual. Proceedings of the British Academy, 65, 113–169.Google Scholar
Tedrus, G. M. A. S., Fonseca, L. C., Fagundes, T. M., & da Silva, G. L. (2015). Religiosity aspects in patients with epilepsy. Epilepsy & Behavior, 50, 67–70.Google Scholar
Tek, C., & Ulug, B. (2001). Religiosity and religious obsessions in obsessivecompulsive disorder. Psychiatry Research, 104(2), 99–108.Google Scholar
Thompson, S. T., Lereya, G., & Lewis, S., et al. (2015). Childhood sleep disturbance and risk of psychotic experiences at 18: UK birth cohort. British Journal of Psychiatry, 207(1), 23–29. https://doi.org/10.1192/bjp.bp.113.144089Google Scholar
Thornton, M. A., & Tamir, D. I. (2020). People represent mental states in terms of rationality, social impact, and valence: Validating the 3d Mind Model. Cortex, 125, 44–59. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2019.12.012Google Scholar
Timmermann, C., Roseman, L., Williams, L., et al. (2018). DMT models the near-death experience. Frontiers of Psychology, 15(9), 1424. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01424Google Scholar
Tononi, G. (2008). Consciousness as integrated information: A provisional manifesto. The Biological Bulletin, 215, 216–242.Google Scholar
Trimble, M., & Freeman, A. (2006). An investigation of religiosity and the Gastaut-Geschwind syndrome in patients with temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior, 9(3), 407–414.Google Scholar
Triveni, D., Grover, S., & Chakrabarti, S. (2017). Religiosity among patients with schizophrenia: An exploratory study. Indian Journal of Psychiatry, 59(4), 420–428.Google Scholar
Tucker, D. M., Novelly, R. A., & Walker, P. J. (1987). Hyperreligiosity in temporal lobe epilepsy: Redefining the relationship. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 175(3), 181–184.Google Scholar
Tunde, A. O. (2016). Discourse functions of Will and Shall in Christian prayers. International Journal of Humanities and Cultural Studies, 2(4), 1096–1107.Google Scholar
Urgesi, C., Aglioti, S. M., Skrap, M., & Fabbro, F. (2010). The spiritual brain: Selective cortical lesions modulate human self-transcendence. Neuron, 65(3), 309–319. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.026Google Scholar
Vaitl, D., Birbaumer, N., Gruzelier, J. H., et al. (2005). Psychobiology of altered states of consciousness. Psychological Bulletin, 131, 98–127.Google Scholar
Van Elk, M., & Aleman, A. (2017). Brain mechanisms in religion and spirituality: An integrative predictive processing framework. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 73, 359–378.Google Scholar
Van Leeuwen, N., & Van Elk, M. (2019). Seeking the supernatural: The interactive religious experience model. Religion, Brain & Behavior, 9(3), 221–251.Google Scholar
van Lommel, P. (2011). Near-death experiences: The experience of the self as real and not as an illusion. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1234, 19–28. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-6632.2011.06080.x. PMID: 21988246.Google Scholar
Veronelli, L., Makaretz, S. J., Quimby, M., Dickerson, B. C., & Collins, J. A. (2017). Geschwind Syndrome in frontotemporal lobar degeneration: Neuroanatomical and neuropsychological features over 9 years. Cortex, 94, 27–38. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2017.06.003Google Scholar
Vogeley, K., & Fink, G. R. (2003). Neural correlates of first-person-perspective. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 38–42.Google Scholar
Vollenweider, F. X., & Kometer, M. (2010). The neurobiology of psychedelic drugs: Implications for the treatment of mood disorders. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 11(9), 642–651. https://doi.org/10.1038/nrn2884Google Scholar
Vollenweider, F. X., & Preller, K. H. (2020). Psychedelic drugs: Neurobiology and potential for treatment of psychiatric disorders. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 1–14. https://doi.org/10.1038/s41583-020-0367-2Google Scholar
Vörös, S. (2015). Saying the unsayable: Mystical experience and language. Disputatio philosophica: International Journal on Philosophy and Religion, 16(1), 79–90.Google Scholar
Walker, M. P., et al. (2002). Cognitive flexibility across the sleep–wake cycle: REM-sleep enhancement of anagram problem solving. Cognitive Brain Research, 14, 317–324.Google Scholar
Watts, J., Sheehan, O., Atkinson, Q. D., Bulbulia, J., & Gray, R. D. (2016). Ritual human sacrifice promoted and sustained the evolution of stratified societies. Nature, 532(7598), 228–231. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature17159Google Scholar
Waxman, S. G., & Geschwind, N. (1975). The interictal behavior syndrome of temporal lobe epilepsy. Archives of General Psychiatry, 32(12), 1580–1586.Google Scholar
Weil, S. (1942). The love of god and affliction. In Panichas, G. A. (Ed.), The Simone Weil reader (pp. 313–339). New York: David McKay Co.Google Scholar
Whitehouse, H. (2000). Arguments and icons: Divergent modes of religiosity. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Whitehouse, H. (2004). Modes of religiosity: A cognitive theory of religious transmission. Walnut Creek, CA: AltaMira Press.Google Scholar
Wildman, W. J., & McNamara, P. (2010). Evaluating reliance on narratives in the psychological study of religious experiences. The International Journal for the Psychology of Religion, 20(4), 223–254.Google Scholar
Wilkins, A. S., Wrangham, R. W., & Fitch, W. T. (2014). The “domestication syndrome” in mammals: A unified explanation based on neural crest cell behavior and genetics. Genetics, 197(3), 795–808. https://doi.org/10.1534/genetics.114.165423Google Scholar
Wilson, D. S., & Wilson, E. O. (2007). Rethinking the theoretical foundation of sociobiology. Quarterly Review of Biology, 82, 327–348.Google Scholar
Wright, S. T., Kerr, C. W., Doroszczuk, N. M., et al. (2014). The impact of dreams of the deceased on bereavement: A survey of hospice caregivers. Am J Hosp Palliat Care, 31(2), 132–138. https://doi.org/10.1177/1049909113479201Google Scholar
Wuerfel, J., Krishnamoorthy, E. S., Brown, R. C., et al. (2004). Religiosity is associated with hippocampal but not amygdala volumes in patients with refractory epilepsy. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 75, 640–642.Google Scholar
Yaden, D. B., Haidt, J., Hood, R. W., Jr., Vago, D. R., & Newberg, A. B. (2017). The varieties of self-transcendent experience. Review of General Psychology, 21, 143–160.Google Scholar
Yaden, D. B., Johnson, M. W., Griffiths, R. R., et al. (2021). Psychedelics and consciousness: Distinctions, demarcations, and opportunities. International Journal of Neuropsychopharmacology, 24(8), 615–623. https://doi.org/10.1093/ijnp/pyab026Google Scholar
Yilmaz, O., & Isler, O. (2019). Reflection increases belief in God through self-questioning among non-believers. Judgment and Decision Making, 14, 649–657.Google Scholar
Yong, J. C., Park, G., and Spitzmuller, M. (2021). From the savannah to the corporate office: The evolution of teams. Small Group Research, 52, 33–67. https://doi.org/10.1177/1046496420960516Google Scholar
Zhong, W., Cristofori, I., Bulbulia, J., Krueger, F., & Grafman, J. (2017). Biological and cognitive underpinnings of religious fundamentalism. Neuropsychologia, 100, 18–25. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.04.009Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • References
  • Patrick McNamara, Boston University
  • Book: The Cognitive Neuroscience of Religious Experience
  • Online publication: 02 June 2022
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108973496.014
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • References
  • Patrick McNamara, Boston University
  • Book: The Cognitive Neuroscience of Religious Experience
  • Online publication: 02 June 2022
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108973496.014
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • References
  • Patrick McNamara, Boston University
  • Book: The Cognitive Neuroscience of Religious Experience
  • Online publication: 02 June 2022
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108973496.014
Available formats
×