Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-78c5997874-v9fdk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-17T11:25:44.442Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Chapter 10 - Aging and Attention

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 November 2019

Kenneth M. Heilman
Affiliation:
University of Florida
Stephen E. Nadeau
Affiliation:
University of Florida
Get access

Summary

Cognitive changes that accompany the gradual degradation of neural systems are countervailed by a set of attention-related processes that serve to reorganize and maintain function with advancing age. This chapter focuses on the potential role of the right hemisphere fronto-parietal network in maintenance of adequate sustained attention to the environment by older adults, as well as self-monitoring of changes in their cognition and behavior over time. Modulation of norepinephrine activity in the locus coeruleus, via its impact on this right lateralized network, may be of particular importance in increasing the capacity of older people to preserve cognitive functioning as a multitude of biological changes take place in their brains. We review studies demonstrating that noninvasive electrical brain stimulation to the right prefrontal cortex improves both sustained attention and error awareness, suggesting that this key interconnected hub region in the right hemisphere holds the potential to be exploited and upregulated in older adults to ameliorate deficits.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Posner, MI, Petersen, SE. The attention system of the human brain. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 1990;13:2542.Google Scholar
Posner, MI, Rothbart, MK. Research on attention networks as a model for the integration of psychological science. Annual Review of Psychology. 2007;58:123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Backman, L, Karlsson, S, Fischer, H, Karlsson, P, Brehmer, Y, Rieckmann, A, et al. Dopamine D1 receptors and age differences in brain activation during working memory. Neurobiology of Aging. 2011;32(10):1849–56.Google Scholar
Mather, M, Harley, CW. The locus coeruleus: essential for maintaining cognitive function and the aging brain. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2016;20(3):214–26.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rolls, ET, Deco, G. Stochastic cortical neurodynamics underlying the memory and cognitive changes in aging. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory. 2015;118:150–61.Google Scholar
Erel, H, Levy, DA. Orienting of visual attention in aging. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2016;69:357–80.Google Scholar
Williams, RS, Biel, AL, Wegier, P, Lapp, LK, Dyson, BJ, Spaniol, J. Age differences in the Attention Network Test: evidence from behavior and event-related potentials. Brain and Cognition. 2016;102:6579.Google Scholar
Nashiro, K, Qin, S, O’Connell, MA, Basak, C. Age-related differences in BOLD modulation to cognitive control costs in a multitasking paradigm: global switch, local switch, and compatibility-switch costs. NeuroImage. 2018;172:146–61.Google Scholar
Gazzaley, A, Clapp, W, Kelley, J, McEvoy, K, Knight, RT, D’Esposito, M. Age-related top-down suppression deficit in the early stages of cortical visual memory processing. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2008;105(35):13122–6.Google Scholar
Ollman, R. Choice reaction time and the problem of distinguishing task effects from strategy effects. Attention and Performance VI. 1977:99–113.Google Scholar
Salthouse, TA. Aging and measures of processing speed. Biological Psychology. 2000;54(1–3):3554.Google Scholar
O’Connell, RG, Dockree, PM, Robertson, IH, Bellgrove, MA, Foxe, JJ, Kelly, SP. Uncovering the neural signature of lapsing attention: electrophysiological signals predict errors up to 20 s before they occur. Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29(26):8604–11.Google Scholar
O’Connell, RG, Dockree, PM, Kelly, SP. A supramodal accumulation-to-bound signal that determines perceptual decisions in humans. Nature Neuroscience. 2012;15(12):1729.Google Scholar
Arnsten, AFT. Catecholamine modulation of prefrontal cortical cognitive function. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 1998;2:436–47.Google Scholar
Ratcliff, R, Thapar, A, McKoon, G. The effects of aging on reaction time in a signal detection task. Psychology and Aging. 2001;16(2):323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ram, N, Rabbitt, P, Stollery, B, Nesselroade, JR. Cognitive performance inconsistency: intraindividual change and variability. Psychology and Aging. 2005;20(4):623.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alain, C, McDonald, KL, Ostroff, JM, Schneider, B. Aging: a switch from automatic to controlled processing of sounds? Psychology and Aging. 2004;19(1):125.Google Scholar
Verhaeghen, P, Cerella, J. Aging, executive control, and attention: a review of meta-analyses. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews. 2002;26(7):849–57.Google Scholar
Berardi, A, Parasuraman, R, Haxby, JV. Overall vigilance and sustained attention decrements in healthy aging. Experimental Aging Research. 2001;27(1):1939.Google Scholar
Jackson, JD, Balota, DA. Mind-wandering in younger and older adults: converging evidence from the sustained attention to response task and reading for comprehension. Psychology and Aging. 2012;27(1):106.Google Scholar
Staub, B, Doignon-Camus, N, Després, O, Bonnefond, A. Sustained attention in the elderly: what do we know and what does it tell us about cognitive aging? Ageing Research Reviews. 2013;12(2):459–68.Google Scholar
Carriere, JS, Cheyne, JA, Solman, GJ, Smilek, D. Age trends for failures of sustained attention. Psychology and Aging. 2010;25(3):569.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giambra, LM. Sustained attention and aging: overcoming the decrement? Experimental Aging Research. 1997;23(2):145–61.Google Scholar
Giambra, LM, Quilter, RE. Sustained attention in adulthood: a unique, large-sample, longitudinal and multicohort analysis using the Mackworth Clock-Test. Psychology and Aging. 1988;3(1):75.Google Scholar
Fortenbaugh, FC, DeGutis, J, Germine, L, Wilmer, JB, Grosso, M, Russo, K, et al. Sustained attention across the life span in a sample of 10,000: dissociating ability and strategy. Psychol Sci. 2015;26(9):1497–510.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McAvinue, LP, Habekost, T, Johnson, KA, Kyllingsbaek, S, Vangkilde, S, Bundesen, C, et al. Sustained attention, attentional selectivity, and attentional capacity across the lifespan. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics. 2012;74(8):1570–82.Google Scholar
Singh-Curry, V, Husain, M. The functional role of the inferior parietal lobe in the dorsal and ventral stream dichotomy. Neuropsychologia. 2009;47:1434–48.Google Scholar
Robertson, I. A right hemisphere role in cognitive reserve. Neurobiology of Aging. 2014;35:1375–85CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robertson, IH. A noradrenergic theory of cognitive reserve: implications for Alzheimer’s disease. Neurobiology of Aging. 2013;34:298308.Google Scholar
Clewett, DV, Lee, T-H, Greening, S, Ponzio, A, Margalit, E, Mather, M. Neuromelanin marks the spot: identifying a locus coeruleus biomarker of cognitive reserve in healthy aging. Neurobiology of Aging. 2016;37:117–26.Google Scholar
Benwell, CSY, Thut, G, Grant, A, Harvey, M. A rightward shift in the visuospatial attention vector with healthy aging. Frontiers in Aging Neuroscience. 2014;6.Google Scholar
Valenzuela, MJ, Sachdev, P. Brain reserve and dementia: a systematic review. Psychological Medicine. 2006;36(4):441–54.Google Scholar
Barulli, D, Stern, Y. Efficiency, capacity, compensation, maintenance, plasticity: emerging concepts in cognitive reserve. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2013;17(10):502–9.Google Scholar
Cognitive Aging: Progress in Understanding and Opportunities for Action. Blazer, DG, Yaffe, K, Liverman, CT, editors. Washington, DC: National Academies Press; 2015.Google Scholar
Stern, Y. What is cognitive reserve? Theory and research application of the reserve concept. Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society. 2002;8(3):448–60.Google Scholar
Nyberg, L, Lövdén, M, Riklund, K, Lindenberger, U, Bäckman, L. Memory aging and brain maintenance. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2012;16(5):292305.Google Scholar
Wilson, RS, Nag, S, Boyle, PA, Hizel, LP, Yu, L, Buchman, AS, et al. Neural reserve, neuronal density in the locus ceruleus, and cognitive decline. Neurology. 2013;80:1202–8.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tsukahara, JS, Harrison, TL, Engle, RW. The relationship between baseline pupil size and intelligence. Cognitive Psychology. 2016;91:109–23.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, P, O’Connell, R, O’Sullivan, M, Robertson, I, Balsters, J. Pupil diameter covaries with BOLD activity in human locus coeruleus. Human Brain Mapping. 2014;35:4140–54.Google Scholar
Sara, SJ. The locus coeruleus and noradrenergic modulation of cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2009;10:211–23.Google Scholar
Parnavelas, JG, Blue, ME. The role of the noradrenergic system on the formation of synapses in the visual cortex of the rat. Developmental Brain Research. 1982;3(1):140–4.Google Scholar
Jhaveri, DJ, Mackay, EW, Hamlin, AS, Marathe, SV, Nandam, LS, Vaidya, VA, et al. Norepinephrine directly activates adult hippocampal precursors via β3-adrenergic receptors. Journal of Neuroscience. 2010;30(7):2795–806.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heneka, MT, Nadrigny, F, Regen, T, Martinez-Hernandez, A, Dumitrescu-Ozimek, L, Terwel, D, et al. Locus ceruleus controls Alzheimer’s disease pathology by modulating microglial functions through norepinephrine. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2010;107(13):6058–63.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Robertson, IH. A right hemisphere role in cognitive reserve. Neurobiology of Aging. 2014;35(6):1375–85.Google Scholar
Robertson, I, Ridgeway, V, Greenfield, E, Parr, A. Motor recovery after stroke depends on intact sustained attention: a two-year follow-up study. Neuropsychology. 1997;11:290–5.Google Scholar
Recanzone, Ga, Schreiner, C, Merzenich, MM. Plasticity in the frequency representation of primary auditory cortex following discrimination training in adult owl monkeys. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1993;13(1):87103.Google Scholar
Maki, B, Mcllroy, W. Influence of arousal and attention on the control of postural sway. Journal of Vestibular Research. 1996;6(1):53–9.Google Scholar
Vankov, A, Hervé-Minvielle, A, Sara, SJ. Response to novelty and its rapid habituation in locus coeruleus neurons of the freely exploring rat. European Journal of Neuroscience. 1995;7(6):1180–7.Google Scholar
Wessel, JR. Error awareness and the error-related negativity: evaluating the first decade of evidence. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2012;6:88.Google Scholar
Graf, H, Abler, B, Freudenmann, R, Beschoner, P, Schaeffeler, E, Spitzer, M, et al. Neural correlates of error monitoring modulated by atomoxetine in healthy volunteers. Biological Psychiatry. 2011;69:890–7.Google Scholar
Brickman, AM, Zimmerman, ME, Paul, RH, Grieve, SM, Tate, DF, Cohen, RA, et al. Regional white matter and neuropsychological functioning across the adult lifespan. Biological Psychiatry. 2006;60(5):444–53.Google Scholar
Nyberg, L, Salami, A, Andersson, M, Eriksson, J, Kalpouzos, G, Kauppi, K, et al. Longitudinal evidence for diminished frontal cortex function in aging. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2010;107(52):22682–6.Google Scholar
Lu, H, Xu, F, Rodrigue, KM, Kennedy, KM, Cheng, Y, Flicker, B, et al. Alterations in cerebral metabolic rate and blood supply across the adult lifespan. Cereb Cortex. 2011;21(6):1426–34.Google Scholar
Small, GW, Ercoli, LM, Silverman, DH, Huang, SC, Komo, S, Bookheimer, SY, et al. Cerebral metabolic and cognitive decline in persons at genetic risk for Alzheimer’s disease. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2000;97(11):6037–42.Google Scholar
Benwell, CS, Thut, G, Grant, A, Harvey, M. A rightward shift in the visuospatial attention vector with healthy aging. Front Aging Neurosci. 2014;6:113.Google Scholar
Brown, JW, Jaffe, J. Hypothesis on cerebral dominance. Neuropsychologia. 1975;13(1):107–10.Google Scholar
Cherry, BJ, Hellige, JB. Hemispheric asymmetries in vigilance and cerebral arousal mechanisms in younger and older adults. Neuropsychology. 1999;13(1):111–20.Google Scholar
Clark, LE, Knowles, JB. Age differences in dichotic listening performance. Journal of Gerontology. 1973;28(2):173–8.Google Scholar
Learmonth, G, Benwell, CSY, Thut, G, Harvey, M. Age-related reduction of hemispheric lateralisation for spatial attention: an EEG study. Neuroimage. 2017;153:139–51.Google Scholar
Oke, A, Keller, R, Mefford, I, Adams, R. Lateralization of norepinephrine in human thalamus. Science. 1978;200:1411–13.Google Scholar
Hartshorne, JK, Germine, LT. When does cognitive functioning peak? The asynchronous rise and fall of different cognitive abilities across the life span. Psychol Sci. 2015;26(4):433–43.Google Scholar
Kaufman, AS, Horn, JL. Age changes on tests of fluid and crystallized ability for women and men on the Kaufman Adolescent and Adult Intelligence Test (KAIT) at ages 17–94 years. Arch Clin Neuropsychol. 1996;11(2):97121.Google Scholar
Yeatman, JD, Wandell, BA, Mezer, AA. Lifespan maturation and degeneration of human brain white matter. Nat Commun. 2014;5:4932.Google Scholar
Lachman, ME, Teshale, S, Agrigoroaei, S. Midlife as a pivotal period in the life course: balancing growth and decline at the crossroads of youth and old age. International Journal of Behavioral Development. 2015;39(1):2031.Google Scholar
Van Vleet, TM, DeGutis, JM, Merzenich, MM, Simpson, GV, Zomet, A, Dabit, S. Targeting alertness to improve cognition in older adults: a preliminary report of benefits in executive function and skill acquisition. Cortex. 2016;82:100–18.Google Scholar
Brosnan, MB, Arvaneh, M, Harty, S, Maguire, T, O’Connell, RG, Robertson, IH, Dockree, PM. Prefrontal modulation of the sustained attention network in ageing, a tDCS-EEG co-registration approach. J Cogn Neurosci. 2018;13:116.Google Scholar
O’Halloran, AM, Finucane, C, Savva, GM, Robertson, IH, Kenny, RA. Sustained attention and frailty in the older adult population. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. 2014;69(2):147–56.Google Scholar
Dockree, PM, Kelly, SP, Robertson, IH, Reilly, RB, Foxe, JJ. Neurophysiological markers of alert responding during goal-directed behavior: a high-density electrical mapping study. Neuroimage. 2005;27(3):587601.Google Scholar
O’Connell, RG, Bellgrove, MA, Dockree, PM, Lau, A, Fitzgerald, M, Robertson, IH. Self-Alert Training: volitional modulation of autonomic arousal improves sustained attention. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46(5):1379–90.Google Scholar
O’Connell, RG, Dockree, PM, Bellgrove, MA, Turin, A, Ward, S, Foxe, JJ, et al. Two types of action error: electrophysiological evidence for separable inhibitory and sustained attention neural mechanisms producing error on go/no-go tasks. J Cogn Neurosci. 2009;21(1):93104.Google Scholar
Buckner, RL, Sepulcre, J, Talukdar, T, Krienen, FM, Liu, H, Hedden, T, et al. Cortical hubs revealed by intrinsic functional connectivity: mapping, assessment of stability, and relation to Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurosci. 2009;29(6):1860–73.Google Scholar
Harty, S, Robertson, IH, Miniussi, C, Sheehy, OC, Devine, CA, McCreery, S, et al. Transcranial direct current stimulation over right dorsolateral prefrontal cortex enhances error awareness in older age. J Neurosci. 2014;34(10):3646–52.Google Scholar
McAvinue, L, O’Keeffe, F, McMackin, D, Robertson, IH. Impaired sustained attention and error awareness in traumatic brain injury: implications for insight. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2005;15(5):569–87.Google Scholar
Harty, S, O’Connell, RG, Hester, R, Robertson, IH. Older adults have diminished awareness of errors in the laboratory and daily life. Psychol Aging. 2013;28(4):1032–41.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fleming, JM, Strong, J, Ashton, R. Self-awareness of deficits in adults with traumatic brain injury: how best to measure? Brain Injury. 1996;10(1):115.Google Scholar
Lacey, E. Behavioural and electrophysiological aspects of error processing in healthy ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. PhD diss., Trinity College Dublin; 2018.Google Scholar
O’Connell, RG, Dockree, PM, Bellgrove, MA, Kelly, SP, Hester, R, Garavan, H, et al. The role of cingulate cortex in the detection of errors with and without awareness: a high-density electrical mapping study. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2007;25(8):2571–9.Google Scholar
Steinhauser, M, Yeung, N. Error awareness as evidence accumulation: effects of speed-accuracy trade-off on error signaling. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2012;6:240.Google Scholar
Yeung, N, Summerfield, C. Metacognition in human decision-making: confidence and error monitoring. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2012;367(1594):1310–21.Google Scholar
Murphy, PR, Robertson, IH, Allen, D, Hester, R, O’Connell, RG. An electrophysiological signal that precisely tracks the emergence of error awareness. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience. 2012;6:65.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Murphy, PR, Robertson, IH, Harty, S, O’Connell, RG. Neural evidence accumulation persists after choice to inform metacognitive judgments. Elife. 2015;4.Google Scholar
Harty, S, Murphy, PR, Robertson, IH, O’Connell, RG. Parsing the neural signatures of reduced error detection in older age. Neuroimage. 2017;161:4355.Google Scholar
Cavanagh, JF, Frank, MJ. Frontal theta as a mechanism for cognitive control. Trends in Cognitive Science. 2014;18(8):414–21.Google Scholar
Braun, N, Debener, S, Solle, A, Kranczioch, C, Hildebrandt, H. Biofeedback-based self-alert training reduces alpha activity and stabilizes accuracy in the Sustained Attention to Response Task. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology. 2015;37(1):1626.Google Scholar
Harsay, HA, Cohen, MX, Spaan, M, Weeda, WD, Nieuwenhuis, S, Ridderinkhof, KR. Error blindness and motivational significance: shifts in networks centering on anterior insula co-vary with error awareness and pupil dilation. Behavioural Brain Research. 2018 Dec 14;355:2435.Google Scholar
Murphy, PR, O’Connell, RG, O’Sullivan, M, Robertson, IH, Balsters, JH. Pupil diameter covaries with BOLD activity in human locus coeruleus. Human Brain Mapping. 2014;35(8):4140–54.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joshi, S, Li, Y, Kalwani, RM, Gold, JI. Relationships between pupil diameter and neuronal activity in the locus coeruleus, colliculi, and cingulate cortex. Neuron. 2016;89(1):221–34.Google Scholar
Coull, JT, Buchel, C, Friston, KJ, Frith, CD. Noradrenergically mediated plasticity in a human attentional neuronal network. Neuroimage. 1999;10(6):705–15.Google Scholar
Nucci, M, Mapelli, D, Mondini, S. Cognitive Reserve Index questionnaire (CRIq): a new instrument for measuring cognitive reserve. Aging Clinical and Experimental Research. 2012;24(3):218–26.Google Scholar
Brosnan, MB, Demaria, G, Petersen, A, Dockree, PM, Robertson, IH, Wiegand, I. Plasticity of the right-lateralized cognitive reserve network in ageing. Cerebral Cortex. 2018;28:1749–59.Google Scholar
Martinez, D, Orlowska, D, Narendran, R, Slifstein, M, Liu, F, Kumar, D, et al. Dopamine type 2/3 receptor availability in the striatum and social status in human volunteers. Biological Psychiatry. 2010;67(3):275–8.Google Scholar
Edwards, JD, Xu, H, Clark, DO, Guey, LT, Ross, LA, Unverzagt, FW. Speed of processing training results in lower risk of dementia. Alzheimer’s & Dementia: Translational Research & Clinical Interventions. 2017;3(4):603–11.Google Scholar
Coslett, HB, Bowers, D, Heilman, KM. Reduction in cerebral activation after right hemisphere stroke. Neurology. 1987;37:957–62.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×