Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-dk4vv Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-18T22:18:05.183Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Bibliography

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 July 2017

Marianne Hundt
Affiliation:
Universität Zürich
Sandra Mollin
Affiliation:
Universität Heidelberg
Simone E. Pfenninger
Affiliation:
Universität Salzburg
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
The Changing English Language
Psycholinguistic Perspectives
, pp. 348 - 406
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2017

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abbot-Smith, Kirsten F., and Behrens, Heike 2006. ‘How known constructions influence the acquisition of other constructions: The German passive and future constructions’, Cognitive Science 30(6): 9951026CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abney, Steven P. 1987. The English noun phrase in its sentential aspect. Doctoral dissertation. Massachusetts Institute of TechnologyGoogle Scholar
Abraham, Werner, and Århammar, Ritva (eds.) 1987. Linguistik in Deutschland: Akten des 21. Linguistischen Kolloqiums, Groningen 1986. Tübingen: NiemeyerCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adams, Michael, Brinton, Laurel J., and Fulk, Robert D. (eds.) 2015. Studies in the history of the English language. VI: Evidence and method in histories of English. Topics in English Linguistics 85. Berlin: Mouton de GruyterCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adamson, Sylvia 1998. ‘The literary language’, in Romaine, (ed.), pp. 589692CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Adelman, James, Brown, Gordon D. A., and Quesada, José F. 2006. ‘Contextual diversity, not word frequency, determines word-naming and lexical decision times’, Psychological Science, 17.9: 814–23CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aguado-Orea, Javier, and Pine, Julian 2015. ‘Comparing different models of the development of verb inflection in early child Spanish’, PloS One 10(3): e0119613CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ahlqvist, Anders (ed.) 1982. Papers from the 5th International Conference on Historical Linguistics. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John BenjaminsGoogle Scholar
Aijmer, Karin 1984. ‘“Sort of” and “kind of” in English conversation’, Studia Linguistica 38: 118–28Google Scholar
Aijmer, Karin 1996a. Conversational routines in English: Convention and creativity. New York: LongmanGoogle Scholar
Aijmer, Karin 1996b. ‘I think – an English modal particle’, in Swan, and Westvik, (eds.), pp. 147CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aitchison, Jean 1991. Language change: Progress or decay? Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Aitchison, Jean. 2003. ‘Psycholinguistic perspectives on language change’, in Joseph, and Janda, (eds.), pp. 736–43CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Akhtar, Nameera 1999. ‘Acquiring basic word order: Evidence for data-driven learning of syntactic structure’, Journal of Child Language 26(2): 339–56CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aksu-Koç, Ayhan 1988. The acquisition of aspect and modality: The case of past perfect in Turkish. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aksu-Koç, Ayhan, and Slobin, Dan I. 1986. ‘A psychological account of the development and use of evidentials in Turkish’, in Chafe, and Nichols, (eds.), pp. 159–67Google Scholar
Allan, Keith, and Jaszczolt, Kasia M. (eds.) 2012. The Cambridge handbook of pragmatics. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allan, Lorraine G. 1980. ‘A note on measurement of contingency between two binary variables in judgment tasks’, Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 15(3): 147–49CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Allerton, David J. 2009. ‘Tag questions’, in Rohdenburg, and Schlüter, (eds.), pp. 306–23CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Altmann, Eduardo G., Pierrehumbert, Janet B., and Motter, Adilson E. 2009. ‘Beyond word frequency: Bursts, lulls and scaling in the temporal distributions of words’, PLoS ONE 4(11): e7678CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Altmann, Gerry T. M. (ed.) 1990. Cognitive models of speech processing: Psycholinguistic and computational perspectives. Cambridge, MA: The MIT PressGoogle Scholar
Altmann, Gerry T. M. 1998. ‘Ambiguity in sentence processing’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2(4): 146–52CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Altmann, Gerry T. M., and Kamide, Yuki 1999. ‘Incremental interpretation at verbs: Restricting the domain of subsequent reference’, Cognition 73(3): 247–64CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Altmann, Gerry T. M., and Steedman, Mark 1988. ‘Interaction with context during human sentence processing’, Cognition 30(3): 191238CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ambridge, Ben, Kidd, Evan J., Rowland, Caroline F., and Theakston, Anna L. 2015. ‘The ubiquity of frequency effects in first language acquisition’, Journal of Child Language 42(2): 239–73CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ambridge, Ben, and Lieven, Elena V. M. 2011. Child language acquisition: Contrasting theoretical approaches. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ambridge, Ben, and Lieven, Elena V. M. 2015. ‘A constructivist account of child language acquisition’, in MacWhinney, and O’Grady, (eds.), pp. 478510CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ambridge, Ben, Pine, Julian M., Rowland, Caroline F., Chang, Franklin, and Bidgood, Amy 2013. ‘The retreat from overgeneralization in child language acquisition: Word learning, morphology and verb argument structure’, Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews Cognitive Science 4(1): 4762CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ambridge, Ben, and Rowland, Caroline F. 2009. ‘Predicting children’s errors with negative questions: Testing a schema-combination account’, Cognitive Linguistics 20(2): 225–66CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ambridge, Ben, Rowland, Caroline F., Theakston, Anna L., and Tomasello, Michael 2006. ‘Comparing different accounts of inversion errors in children’s non-subject wh-questions: “What experimental data can tell us?”’, Journal of Child Language 33(3): 519–57CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andersen, Henning 1973. ‘Abductive and deductive change’, Language 49(4): 765–93CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andersen, Henning 2001a. ‘Actualization and the (uni)directionality of change’, in Andersen, (ed.), pp. 225–48CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andersen, Henning (ed.) 2001b. Actualization: Linguistic change in progress. Amsterdam: John BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Andersen, Roger W. 1984. ‘The one-to-one principle of interlanguage construction’, Language Learning 34(4): 7795CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, John R. 1982. ‘Acquisition of cognitive skill’, Psychological Review 89(4): 369406CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, John R. 1983. The architecture of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University PressGoogle Scholar
Anderson, John R. 1990. The adaptive character of thought. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence ErlbaumGoogle Scholar
Anderson, John R. 1991. ‘Is human cognition adaptive?’, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 14(3): 471517CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, John R. 1992. ‘Automaticity and the ACT theory’, American Journal of Psychology 105(2): 165–80Google ScholarPubMed
Anderson, John R. 1996. ‘ACT: A simple theory of complex cognition’, American Psychologist 51(4): 355–65CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anderson, John R. 2000. Cognitive psychology and its implications. 5th edition. New York: W.H. FreemanGoogle Scholar
Anderson, John R. 2009. Cognitive psychology and its implications. 7th edition. New York: Worth PublishersGoogle Scholar
Andronis, Mary, Debenport, Erin, Pycha, Anne, and Yoshimura, Keiko (eds.) 2002. Proceedings of the 38th Meeting of the Chicago Linguistics Society. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic SocietyGoogle Scholar
Anshen, Frank, and Aronoff, Mark 1999. ‘Using dictionaries to study the mental lexicon’, Brain and Language 68(1): 1626CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Anttila, Raymond 2003. ‘Analogy: The warp and woof of cognition’, in Joseph, and Janda, (eds.), pp. 425–40CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arcodia, Giorgio Francesco 2007. ‘Chinese: A language of compound words?’, in Montermini, , Boyé, and Hathout, (eds.), pp. 7990Google Scholar
Ariel, Mira 1990. Accessing noun-phrase antecedents. London: RoutledgeGoogle Scholar
Arnon, Inbal, Casillas, Marisa, Kurumada, Chigusa, and Estigarribia, Bruno (eds.) 2014. Language in interaction: Studies in honor of Eve V. Clark. Amsterdam: BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnon, Inbal, and Snider, Neal 2010. ‘More than words: Frequency effects for multi-word phrases’, Journal of Memory and Language 62(1): 6782CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Aronoff, Mark 1976. Word formation in generative grammar. Cambridge, MA: The MIT PressGoogle Scholar
Aslin, Richard N., and Newport, Elissa L. 2012. ‘Statistical learning: From acquiring specific items to forming general rules’, Current Directions in Psychological Science 21(3): 170–76CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Auer, Peter 2014. ‘Anmerkungen zum Salienzbegriff in der Soziolinguistik’, Linguistik Online 66(4). http://dx.doi.org/10.13092/lo.66.1569 [accessed April 14, 2016]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Auer, Peter, Hinskens, Frans, and Kerswill, Paul (eds.) 2005. Dialect change: Convergence and divergence in European languages. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Axmaker, Shelley, Jaisser, Annie, and Singmaster, Helen (eds.) 1988. Berkeley Linguistics Society 14: General session and parasession on grammaticalization. Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics SocietyGoogle Scholar
Aylett, Matthew, and Turk, Alice 2004. ‘The smooth signal redundancy hypothesis: A functional explanation for relationships between redundancy, prosodic prominence and duration in spontaneous speech’, Language and Speech 47(1): 3156CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aylett, Matthew, and Turk, Alice 2006. ‘Language redundancy predicts syllabic duration and the spectral characteristics of vocalic syllable nuclei’, The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 119(5): 3048–58CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baars, Bernard J. 1997. ‘In the theatre of consciousness: Global workspace theory, a rigorous scientific theory of consciousness’, Journal of Consciousness Studies 4(4): 292309Google Scholar
Baayen, R. Harald 1993. ‘On frequency, transparency and productivity’, in Booij, and van Marle, (eds.), pp. 181208CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. Harald 1994. ‘Productivity in language production’, Language and Cognitive Processes 9(3): 447–69CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. Harald 2005. ‘Morphological productivity’, in Köhler, , Altmann, and Piotrowski, (eds.), pp. 243–56Google Scholar
Baayen, R. Harald 2011a. ‘Corpus linguistics and naive discriminative learning’, Brazilian Journal of Applied Linguistics 11(2): 295328Google Scholar
Baayen, R. Harald 2011b. ‘Demythologizing the word frequency effect: A discriminative learning perspective’, The Mental Lexicon 5(3): 436–61Google Scholar
Baayen, R. Harald 2014. ‘Multivariate statistics’, in Podesva, and Sharma, (eds.), pp. 337–72CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. Harald, Hendrix, Peter, and Ramscar, Michael 2013. ‘Sidestepping the combinatorial explosion: Towards a processing model based on discriminative learning’, Language and Speech 56(3): 329–47CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. Harald, Kuperman, Victor, and Bertram, Raymond 2010. ‘Frequency effects in compound processing’, in Scalise, and Vogel, (eds.), pp. 257270CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. Harald, Milin, Petar, Durdević, Dusica Filipović, Hendrix, Peter, and Marelli, Marco 2011. ‘An amorphous model for morphological processing in visual comprehension based on naive discriminative learning’, Psychological Review 118(3): 438–81CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baayen, R. Harald, Milin, Petar, and Ramscar, Michael 2015. ‘Frequency in lexical processing’, Aphasiology 30(11): 1174–220Google Scholar
Baayen, R. Harald, and Ramscar, Michael 2015. ‘Abstraction, storage and naive discriminative learning’, in Dąbrowska, and Divjak, (eds.), pp. 99120CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. Harald, Shaoul, Cyrus, Willits, Jon, and Ramscar, Michael 2016. ‘Comprehension without segmentation: A proof of concept with naive discriminative learning’, Language, Cognition, and Neuroscience 31(1): 106–28CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. Harald, van Halteren, Hans, and Tweedie, Fiona 1996. ‘Outside the cave of shadows: Using syntactic annotation to enhance authorship attribution’, Literary and Linguistic Computing 11(3): 121–31CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bach, Emmon, and Harms, Robert T. (eds.) 1968. Universals in linguistic theory. New York: Holt, Rinehart and WinstonGoogle Scholar
Baddeley, Alan D. 1997. Human memory: Theory and practice. Hove: Psychology PressGoogle Scholar
Bader, Markus, and Lasser, Ingeborg 1994. ‘German verb-final clauses and sentence processing: Evidence for immediate attachment’, in Clifton, , Frazier, and Rayner, (eds.), pp. 225–42Google Scholar
Bailey, Ashlee C., Moore, Kevin E., and Moxley, Jeri L. (eds.) 1997. Proceedings of the 23rd Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society, February 14–17, 1997. Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics SocietyGoogle Scholar
Bailey, Nathalie, Madden, Carolyn, and Krashen, Stephen D. 1974. ‘Is there a “natural sequence” in adult second language learning?’, Language Learning 24(2): 235–43CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baker, Carl L. 1995. ‘Contrast, discourse prominence, and intensification, with special reference to locally free reflexives in British English’, Language 71(1): 63101CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Balota, David A., and Chumbley, James I. 1984. ‘Are lexical decisions a good measure of lexical access? The role of word frequency in the neglected decision stage’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 10(3): 340–57Google ScholarPubMed
Balota, David A., Yap, Melvin J., Cortese, Michael J., Hutchison, Keith I., Kessler, Brett, Loftis, Bjorn, Neely, James H., Nelson, Douglas L., Simpson, Greg B., and Treiman, Rebecca 2007. ‘The English lexicon project’, Behavior Research Methods 39(3): 445–59CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baltin, Mark, and Collins, Chris (eds.) 2001. The handbook of contemporary syntactic theory. Oxford: BlackwellCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bannard, Colin, and Matthews, Danielle 2008. ‘Stored word sequences in language learning: The effect of familiarity on children’s repetition of four-word combinations’, Psychological Science 19(3): 241–48CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barabási, Albert-László 2005. ‘The origin of bursts and heavy tails in human dynamics’, Nature Reviews Neuroscience 435(7039): 207–11Google ScholarPubMed
Barabási, Albert-László 2010. Bursts: The hidden patterns behind everything we do, from your e-mail to bloody crusades. London: PenguinGoogle Scholar
Bardovi-Harlig, Kathleen 2000. Tense and aspect in second language acquisition: Form, meaning and use. Oxford: BlackwellGoogle Scholar
Barðdal, Jóhanna 2008. Productivity: Evidence from case and argument structure in Icelandic. Amsterdam: BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barlow, Michael, and Kemmer, Suzanne (eds.) 2000. Usage-based models of language. Stanford, CA: CSLI PublicationsGoogle Scholar
Baron, Naomi S. 1977. Language acquisition and historical change. Amsterdam: North-HollandGoogle Scholar
Barry, Christopher, and Seymour, Philip H. 1988. ‘Lexical priming and sound-to-spelling contingency effects in nonword spelling’, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 40(1): 540CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barsalou, Lawrence W. 1999. ‘Perceptual symbol systems’, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22(4): 577660CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barsalou, Lawrence W. 2008. ‘Grounded cognition’, Annual Review of Psychology 59: 617–45CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barsalou, Lawrence W., Huttenlocher, Janellen, and Lamberts, Koen 1998. ‘Basing categorization on individuals and events’, Cognitive Psychology 36(3): 203–72CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartlett, Frederic C. 1967. Remembering: A study in experimental and social psychology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press [1st edition 1932]Google Scholar
Bates, Elizabeth, and Goodman, Judith C. 1997. ‘On the inseparability of grammar and the lexicon: Evidence from acquisition, aphasia and real-time processing’, Language and Cognitive Processes 12(5–6): 507–86Google Scholar
Bates, Elizabeth, and MacWhinney, Brian 1987. ‘Competition, variation, and language learning’, in MacWhinney, (ed.), pp. 157–93Google Scholar
Bates, Elizabeth, MacWhinney, Brian, Caselli, Cristina, Devescovi, Antonella, Natale, Francesco, and Venza, Valeria 1984. ‘A crosslinguistic study of the development of sentence interpretation strategies’, Child Development 55(2): 341–54CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bauer, Laurie 2001. Morphological productivity. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bavin, Edith L. 1995. ‘The obligation modality in Western Nilotic languages’, in Bybee, and Fleischman, (eds.), pp. 107–33CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bayley, Robert, Cameron, Richard, and Lucas, Ceil (eds.) 2013. The Oxford handbook of sociolinguistics. Oxford: Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beals, Katharine (ed.) 1993. Papers from the 29th regional meeting of the Chicago Linguistic Society. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic SocietyGoogle Scholar
Becker, Angelika, and Veenstra, Tonjes 2003. ‘Creole prototypes as basic varieties and inflectional morphology’, in Dimroth, and Starren, (eds.), pp. 235–66CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beckett, Samuel 1954. Waiting for Godot. New York: Grove PressGoogle Scholar
Beckner, Clay, Blythe, Richard, Bybee, Joan L., Christiansen, Morten H., Croft, William, Ellis, Nick C., Holland, John, Ke, Jinyun, Larsen-Freeman, Diane, and Schoenemann, Tom 2009. ‘Language is a complex adaptive system: Position paper’, Language Learning 59(s1): 126Google Scholar
Beckner, Clay, and Bybee, Joan L. 2009. ‘A usage-based account of constituency and reanalysis’, Language Learning 59(s1): 2746CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Behaghel, Otto 1909. ‘Beziehungen zwischen Umfang und Reihenfolge von Satzgliedern’, Indogermanische Forschungen 25: 110–42Google Scholar
Behaghel, Otto 1923–1932. Deutsche Syntax. 4 vols. Heidelberg: WinterGoogle Scholar
Behrens, Heike 2002. ‘Learning multiple regularities: Evidence from overgeneralization errors in the German plural’, in Skarabela, , Fish, and Do, (eds.), pp. 7283Google Scholar
Behrens, Heike 2006. ‘The input-output relationship in first language acquisition’, Language and Cognitive Processes 21(1–3): 224CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Behrens, Heike (ed.) 2008. Corpora in language acquisition research: History, methods, perspectives. Amsterdam: John BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Behrens, Heike 2009. ‘Usage-based and emergentist approaches to language acquisition’, Linguistics [Special Issue: Current Approaches to Language Learning] 47(2): 383411Google Scholar
Behrens, Heike 2011. ‘Cues to form and function in the acquisition of German number and case inflection’, in Clark, and Arnon, (eds.), pp. 3551CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Behrens, Heike, and Pfänder, Stefan (eds.) 2016. Experience counts: Frequency effects in language. Berlin: Mouton de GruyterCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bello, Paul, Guarini, Marcello, McShane, Marjorie, and Scassellati, Brian (eds.) 2014. Proceedings of the 36th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society. Austin, TX: Cognitive Science SocietyGoogle Scholar
Benveniste, Émile 1968. ‘Mutations of linguistic categories’, in Lehmann, and Malkiel, (eds.), pp. 8594Google Scholar
Bergen, Benjamin, and Chang, Nancy 2013. ‘Embodied construction grammar’, in Hoffmann, and Trousdale, (eds.), pp. 168–90CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bergs, Alex, and Pentrel, Maike 2015. ‘Ælc þara þe þas min word gehierþ and þa wyrcþ …: Psycholinguistic perspectives on early Englishes’, in Adams, , Brinton, and Fulk, (eds.), pp. 249–76CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bergs, Alexander, and Brinton, Laurel J. (eds.) 2012. English historical linguistics. An international handbook, vol. 2. Berlin and New York: Mouton de GruyterGoogle Scholar
Bergs, Alexander, and Diewald, Gabriele (eds.) 2008. Constructions and language change. Berlin and New York: Mouton de GruyterCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernolet, Sarah, and Hartsuiker, Robert J. 2010. ‘Does verb bias modulate syntactic priming?’, Cognition 114(3): 455–61CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernolet, Sarah, Hartsuiker, Robert J., and Pickering, Martin J. 2007. ‘Shared syntactic representations in bilinguals: Evidence for the role of word-order repetition’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 33(5): 931–49Google ScholarPubMed
Bernolet, Sarah, Hartsuiker, Robert J., and Pickering, Martin J. 2009. ‘Persistence of emphasis in language production: A cross-linguistic approach’, Cognition 112(2): 300–17CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bernolet, Sarah, Hartsuiker, Robert J., and Pickering, Martin J. 2012. ‘Effects of phonological feedback on the selection of syntax: Evidence from between-language syntactic priming’, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 15(3): 503–16CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bernolet, Sarah, Hartsuiker, Robert J., and Pickering, Martin J. 2013. ‘From language-specific to shared syntactic representations: The influence of second language proficiency on syntactic sharing in bilinguals’, Cognition 127(3): 287306CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berwick, Robert, and Weinberg, Amy 1984. The grammatical basis of linguistic performance: Language use and acquisition. Cambridge, MA: The MIT PressGoogle Scholar
Biber, Douglas, Johansson, Stig, Leech, Geoffrey, Conrad, Susan, and Finegan, Edward 1999. Longman grammar of spoken and written English. Harlow, UK: Pearson EducationGoogle Scholar
Bickerton, Derek 1981. Roots of language. Ann Arbor: Karoma PublishersGoogle Scholar
Bickerton, Derek 1990. Language and species. Chicago: University of Chicago PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bien, Heidrun, Baayen, R. Harald, and Levelt, Willem J. M. 2011. ‘Frequency effects in the production of Dutch deverbal adjectives and inflected verbs’, Language and Cognitive Processes 27(4–6): 683715CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bird, Steven 2006. ‘NLTK: The natural language toolkit’. Proceedings of the COLING/ACL 2006 Interactive Presentation Sessions, Sydney, July 2006. Association for Computational Linguistics, pp. 6972Google Scholar
Bird, Steven, Klein, Ewan, and Loper, Edward 2009. Natural language processing with Python. Sebastopol, CA: O’Reilly Media, Inc.Google Scholar
Bisang, Walter, Himmelmann, Nikolaus P., and Wiemer, Björn (eds.) 2004. What makes grammaticalization – A look from its fringes and its components. Berlin and New York: Mouton de GruyterCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bittner, Dagmar, and Köpcke, Klaus-Michael 2001. ‘Acquisition of the German plural markings: A case study in natural and cognitive morphology’, in Schaner-Wolles, , Rennison, and Neubarth, (eds.), pp. 4758Google Scholar
Black, Abraham H., and Prokasy, William F. (eds.) 1972. Classical conditioning II: Current theory and research. New York: Appleton-Century-CroftsGoogle Scholar
Blumenthal-Dramé, Alice 2012. Entrenchment in usage-based theories: What corpus data do and do not reveal about the mind. Berlin and New York: Mouton de GruyterCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blything, Ryan P., Ambridge, Ben, and Lieven, Elena V. M. 2014. ‘Children use statistics and semantics in the retreat from overgeneralization’, PLoS One 9.10: e110009CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bock, J. Kathryn 1986. ‘Syntactic persistence in language production’, Cognitive Psychology 18(3): 355–87CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bock, J. Kathryn, Dell, Gary S., Chang, Franklin, and Onishi, Kristine H. 2007. ‘Persistent structural priming from language comprehension to language production’, Cognition 104(3): 437–58CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bock, J. Kathryn, and Ferreira, Victor S. 2014. ‘Syntactically speaking’, in Goldrick, , Ferreira, and Miozzo, (eds.), pp. 2146Google Scholar
Bock, J. Kathryn, and Griffin, Zenzi M. 2000. ‘The persistence of structural priming: Transient activation or implicit learning?’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 129(2): 177–92Google ScholarPubMed
Bock, J. Kathryn, and Levelt, Willem J. M. 1994. ‘Language production: Grammatical encoding’, in Gernsbacher, (ed.), pp. 741–79Google Scholar
Bod, Rens 2009. ‘From exemplar to grammar: A probabilistic analogy-based model of language learning’, Cognitive Science 33(5): 752–93CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bod, Rens, Hay, Jennifer, and Jannedy, Stefanie (eds.) 2003. Probabilistic linguistics. Cambridge, MA: The MIT PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bond, Zinny 1999. Slips of the ear: Errors in the perception of casual conversation. New York: Academic PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Booij, Geert E., and van Marle, Jaap (eds.) 1993. Yearbook of morphology. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic PublishersGoogle Scholar
Bornschein, Matthias, and Butt, Matthias 1987. ‘Zum Status des -s- Plurals im gegenwärtigen Deutsch’, in Abraham, and Århammar, (eds.), pp. 135–54CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bouma, Gerlof, Krämer, Irene, and Zwarts, Joost (eds.) 2007. Cognitive foundations of interpretation. Amsterdam: Royal Netherlands Academy of ScienceGoogle Scholar
Bowerman, Melissa 1973. Early syntactic development. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Bowerman, Melissa 1985. ‘What shapes children’s grammars?’, in Slobin, (ed.), pp. 1257–319Google Scholar
Bowerman, Melissa 1988. ‘The “no negative evidence” problem: How do children avoid constructing an overgeneral grammar?’, in Hawkins, (ed.), pp. 73101Google Scholar
Bowerman, Melissa, and Brown, Penelope 2006a. ‘Introduction’, in Bowerman, and Brown, (eds.), pp. 126Google Scholar
Bowerman, Melissa, and Brown, Penelope (eds.) 2006b. Crosslinguistic perspectives on argument structure: Implications for language acquisition. Mahwah, NJ: ErlbaumGoogle Scholar
Bowerman, Melissa, and Choi, Soonja 2003. ‘Space under construction: Language specific spatial categorization in first language acquisition’, in Gentner, and Goldin-Meadow, (eds.), pp. 387427CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowern, Claire, and Evans, Bethwyn (eds.) 2014. The Routledge handbook of historical linguistics. London and New York: RoutledgeGoogle Scholar
Boyd, Jeremy K., and Goldberg, Adele E. 2011. ‘Learning what not to say: The role of statistical preemption and categorization in a-adjective production’, Language 87(1): 5583CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boye, Kasper, and Harder, Peter 2012. ‘A usage-based theory of grammatical status and grammaticalization’, Language 88(1): 144CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Braine, Martin D. S., and Bowerman, Melissa 1976. ‘Children’s first word combinations’, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 41(1): 1104CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brandt, Silke, Kidd, Evan, Lieven, Elena V. M., and Tomasello, Michael 2009. ‘The discourse bases of relativization: An investigation of young German and English-speaking children’s comprehension of relative clauses’, Cognitive Linguistics 20(3): 539–70CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Branigan, Holly P., Pickering, Martin J., and Cleland, Alexandra A. 2000. ‘Syntactic co-ordination in dialogue’, Cognition 75(2): B13B25CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Branigan, Holly P., Pickering, Martin J., McLean, Janet F., and Cleland, Alexandra A. 2007. ‘Syntactic alignment and participant role in dialogue’, Cognition 104(2): 163–97CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Branigan, Holly P., Pickering, Martin J., Pearson, Jamie, McLean, Janet F., and Brown, Ash 2011. ‘The role of beliefs in lexical alignment: Evidence from dialogues with humans and computers’, Cognition 121(1): 4157CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brems, Lieselotte 2003. ‘Measure noun constructions: An instance of semantically-driven grammaticalization’, International Journal of Corpus Linguistics 8(2): 238312CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brems, Lieselotte 2011. Layering of size and type noun constructions in English. Berlin and New York: Mouton de GruyterCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brems, Lieselotte, and Davidse, Kristin 2010. ‘The grammaticalisation of nominal type noun constructions with kind/sort of: Chronology and paths of change’, English Studies 91(2): 180202CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brennan, Susan E., and Clark, Herbert H. 1996. ‘Conceptual pacts and lexical choice in conversation’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 22(6): 1482–93Google ScholarPubMed
Bresnan, Joan 2007. ‘Is syntactic knowledge probabilistic? Experiments with the English dative alternation’, in Featherston, and Sternefeld, (eds.), pp. 7796CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bresnan, Joan, Cueni, Anna, Nikitina, Tatiana, and Baayen, R. Harald 2007. ‘Predicting the dative alternation’, in Bouma, , Krämer, and Zwarts, (eds.), pp. 6994Google Scholar
Brinton, Laurel J. 1991. ‘The origin and development of quasimodal have to in English’. Paper presented to the 10th ICHL, Amsterdam 1991. Unpublished manuscript. http://faculty.arts.ubc.ca/lbrinton/HAVETO.pdf [accessed April 24, 2016]Google Scholar
Brinton, Laurel J. 1996. Pragmatic markers in English: Grammaticalization and discourse functions. Berlin and New York: Mouton de GruyterCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brinton, Laurel J. 2008. The comment clause in English: Syntactic origins and pragmatic development. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brinton, Laurel J., and Traugott, Elizabeth C. 2005. Lexicalization and language change. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Britt, M. Anne 1994. ‘The interaction of referential ambiguity and argument structure’, Journal of Memory and Language 33(2): 251–83CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broeder, Peter, and Murre, Jaap (eds.) 2000. Models of language acquisition: Inductive and deductive approaches. Oxford: Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brooks, Patricia J., Tomasello, Michael, Lewis, Lawrence B., and Dodson, Kelly 1999. ‘Children’s overgeneralization of fixed transitivity verbs: The entrenchment hypothesis’, Child Development 70(6): 1325–37CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, Roger 1973. A first language: The early stages. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brysbaert, Marc, and Mitchell, Don 1996. ‘Modifier attachment in sentence parsing: Evidence from Dutch’, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 49(3): 664–95Google Scholar
Burrows, John F. 1992. ‘Computers and the study of literature’, in Butler, (ed.), pp. 167204Google Scholar
Butler, Christopher S. (ed.) 1992. Computers and written texts. Oxford: BlackwellGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan L. 1985. Morphology: A study of the relation between meaning and form. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan L. 1998. ‘A functionalist approach to grammar and its evolution’, Evolution of Communication 2(2): 249–78CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan L. 2001. Phonology and language use. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan L. 2002. ‘Sequentiality as the basis of constituent structure’, in Givón, and Malle, (eds.), pp. 109–34CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan L. 2003. ‘Mechanisms of change in grammaticalization: The role of frequency’, in Joseph, and Janda, (eds.), pp. 602–23CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan L. 2006. ‘From usage to grammar: The mind’s response to repetition’, Language 82(4): 711–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan L. 2008a. ‘Grammaticalization: Implications for a theory of language’, in Guo, , Lieven, , Budwig, , Ervin-Tripp, , Nakamura, and Őzçalişkan, (eds.), pp. 345–55Google Scholar
Bybee, Joan L. 2008b. ‘Usage-based grammar and second language acquisition’, in Robinson, and Ellis, (eds.), pp. 216–36Google Scholar
Bybee, Joan L. 2010. Language, usage and cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan L. 2013. ‘Usage-based theory and exemplar representations of constructions’, in Hoffmann, and Trousdale, (eds.), pp. 4969CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan L. 2014. ‘Analytic and holistic processing in the development of constructions’, in Arnon, , Casillas, , Kurumada, and Estigarribia, (eds.), pp. 303–13CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan L., and Beckner, Clay 2014. ‘Language use, cognitive processes and linguistic change’, in Bowern, and Evans, (eds.), pp. 503–18Google Scholar
Bybee, Joan L., and Fleischman, Suzanne (eds.) 1995. Modality in grammar and discourse. Amsterdam: John BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan L., and Hopper, Paul (eds.) 2001. Frequency and the emergence of linguistic structure. Typological Studies in Language 45. Amsterdam: John BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan L., and Pagliuca, William 1987. ‘The evolution of future meaning’, in Giacalone Ramat, , Carruba, and Bernini, (eds.), pp. 108–22Google Scholar
Bybee, Joan L., Perkins, Revere, and Pagliuca, William 1994. The evolution of grammar: Tense, aspect, and modality in the languages of the world. Chicago: University of Chicago PressGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan L., and Scheibman, Joanne 1999. ‘The effect of usage on degrees of constituency: The reduction of don’t in English’, Linguistics 37(4): 575–96CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan L., and Slobin, Dan I. 1982. ‘Rules and schemas in the development and use of the English past tense’, Language 58(2): 265–89CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, Joan L., and Thompson, Sandra 1997. ‘Three frequency effects in syntax’, in Bailey, , Moore, and Moxley, (eds.), pp. 378–88CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cai, Zhenguang G., Pickering, Martin J., Yan, Hao, and Branigan, Holly P. 2011. ‘Lexical and syntactic representations in closely related languages: Evidence from Mandarin and Cantonese’, Journal of Memory and Language 65(4): 431–45CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cameron-Faulkner, Thea, Lieven, Elena V. M., and Tomasello, Michael 2003. ‘A construction based analysis of child directed speech’, Cognitive Science 27(6): 843–73CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Campbell, Byron A., and Church, Russell M. (eds.) 1969. Punishment and aversive behavior. New York: Appleton-Century-CroftsGoogle Scholar
Carey, Kathleen 1990. ‘The role of conversational implicature in the early grammaticalization of the English perfect’, in Hall, , Koenig, , Meacham, , Reinman, , and Sutton, (eds.), pp. 371–80CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carey, Kathleen 1994. ‘The grammaticalization of the perfect in Old English: An account based on pragmatics and metaphor’, in Pagliuca, (ed.), pp. 103–17CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carreiras, Manuel, and Clifton, Charles 1993. ‘Relative clause interpretation preferences in Spanish and English’, Language and Speech 36(4): 353–72CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carreiras, Manuel, and Clifton, Charles (eds.) 2004. The on-line study of sentence comprehension. Hove: Psychology PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carroll, John B., and White, Margaret N. 1973. ‘Word frequency and age of acquisition as determiners of picture-naming latency’, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 25(1): 8595CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chafe, Wallace, and Nichols, Johanna (eds.) 1986. Evidentiality. The linguistic coding of epistemology. Norwood, NJ: AblexGoogle Scholar
Chalmers, David J., French, Robert M., and Hofstadter, Douglas R. 1992. ‘High-level perception, representation, and analogy: A critique of artificial intelligence methodology’, Journal of Experimental and Theoretical Artificial Intelligence 4(3): 185211CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chambers, Jack K., Trudgill, Peter, and Schilling-Estes, Natalie (eds.) 2002. The handbook of language variation and change. Oxford: BlackwellGoogle Scholar
Chang, Franklin, Dell, Gary S., and Bock, J. Kathryn 2006. ‘Becoming syntactic’, Psychological Review 113(2): 234–72CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chapman, Carol 1995. ‘Perceptual salience and analogical change: Evidence from vowel lengthening in modern Swiss German dialects’, Journal of Linguistics 31(1): 113CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chater, Nick, and Manning, Christopher 2006. ‘Probabilistic models of language processing and acquisition’, Trends in Cognitive Science 10(7): 335–44CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheshire, Jenny, Kerswill, Paul, Fox, Sue, and Torgersen, Eivind 2013. ‘English as a contact language: The role of children and adolescents’, in Schreier, and Hundt, (eds.), pp. 560607CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chiarcos, Christian 2011. ‘On the dimensions of discourse salience’. www.linguistics.ruhr-uni-bochum.de/bla/beyondsem2011/chiarcos_final.pdf [accessed April 19, 2016]Google Scholar
Chiarcos, Christian, Claus, Berry, and Grabski, Michael 2011a. ‘Introduction’, in Chiarcos, , Claus, and Grabski, (eds.), pp. 128CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chiarcos, Christian, Claus, Berry, and Grabski, Michael (eds.) 2011b. Salience: Multidisciplinary perspectives on its function in discourse. Berlin and New York: Mouton de GruyterCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Childers, Jane B., and Tomasello, Michael 2001. ‘The role of pronouns in young children’s acquisition of the English transitive construction’, Child Development 37(6): 739–48Google ScholarPubMed
Chomsky, Noam 1981. Lectures on Government and Binding. Dordrecht: Foris PublicationsGoogle Scholar
Christiansen, Morten H., and Chater, Nick (eds.) 2001. Connectionist psycholinguistics. Westport, CO: AblexGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christiansen, Morten H., and Chater, Nick 2008. ‘Language as shaped by the brain’, Behavioral and Brain Sciences [Target Article for Multiple Peer Commentary] 31(5): 489509CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christiansen, Morten H., and Chater, Nick 2016. Creating language: Integrating evolution, acquisition, and processing. Cambridge, MA: The MIT PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Christiansen, Morten H., and Kirby, Simon (eds.) 2003. Language evolution. Oxford: Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christianson, Kiel, Hollingworth, Andrew, Halliwell, John F., and Ferreira, Fernanda 2001. ‘Thematic roles assigned along the garden path linger’, Cognitive Psychology 42(2): 368407CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Christie, William (ed.) 1976. Current progress in historical linguistics. Amsterdam: North HollandGoogle Scholar
Cienki, Alan J., Luka, Barbara J., and Smith, Michael B. (eds.) 2001. Conceptual and discourse factors in linguistic structure. Stanford: CSLI PublicationsGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, Harald 1999. ‘Lexical entries and rules of language: A multidisciplinary study of German inflection’, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22(6): 9911060CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clahsen, Harald, Rothweiler, Monika, Woest, Andreas, and Marcus, Gary F. 1992. ‘Regular and irregular inflection in the acquisition of German noun plurals’, Cognition 45(3): 225–55CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Claridge, Claudia, and Kytö, Merja 2014. ‘“You are a bit of a sneak”: Exploring a degree modifier in the Old Bailey Corpus’, in Hundt, (ed.), pp. 239–68CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, Andy. 2013. ‘Whatever next? Predictive brains, situated agents, and the future of cognitive science’, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36(3): 181204CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, Eve V. 1978. ‘Discovering what words can do’, in Farkas, , Jacobsen, and Todrys, (eds.), pp. 3457Google Scholar
Clark, Eve V. 1982. ‘Language change during language acquisition’, in Lamb, and Brown, (eds.), pp. 171–95Google Scholar
Clark, Eve V., and Arnon, Inbal (eds.) 2011 Experience, variation, and generalization: Learning a first language. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John BenjaminsGoogle Scholar
Clark, Herbert H. 1992. Arenas of language use. Chicago: University of Chicago PressGoogle Scholar
Clark, Herbert H. 1996. Using language. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, Herbert H., and Wilkes-Gibbs, Deanna 1986. ‘Referring as a collaborative process’, Cognition 22(1): 139CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cleeremans, Axel, and McClelland, James L. 1991. ‘Learning the structure of event sequences’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 120(3): 235–53Google ScholarPubMed
Cleland, Alexandra A., and Pickering, Martin J. 2003. ‘The use of lexical and syntactic information in language production: Evidence from the priming of noun-phrase structure’, Journal of Memory and Language 49(2): 214–30CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clifton, Charles, Frazier, Lyn, and Rayner, Keith (eds.) 1994. Perspectives on sentence processing. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence ErlbaumGoogle Scholar
Clifton, Charles, and Staub, Adrian 2008. ‘Parallelism and competition in syntactic ambiguity resolution’, Language and Linguistics Compass 2(2): 234–50CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Coates, Jennifer 1983. The semantics of the modal auxiliaries. London: Croom HelmGoogle Scholar
Cole, Peter, and Sadock, Jerrold Murray (eds.) 1977. Syntax and semantics, vol. 8: Grammatical relations. New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
Coltheart, Max 1978. ‘Lexical access in simple reading tasks’, in Underwood, (ed.), pp. 151216Google Scholar
Coltheart, Max (ed.) 1987. Attention and performance XII: The psychology of reading. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum AssociatesGoogle Scholar
Coltheart, Max, and Leahy, Judi 1996. ‘Assessment of lexical and nonlexical reading abilities in children: Some normative data’, Australian Journal of Psychology 48(3): 136–40CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Comrie, Bernard 2003. ‘Reconstruction, typology and reality’, in Hickey, (ed.), pp. 243–57CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corder, Stephen P. 1967. ‘The significance of learners’ errors’, International Review of Applied Linguistics 5(1–4): 161–69CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Corral, Alvaro, Boleda, Gemma, and Ferrer-i-Cancho, Ramon 2015. ‘Zipf’s law for word frequencies: Word forms versus lemmas in long texts’, PLoS ONE 10(7): e0129031CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cottrell, Garrison W., and Plunkett, Kim 1994. ‘Acquiring the mapping from meaning to sounds’, Connection Science 6(4): 379412CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Craig, Colette G. (ed.) 1986. Noun classes and categorization. Amsterdam: John BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crocker, Matthew W. 1996. Computational psycholinguistics: An interdisciplinary approach to the study of language. Dordrecht: Kluwer AcademicCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Croft, William A. 2000. Explaining language change: An evolutionary approach. Harlow, Essex: LongmanGoogle Scholar
Croft, William A. 2001. Radical construction grammar: Syntactic theory in typological perspective. Oxford: Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Croft, William A. 2010. ‘The origins of grammaticalization in the verbalization of experience’, Linguistics 48(1): 148CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Croft, William, and Cruse, Alan D. 2004. Cognitive linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Crystal, David 2004. The stories of English. New York: The Overlook PressGoogle Scholar
Cuetos, Fernando, and Mitchell, Don 1988. ‘Cross-linguistic differences in parsing: Restrictions on the use of the late closure strategy in Spanish’, Cognition 30(1): 73105CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Culpeper, Jonathan, and Kytö, Merja 2010. Early Modern English dialogues: Spoken interaction as writing. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Cutler, Anne, and Carter, David M. 1987. ‘The predominance of strong initial syllables in the English vocabulary’, Computer Speech and Language 2(3): 133–42CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cutting, J. Cooper, and Bock, J. Kathryn 1997. ‘That’s the way the cookie bounces: Syntactic and semantic components of experimentally elicited idiom blends’, Memory and Cognition 25(1): 5771CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dąbrowska, Ewa 2000. ‘From formula to schema: The acquisition of English questions’, Cognitive Linguistics 11(1–2): 83102CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dąbrowska, Ewa 2001. ‘Learning a morphological system without a default: The Polish genitive’, Journal of Child Language 28(3): 545–74CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dąbrowska, Ewa 2004. Language, mind and brain: Some psychological and neurological constraints on theories of grammar. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dąbrowska, Ewa 2012. ‘Different speakers, different grammars: Individual differences in native language attainment’, Linguistic Approaches to Bilingualism 2(3): 219–53CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dąbrowska, Ewa, and Divjak, Dagmar (eds.) 2015. Handbook of cognitive linguistics. Berlin and New York: Mouton de GruyterCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Danchev, Andrei, and Kytö, Merja 1994. ‘The construction be going to + infinitive in Early Modern English’, in Kastovsky, (ed.), pp. 5978CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Danks, David 2003. ‘Equilibria of the Rescorla-Wagner model’, Journal of Mathematical Psychology 47(2): 109–21CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Daugherty, Kim G., and Seidenberg, Mark S. 1994. ‘Beyond rules and exceptions: A connectionist approach to inflectional morphology’, in Lima, , Corrigan, and Iverson, (eds.), pp. 353–88CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davidse, Kristin 2009. ‘Complete and sort of: From identifying to intensifying?’, Transactions of the Philological Society 107(3): 262–92CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davies, Mark 2010. ‘The corpus of contemporary American English as the first reliable monitor corpus of English’, Literary and Linguistic Computing 25(4): 447–64CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dawkins, Richard 1985. The blind watchmaker. New York: NortonGoogle Scholar
Deacon, Terrence W. 1997. The symbolic species: The co-evolution of language and the brain. New York: W. W. NortonGoogle Scholar
Deacon, Terrence W. 2003. ‘Universal grammar and semiotic constraints’, in Christiansen, and Kirby, (eds.), pp. 111–39.Google Scholar
de Bot, Kees, Lowie, Wander, Thorne, Steven L., and Verspoor, Marjolijn 2013. ‘Dynamic systems theory as a comprehensive theory of second language development’, in Mayo, García, Mangado, Gutierrez, and Adrián, Martínez (eds.), pp. 199220CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Declerck, Renaat 1982. ‘The triple origin of participial perception verb complements’, Linguistic Analysis 10: 126Google Scholar
Degand, Liesbeth, and Simon, Anne Catherine 2005. ‘“My brother, he drives like crazy”: Contextual salience, linguistic marking and discourse organisation in spoken French’, in Stede, , Chiarcos, , Grabski, , and Lagerwerf, (eds.), pp. 4352Google Scholar
de Houwer, Annick 2007. ‘Parental language input patterns and children’s bilingual use’, Applied Psycholinguistics 28(3): 411–24CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Jong, Nivia H., Schreuder, Rob, and Baayen, R. Harald 2000. ‘The morphological family size effect and morphology’, Language and Cognitive Processes 15(4–5): 329–65CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeKeyser, Robert M. 2001. ‘Automaticity and automatization’, in Robinson, (ed.), pp. 125–51CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dell, Gary S., and Chang, Franklin 2014. ‘The P-Chain: Relating sentence production and its disorders to comprehension and acquisition’, Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences 369(1634): 20120394CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Demberg, Vera, and Keller, Frank 2008. ‘Data from eye-tracking corpora as evidence for theories of syntactic processing complexity’, Cognition 109(2): 193210CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Denison, David 1985. ‘Why Old English had no prepositional passive’, English Studies 66: 189204CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denison, David 1993. English historical syntax: Verbal constructions. London and New York: LongmanGoogle Scholar
Denison, David 2002. ‘History of the sort of construction family’. Paper presented at ICCG2: Second International Conference on Construction Grammar, Helsinki, September 2002Google Scholar
Denison, David 2006. ‘Category change and gradience in the determiner system’, in van Kemenade, and Los, (eds.), pp. 279304CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denison, David 2010a. ‘Category change in English with and without structural change’, in Traugott, and Trousdale, (eds.), pp. 105–28CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denison, David 2010b. ‘SKT-constructions: The relation between synchronic and diachronic analysis’. Paper presented at SLE 43, VilniusGoogle Scholar
Denison, David 2012. ‘On the history of English (and) word classes’. Paper presented at ICEHL17, ZurichGoogle Scholar
Denison, David 2013. ‘Parts of speech: Solid citizens or slippery customers?’, Journal of the British Academy 1: 151–85CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Denison, David in preparation. English word classes: Categories and their limits. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
de Smedt, Liesbeth 2005. Functions of the T-nouns kind, sort and type: A comprehensive, data-based description. MA thesis. University of LeuvenGoogle Scholar
de Smedt, Liesbeth, Brems, Lieselotte, and Davidse, Kristin 2007. ‘NP-internal functions and extended uses of the ‘type’ nouns kind, sort, and type: Towards a comprehensive, corpus-based description’, in Facchinetti, (ed.), pp. 227–57CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Smet, Hendrik 2009. ‘Analysing reanalysis’, Lingua 119(11): 1728–55CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Smet, Hendrik 2012. ‘The course of actualization’, Language 88(3): 601–33CrossRefGoogle Scholar
des Rosiers, Gabriel, and Ivison, David 1986. ‘Paired-associate learning: Normative data for differences between high and low associate word pairs’, Journal of Clinical Experimental Neuropsychology 8(6): 637–42Google ScholarPubMed
de Vincenzi, Marica, and Lombardo, Vincenzo (eds.) 2000. Proceedings of AMLaP-96. Dordrecht: Kluwer Academic PressGoogle Scholar
Dewaele, Jean-Marc 2004. ‘Retention or omission of the ne in advanced French interlanguage: The variable effect of extralinguistic factors’, Journal of Sociolinguistics 8(3): 433–50CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diessel, Holger 1999. Demonstratives: Form, function, and grammaticalization. Amsterdam: BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diessel, Holger 2004. The acquisition of complex sentences. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diessel, Holger 2007. ‘Frequency effects in language acquisition, language use, and diachronic change’, New Ideas in Psychology 25(2): 108–27CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diessel, Holger 2011. ‘Grammaticalization and language acquisition’, in Heine, and Narrog, (eds.), pp. 130–41CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diessel, Holger 2012. ‘Diachronic change and language acquisition’, in Bergs, and Brinton, (eds.), pp. 1599–613CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diessel, Holger, and Tomasello, Michael 2001. ‘The acquisition of finite complement clauses in English: A corpus-based analysis’, Cognitive Linguistics 12(2): 97141CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diewald, Gabriele 2002. ‘A model for relevant types of contexts in grammaticalization’, in Wischer, and Diewald, (eds.), pp. 103–20CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Diewald, Gabriele 2006. ‘Context types in grammaticalization as constructions’, Constructions 1: 129. http://elanguage.net/journals/index.php/constructions/article/viewFile/24/29 [accessed June 6, 2013]Google Scholar
Dimroth, Christine, and Starren, Marianne (eds.) 2003. Information structure and the dynamics of language acquisition. Amsterdam: BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Drachman, Geberell 1978. ‘Child language and language change: A conjecture and some refutations’, in Fisiak, (ed.), pp. 123–44CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dreschler, Gea 2015. Passives and the loss of verb second: A study of syntactic and information-structural factors. Utrecht: LOTGoogle Scholar
du Bois, John W. 1985. ‘Competing motivations’, in Haiman, (ed.), pp. 343–65CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dufouil, Carole, Pereira, Ewige, Chêne, Geneviève, Glymour, M. Maria, Alpérovitch, Annick, Saubusse, Elodie, Risse-Fleury, Mathilde, Heuls, Brigitte, Salord, Jean-Claude, Brieu, Marie-Anne, and Forette, Françoise 2014. ‘Older age at retirement is associated with decreased risk of dementia’, European Journal of Epidemiology 29(5): 353–61CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Durrant, Philip, and Doherty, Alice 2010. ‘Are high-frequency collocations psychologically real? Investigating the thesis of collocational priming’, Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory 6(2): 125–55CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ebbinghaus, Hermann 1885. Memory: A contribution to experimental psychology. New York: Teachers College, Columbia [Trans. Ruger, Henry A. and Bussenius, Clara E., 1913]Google Scholar
Eckardt, Regine 2006. Meaning change in grammaticalization. An enquiry into semantic reanalysis. Oxford: Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edwards, Jonathan 1752. Misrepresentations corrected, and truth vindicated. In a reply to the Rev. Mr. Solomon Williams’s book, intitled, The True State of the Question Concerning the Qualifications Necessary to Lawful Communion in the Christian Sacraments. Boston: S. KneelandGoogle Scholar
Eisenbeiss, Sonja, Bartke, Sonja, and Clahsen, Harald 2006. ‘Structural and lexical case in child German: Evidence from language-impaired and typically developing children’, HLAC 13(1): 332CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekwall, Eilert 1975. A history of Modern English sounds and morphology. Translated and edited by Ward, Alan. Oxford: BlackwellGoogle Scholar
Ellis, Nick C. 1994a. ‘Vocabulary acquisition: The implicit ins and outs of explicit cognitive mediation’, in Ellis, (ed.), pp. 211–82Google Scholar
Ellis, Nick C. (ed.) 1994b. Implicit and explicit learning of languages. San Diego, CA: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
Ellis, Nick C. 1996. ‘Sequencing in SLA: Phonological memory, chunking, and points of order’, Studies in Second Language Acquisition 18(1): 91126CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, Nick C. 2002. ‘Frequency effects in language processing: A review with implications for theories of implicit and explicit language acquisition’, Studies in Second Language Acquisition 24(2): 143–88CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, Nick C. 2005. ‘At the interface: Dynamic interactions of explicit and implicit language knowledge’, Studies in Second Language Acquisition 27(2): 305–52CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, Nick C. 2006a. ‘Language acquisition as rational contingency learning’, Applied Linguistics 27(1): 124CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, Nick C. 2006b. ‘Selective attention and transfer phenomena in L2 acquisition: Contingency, cue competition, salience, interference, overshadowing, blocking, and perceptual learning’, Applied Linguistics 27(2): 164–94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, Nick C. 2012. ‘What can we count in language, and what counts in language acquisition, cognition, and use?’, in Gries, and Divjak, (eds.), pp. 734CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, Nick C. 2013. ‘Construction grammar and second language acquisition’, in Hoffmann, and Trousdale, (eds.), pp. 365–78CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, Nick C. 2016. ‘Frequency in language learning and language change: The contributions to this volume from a cognitive and psycholinguistic perspective’, in Behrens, and Pfänder, (eds.), pp. 239–54CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, Nick C., and Beaton, Alan 1993. ‘Psycholinguistic determinants of foreign language vocabulary learning’, Language Learning 43(4): 559617CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, Nick C., and Larsen-Freeman, Diane 2006. ‘Language emergence: Implications for applied linguistics’, Applied Linguistics 27(4): 558589CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, Nick C., and Larsen-Freeman, Diane 2009. Language as a complex adaptive system. Oxford: Wiley-BlackwellGoogle Scholar
Ellis, Nick C., Römer, Ute, and O’Donnell, Matthew B. 2016. Usage-based approaches to language acquisition and processing: Cognitive and corpus investigations of construction grammar. Malden, MA: Wiley-BlackwellGoogle Scholar
Ellis, Nick C., and Sagarra, Nuria 2010. ‘The bounds of adult language acquisition: Blocking and learned attention’, Studies in Second Language Acquisition 32(4): 553–80CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, Nick C., and Sagarra, Nuria 2011. ‘Learned attention in adult language acquisition: A replication and generalization study and meta-analysis’, Studies in Second Language Acquisition 33(4): 589624CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, Nick C., and Schmidt, Richard 1998. ‘Rules or associations in the acquisition of morphology? The frequency by regularity interaction in human and PDP learning of morphosyntax’, Language and Cognitive Processes 13(2–3): 307–36CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, Nick C., and Simpson-Vlach, Rita 2009. ‘Formulaic language in native speakers: Triangulating psycholinguistics, corpus linguistics, and education’, Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory 5(1): 6178CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, Nick C., Simpson-Vlach, Rita, and Maynard, Carson 2008. ‘Formulaic language in native and second-language speakers: Psycholinguistics, corpus linguistics, and TESOL’, TESOL Quarterly 42(3): 375–96CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elman, Jeffrey L. 1990. ‘Finding structure in time’, Cognitive Science 14(2): 179211CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Elman, Jeffrey L. 2003. ‘Generalization from sparse input’. Proceedings of the 38th Annual Meeting of the Chicago Linguistics Society, pp. 175200Google Scholar
Elman, Jeffrey L. 2004. ‘An alternative view of the mental lexicon’, Trends in Cognitive Science 8(7): 301–6CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Elman, Jeffrey L., Bates, Elizabeth A., Johnson, Mark H., Karmiloff-Smith, Anette, Parisi, Domenico, and Plunkett, Kim 1996. Rethinking innateness: A connectionist perspective on development. Cambridge, MA: The MIT PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erbaugh, Mary S. 1986. ‘Taking stock: The development of Chinese noun classifiers historically and in young children’, in Craig, (ed.), pp. 399436CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erkelens, Maria A. 2009. ‘Learning to categorize verbs and nouns: Studies on Dutch’. Leiden: LOT dissertation, Nr. 211Google Scholar
Erman, Britt, and Kotsinas, Ulla-Britt 1993. Pragmaticalization: The case of ‘ba’ and ‘you know’, Studier i Modernspråkvetenskap 10: 7693.Google Scholar
Erman, Britt, and Warren, Beatrice 2000. ‘The idiom principle and the open choice principle’, Text 20(1): 2962Google Scholar
Evans, Nicholas, and Wilkins, David 2000. ‘In the mind’s ear: The Semantic extensions of perception verbs in Australian languages’, Language 76(3): 546–92CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Evans, Vyvyan, and Pourcel, Stephanie S. (eds.) 2009. New directions in cognitive linguistics. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Everett, Daniel L. 2012. Language: The cultural tool. New York: Pantheon BooksGoogle Scholar
Facchinetti, Roberta (ed.) 2007. Corpus linguistics 25 years on. Language and Computers – Studies in Practical Linguistics 62. Amsterdam and New York: RodopiGoogle Scholar
Farkas, Donka, Jacobsen, Wesley M., and Todrys, Karol W. (eds.) 1978. Papers from the parasession on the lexicon. Chicago: Chicago Linguistic SocietyGoogle Scholar
Featherston, Sam, and Sternefeld, Wolfgang (eds.) 2007. Roots: Linguistics in search of its evidential base. Berlin and New York: Mouton de GruyterCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Felser, Claudia 1999. Verbal complement clauses: A minimalist study of direct perception constructions. Amsterdam: BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferguson, Charles A., and Slobin, Dan I. (eds.) 1973. Studies of child language development. New York: Holt, Reinhart, and WinstonGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, Fernanda 2003. ‘The misinterpretation of noncanonical sentences’, Cognitive Psychology 47(2): 164203CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferreira, Fernanda, and Clifton, Charles 1986. ‘The independence of syntactic processing’, Journal of Memory and Language 25(3): 348–68CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, Fernanda, and Henderson, John M. 1991. ‘Recovery from misanalyses of garden-path sentences’, Journal of Memory and Language 30(6): 725–45CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, Fernanda, and Patson, Nikole D. 2007. ‘The “good enough” approach to language comprehension’, Language and Linguistics Compass 1(1–2): 7183CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, Victor S., Bock, J. Kathryn, Wilson, Michael P., and Cohen, Neal J. 2008. ‘Memory for syntax despite amnesia’, Psychological Science 19(9): 940–46CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferrer-i-Cancho, Ramon, Dębowski, Łukasz, and Moscoso del Prado Martín, Fermín 2013. ‘Constant conditional entropy and related hypotheses’, Journal of Statistical Mechanics: Theory and Experiment 07: L07001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferrer-i-Cancho, Ramon, and Solé, Richard V. 2003. ‘Least effort and the origins of scaling in human language’. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 100(3): 788–91Google ScholarPubMed
Field, John 2008. ‘Bricks or mortar: Which parts of the input does a second language listener rely on?’, Tesol Quarterly 42(3): 411–32CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fillmore, Charles J. 1977. ‘The case for case reopened’, in Cole, and Sadock, (eds.), pp. 5981CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fine, Julian M., and Lieven, Elena V. M. 1993. ‘Reanalyzing rote-learned phrases: Individual differences in the transition to multi-word speech’, Journal of Child Language 20(3): 551–71CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Firbas, Jan 1992. Functional sentence perspective in written and spoken communication. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Firth, John R. 1957. A synopsis of linguistic theory, 1930–1955. Oxford: Basil BlackwellGoogle Scholar
Fischer, Kirsten (ed.) 2006. Approaches to discourse markers. Oxford: ElsevierCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, Olga 1988. ‘The rise of the “for NP to V” construction: An explanation’, in Nixon, and Honey, (eds.), pp. 6788CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, Olga 1994. ‘The development of quasi-auxiliaries in English and changes in word order’, Neophilologus 78(1): 137–64CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, Olga 2000. ‘Grammaticalization: Unidirectional, non-reversable? The case of to before the infinitive in English’, in Fischer, , Rosenbach, and Stein, (eds.), pp. 149–70CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, Olga 2007. Morphosyntactic change: Functional and formal perspectives. Oxford: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
Fischer, Olga 2011. ‘Grammaticalization as analogically driven change?’, in Narrog, and Heine, (eds.), pp. 3142CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, Olga 2015. ‘The influence of the grammatical system and analogy in processes of language change: The case of the auxiliation of HAVE-TO once again’, in Toupin, and Lowrey, (eds.), pp. 120–50Google Scholar
Fischer, Olga, and Olbertz, Hella forthcoming. ‘The role played by analogy in processes of language change: The case of English have to compared to Spanish tener que’, in Yáñez-Bouza, , Moore, , Hollmann, and van Bergen, (eds.)Google Scholar
Fischer, Olga, Rosenbach, Annette, and Stein, Dieter (eds.) 2000. Pathways of change: Grammaticalization in English. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, Olga, and van der Wurff, Wim 2006. ‘Syntax’, in Hogg, and Denison, (eds.), pp. 109–98CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, Olga, van Kemenade, Ans, Koopman, Willem, and van der Wurff, Wim 2000. The syntax of early English. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Fisher, Cynthia 1996. ‘Structural limits on verb mapping: The role of analogy in children’s interpretations of sentences’, Cognitive Psychology 31(1): 4181CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fisiak, Jacek (ed.) 1978. Recent developments in historical phonology. Berlin and New York: Mouton de GruyterCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fisiak, Jacek (ed.) 1980. Historical morphology. The Hague, Paris and New York: MoutonCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fisiak, Jacek (ed.) 1985. Historical semantics. Historical word-formation. Berlin, New York and Amsterdam: Mouton de GruyterCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fleischman, Suzanne 1982. The future in thought and language: Diachronic evidence from Romance. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Fletcher, Paul, and Garman, Michael (eds.) 1986. Language acquisition. Studies in first language development. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fletcher, Paul, and MacWhinney, Brian (eds.) 1994. The handbook of child language. Oxford: BlackwellGoogle Scholar
Fodor, Janet D., and Inoue, Atsu 1994. ‘The diagnosis and cure of garden-paths’, Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 23(5): 407–34CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fodor, Janet D., and Inoue, Atsu 2000. ‘Garden path reanalysis: Attach (anyway) and revision as last resort’, in de Vincenzi, and Lombardo, (eds.), pp. 2162CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forster, Kenneth I. 1976. ‘Accessing the mental lexicon’, in Wales, and Walker, (eds.), pp. 231–56Google Scholar
Fought, Carmen (ed.) 2004. Sociolinguistic variation: Critical reflections. Oxford: Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Francis, Elaine J., and Yuasa, Etsuyo 2008. ‘A multi-modular approach to gradual change in grammaticalization’, Journal of Linguistics 44(1): 4586CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Francis, W. Nelson, and Kučera, Henry 1979. Brown corpus manual. Brown University: Department of LinguisticsGoogle Scholar
Frazier, Lyn 1979. On comprehending sentences: Syntactic parsing strategies. Doctoral dissertation. University of Connecticut.Google Scholar
Frazier, Lyn 1987. ‘Sentence processing: A tutorial review’, in Coltheart, (ed.), pp. 559–86Google Scholar
Frazier, Lyn, and Clifton, Charles 1996. Construal. Cambridge, MA: The MIT PressGoogle Scholar
Frazier, Lyn, and Rayner, Keith 1982. ‘Making and correcting errors during sentence comprehension: Eye movements in the analysis of structurally ambiguous sentences’, Cognitive Psychology 14(2): 178210CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frenck-Mestre, Cheryl, and Pynte, Joël 1997. ‘Syntactic ambiguity resolution while reading in second and native languages’, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 50(1): 119–48Google Scholar
Freudenthal, Daniel, Pine, Julian M., Aguado-Orea, Javier, and Gobet, Fernand 2007. ‘Modelling the developmental patterning of finiteness marking in English, Dutch, German and Spanish using MOSAIC’, Cognitive Science 31(2): 311–41CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Freudenthal, Daniel, Pine, Julian M., and Gobet, Fernand 2009. ‘Simulating the referential properties of Dutch, German, and English root infinitives in MOSAIC’, Language Learning and Development 5(1): 129CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Freudenthal, Daniel, Pine, Julian M., and Gobet, Fernand 2010. ‘Explaining quantitative variation in the rate of optional infinitive errors across languages: A comparison of MOSAIC and the Variational Learning Model’, Journal of Child Language 37(3): 643–69CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Friederici, Angela 2002. ‘Towards a neural basis of auditory sentence processing’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6(2): 7884CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Frisch, Stefan F., Large, Nathan R., Zawaydeh, Bushra, and Pisoni, David B. 2001. ‘Emergent phonotactic generalizations in English and Arabic’, in Bybee, and Hopper, (eds.), pp. 159–80CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fuchs, Susanne, Grice, Martine, Hermes, Anne, Lancia, Leonardo, and Mücke, Doris (eds.) 2014. Proceedings of the 10th International Seminar on Speech Production (ISSP), 5–8 May 2014. Cologne, Germany. Köln: Universität KölnGoogle Scholar
Gaeta, Livio, and Ricca, Davide 2006. ‘Productivity in Italian word formation: A variable-corpus approach’, Linguistics 44(1): 5789CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gahl, Susanne 2008. ‘Time and thyme are not homophones: The effect of lemma frequency on word durations in spontaneous speech’, Language 84(3): 474–96CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gahl, Susanne, and Baayen, R. Harald in preparation. ‘Vowel space expands over the lifespan’.Google Scholar
Gahl, Susanne, Cibelli, Emily, Hall, Kathleen, and Sprouse, Ronald 2014. ‘The “UP” corpus: A corpus of speech samples across adulthood’, Corpus Linguistics and Lingustic Theory 10(2): 315–28Google Scholar
Gahl, Susanne, Yao, Yao, and Johnson, Keith 2012. ‘Why reduce? Phonological neighborhood density and phonetic reduction in spontaneous speech’, Journal of Memory and Language 66(4): 789806CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galaburda, Albert M. (ed.) 1989. From reading to neurons. Issues in the Biology of Language and Cognition. Cambridge, MA: The MIT PressGoogle Scholar
Galbi, Douglas A. 2002. ‘Long-term trends in personal given name frequencies in England and Wales’, A Journal of Onomastic 51(4): 105–32Google Scholar
García Mayo, María d. P., Gutiérrez Mangado, María J., and Martínez Adrián, María (eds.) 2013. Contemporary approaches to second language acquisition. Amsterdam: John BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, Anne 2014. Derivation in Middle English: Regional and text type variation. Mémoires de la Société Néophilologique de Helsinki 92. Helsinki: Société NéophilologiqueGoogle Scholar
Gardner, Michael K., Rothkopf, Ernst Z., Lapan, Richard, and Lafferty, Toby 1987. ‘The word frequency effect in lexical decision: Finding a frequency-based component’, Memory and Cognition 15(1): 2428CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garnsey, Susan M., Pearlmutter, Neal J., Myers, Elizabeth, and Lotocky, Melanie A. 1997. ‘The contributions of verb bias and plausibility to the comprehension of temporarily ambiguous sentences’, Journal of Memory and Language 37(1): 5893CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garrett, Andrew 2011. ‘The historical syntax problem: Reanalysis and directionality’, in Jonas, , Whitman, and Garrett, (eds.), pp. 5272CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garrod, Simon, and Anderson, Anthony 1987. ‘Saying what you mean in dialogue: A study in conceptual and semantic co-ordination’, Cognition 27(2): 181218CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Garrod, Simon, and Clark, Aileen 1993. ‘The development of dialogue co-ordination skills in schoolchildren’, Language and Cognitive Processes 8(1): 101–26CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garrod, Simon, and Doherty, Gwyneth 1994. ‘Conversation, co-ordination and convention: An empirical investigation of how groups establish linguistic conventions’, Cognition 53(3): 181215CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gass, Susan M., and Mackey, Alison (eds.) 2012. The Routledge handbook of second language acquisition. London and New York: RoutledgeGoogle Scholar
Geeraerts, Dirk, and Cuyckens, Hubert (eds.) 2007. The Oxford handbook of cognitive linguistics. Oxford and New York: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
Gentner, Dedre 1983. ‘Structure-mapping: A theoretical framework for analogy’, Cognitive Science 7(2): 155–70Google Scholar
Gentner, Dedre 1988. ‘Metaphor as structure mapping: The relational shift’, Child Development 59(1): 4759CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gentner, Dedre 2003. ‘Why we’re so smart’, in Gentner, and Goldin-Meadow, (eds.), pp. 195235CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gentner, Dedre 2010. ‘Bootstrapping the mind: Analogical processes and symbol systems’, Cognitive Science 34(5): 752–75CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gentner, Dedre, Anggoro, Florencia K., and Klibanoff, Raquel S. 2011. ‘Structure mapping and relational language support children’s learning of relational categories’, Child Development 82(4): 1173–88CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gentner, Dedre, and Colhoun, Julie 2010. ‘Analogical processes in human thinking and learning’, in Glatzeder, , Goel, and Müller, (eds.), pp. 3548CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gentner, Dedre, and Goldin-Meadow, Susan (eds.) 2003. Language in mind: Advances in the study of language and cognition. Cambridge, MA: The MIT PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gentner, Dedre, Holyoak, Keith J., and Kokinov, Boicho K. (eds.) 2001. The analogical mind. Perspectives from cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: The MIT PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gentner, Dedre, Levine, Susan C., Ping, Raedy, Isaia, Ashley, Dhillon, Sonica, Bradley, Claire, and Honke, Garrett 2016. ‘Rapid learning in a children’s museum via analogical comparison’, Cognitive Science 40(1): 224–40CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gentner, Dedre, and Markman, Arthur B. 1997. ‘Structure mapping in analogy and similarity’, American Psychologist 52(1): 4556CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gentner, Dedre, and Medina, Jose 1998. ‘Similarity and the development of rulesCognition 65(2): 263–97CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gentner, Dedre, and Namy, Laura L. 2006. ‘Analogical processes in language learning’, Current Directions in Psychological Science 15(6): 297301CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gentner, Dedre, Rattermann, Mary J., and Forbus, Kenneth D. 1993. ‘The roles of similarity in transfer: Separating retrieval from inferential soundness’, Cognitive Psychology 25(4): 524–75CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gentner, Dedre, and Smith, Linsey 2012. ‘Analogical reasoning’, in Ramachandran, (ed.), pp. 130–36CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gernsbacher, Morton A. (ed.) 1994. Handbook of psycholinguistics. San Diego, CA: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
Gescheider, George A. 2013. Psychophysics: The fundamentals. New York and London: Psychology PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giacalone Ramat, Anna, Carruba, Onofrio, and Bernini, Giuliano (eds.) 1987. Papers from the 7th International Conference on Historical Linguistics. Amsterdam: John BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giacalone Ramat, Anna, Mauri, Caterina, and Molinelli, Piera (eds.) 2013. Synchrony and diachrony: A dynamic interface. Amsterdam: BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, Edward 1991. A computational theory of human linguistic processing: Memory limitations and processing breakdown. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. Carnegie Mellon.Google Scholar
Gibson, Edward 1998. ‘Syntactic complexity: Locality of syntactic dependencies’, Cognition 68(1): 175CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibson, Edward, and Pearlmutter, Neal 1998. ‘Constraints on sentence comprehension’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences 2(7): 262–68CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gibson, James J. 1977. The theory of affordances. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence ErlbaumGoogle Scholar
Gick, Bryan 2002. ‘The use of ultrasound for linguistic phonetic fieldwork’, Journal of the International Phonetic Association 32(2): 113–21CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gick, Mary L., and Holyoak, Keith J. 1980. ‘Analogical problem solving’, Cognitive Psychology 12(3): 306–55CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gilhooly, Ken J., and Logie, Robert H. 1980. ‘Age-of-acquisition, imagery, concreteness, familiarity, and ambiguity measures for 1,944 words’, Behavior Research Methods and Instrumentation 12(4): 395427CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giora, Rachel 1997. ‘Understanding figurative and literal language: The graded salience hypothesis’, Cognitive Linguistics 8(3): 183206CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giora, Rachel 2003. On our mind: Salience, context, and figurative language. New York: Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giora, Rachel 2012. ‘The psychology of utterance processing’, in Allan, and Jaszczolt, (eds.), pp. 151–67CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Givón, Talmy 1971. ‘Historical syntax and synchronic morphology: An archeologist’s field trip’, Chicago Linguistic Society 7(1): 394415Google Scholar
Givón, Talmy 1979. On understanding grammar. New York: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
Givón, Talmy 1998. ‘On the co-evolution of language, mind and brain’, Evolution of Communication 2(1): 45116CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Givón, Talmy 2002. ‘The visual information-processing system as an evolutionary precursor of human language’, in Givón, and Malle, (eds.), pp. 350CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Givón, Talmy 2009. The genesis of syntactic complexity: Diachrony, ontogeny, neuro-cognition, evolution. Amsterdam: John BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Givón, Talmy, and Malle, Bertram (eds.) 2002. The evolution of language out of pre-language. Amsterdam: BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glatzeder, Britt, Goel, Vinod, and Müller, Albrecht (eds.) 2010. On thinking, vol. 2: Towards a theory of thinking. Berlin: SpringerCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gleitman, Lila R. 1990. ‘The structural sources of verb meaning’, Language Acquisition 1(1): 355CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gluck, Mark A., Meeter, Martijn, and Myers, Catherine E. 2003. ‘Computational models of the hippocampal region: Linking incremental learning and episodic memory’, Trends in Cognitive Science 7(6): 269–76CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glushko, Robert J. 1979. ‘The organization and activation of orthographic knowledge in reading aloud’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance 5(4): 674–91Google Scholar
Glynn, Dylan, and Fischer, Kerstin (eds.) 2010. Quantitative methods in cognitive semantics: Corpus-driven approaches. Berlin and New York: Mouton de GruyterCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gnutzmann, Claus 1975. ‘Some aspects of grading’, English Studies 56(5): 421–33CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Godden, Duncan R., and Baddeley, Alan D. 1975. ‘Context-dependent memory in two natural environments: On land and underwater’, British Journal of Psychology 66(3): 325–31CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goebel, Rainer, and Indefrey, Peter 2000. ‘A recurrent network with short-term memory capacity learning the German-s plural’, in Broeder, and Murre, (eds.), pp. 177200CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, Adele E. 1995. Constructions: A construction grammar approach to argument structure. Chicago: University of Chicago PressGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, Adele E. 2003. ‘Constructions: A new theoretical approach to language’, Trends in Cognitive Science 7(5): 219–24CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, Adele E. 2006. Constructions at work. The nature of generalization in language. Oxford: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
Goldrick, Matthew, Ferreira, Victor S., and Miozzo, Michele (eds.) 2014. The Oxford handbook of language production. Oxford: Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldschneider, Jennifer M., and DeKeyser, Robert 2001. ‘Explaining the “natural order of L2 morpheme acquisition” in English: A meta-analysis of multiple determinants’, Language Learning 51(1): 150CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldwater, Micah B., Tomlinson, Marc T., Echols, Catharina H., and Love, Bradley C. 2011. ‘Structural priming as structure-mapping: Children use analogies from previous utterances to guide sentence production’, Cognitive Science 35(1): 156–70CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodman, Judith C., Nusbaum, Howard C., Lee, Lisa, and Broihier, Kevin 1990. ‘The effects of syntactic and discourse variables on the segmental intelligibility of speech’. The First International Conference on Spoken Language Processing, ICSLP 1990, Kobe, Japan, November 18–22.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goosens, Louis 1982. ‘On the development of the modals and of the epistemic function in English’, in Ahlqvist, (ed.), pp. 7484CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gould, Stephen Jay 1977. Ontogeny and phylogeny. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University PressGoogle Scholar
Graddol, David 2000. The future of English. London: The British CouncilGoogle Scholar
Greene, Robert L. 1986. ‘Sources of recency effects in free recall’, Psychological Bulletin 99(2): 221–28CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gries, Stefan Th. 2003. ‘Towards a corpus-based identification of prototypical instances of constructions’, Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics 1(1): 127CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gries, Stefan Th. 2005. ‘Syntactic priming: A corpus-based approach’, Journal of Psycholinguistic Research 34(4): 365–99CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gries, Stefan Th. 2008. ‘Dispersions and adjusted frequencies in corpora’, International Journal of Corpus Linguistics 13(4): 403–37CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gries, Stefan Th. 2010. ‘Dispersions and adjusted frequencies in corpora: Further explorations’, in Gries, , Wulff, and Davies, (eds.), pp. 197212CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gries, Stefan Th. 2013. ‘Sources of variability relevant to the (cognitive) sociolinguist, and quantitative corpus methods to handle them’, Journal of Pragmatics 52(6): 516CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gries, Stefan Th. 2015. ‘The most underused statistical method in corpus linguistics: Multi-level (and mixed-effects) models’, Corpora 10(1): 95125CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gries, Stefan Th., and David, Caroline 2007. ‘This is kind of/sort of interesting: Variation in hedging in English’, VARIENG e-Series 2: Towards Multimedia in Corpus Studies. Helsinki: Research Unit for Variation, Contacts and Change in English (VARIENG). www.helsinki.fi/varieng/journal/volumes/02/gries_david/ [accessed August 23, 2010]Google Scholar
Gries, Stefan Th., and Divjak, Dagmar S. 2009. ‘Behavioral profiles: A corpus-based approach towards cognitive semantic analysis’, in Evans, and Pourcel, (eds.), pp. 5775CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gries, Stefan Th., and Divjak, Dagmar S. (eds.) 2012. Frequency effects in language learning and processing. Berlin and New York: Mouton de GruyterCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gries, Stefan Th., and Ellis, Nick C. 2015. ‘Statistical measures for usage-based linguistics’, Currents in Language Learning 65(s1): 228–55Google Scholar
Gries, Stefan Th., Hampe, Beate, and Schönefeld, Doris 2005. ‘Converging evidence: Bringing together experimental and corpus data on the association of verbs and constructions’, Cognitive Linguistics 16(4): 635–76CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gries, Stefan Th., and Stefanowitsch, Anatol 2004. ‘Extending collostructional analysis: A corpus-based perspective on “alternations”’, International Journal of Corpus Linguistics 9(1): 97129CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gries, Stefan Th., Wulff, Stefanie, and Davies, Mark (eds.) 2010. Corpus linguistic applications: Current studies, new directions. Amsterdam: RodopiCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Griffin, Zenzi M. 2001. ‘Gaze durations during speech reflect word selection and phonological encoding’, Cognition 82(1): B1B14CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guo, Jiansheng, Lieven, Elena V. M., Budwig, Nancy, Ervin-Tripp, Susan, Nakamura, Kei, and Őzçalişkan, Seyda (eds.) 2008. Cross-linguistic approaches to the psychology of language: Research in the tradition of Dan Isaac Slobin. New York and London: Psychology PressGoogle Scholar
Hahn, Ulrike, and Nakisa, Ramin C. 2000. ‘German inflection: Single route or dual route?’, Cognitive Psychology 41(4): 313–60CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haiman, John 1983. ‘Iconic and economic motivation’, Language 59(4): 781819CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haiman, John (ed.) 1985. Iconicity in syntax. Amsterdam: John BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haiman, John 1994. ‘Ritualization and the development of language’, in Pagliuca, (ed.), pp. 328CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hale, John T. 2004. Grammar, uncertainty and sentence processing. Doctoral dissertation. The Johns Hopkins University.Google Scholar
Hale, John T. 2011. ‘What a rational parser would do’, Cognitive Science 35(3): 399443CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hall, Kira, Koenig, Jean-Pierre, Meacham, Michael, Reinman, Sondra, and Sutton, Laurel A. (eds.) 1990. Proceedings of the Sixteenth Annual Meeting of the Berkeley Linguistics Society. Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics SocietyGoogle Scholar
Halle, Michael 1962. ‘Phonology in generative grammar’, Word 18: 5472CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hanks, Patrick 2013. Lexical analysis: Norms and exploitations. Cambridge, MA: The MIT PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hansen, Björn, and de Haan, Ferdinand 2009. Modals in the languages of Europe. Berlin: Mouton de GruyterCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hansen Mosegaard, Maj-Britt, and Waltereit, Richard 2006. ‘GCI theory and language change’, Acta Linguistica Hafniensia 38(1): 235–68Google Scholar
Harm, Michael W., and Seidenberg, Mark S. 1999. ‘Phonology, reading acquisition, and dyslexia: Insights from connectionist models’, Psychological Review 106(3): 491528CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harnad, Steven R., Steklis, Horst D., and Lancaster, Jane (eds.) 1976. The origins and evolution of language and speech. New York: New York Academy of SciencesGoogle Scholar
Harris, Alice C., and Campbell, Lyle 1995. Historical syntax in cross-linguistic perspective. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartsuiker, Robert J., Bernolet, Sarah, Schoonbaert, Sophie, Speybroeck, Sara, and Vanderelst, Dieter 2008. ‘Syntactic priming persists while the lexical boost decays: Evidence from written and spoken dialogue’, Journal of Memory and Language 58(2): 214–38CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hartsuiker, Robert J., and Kolk, Herman H. J. 1998. ‘Syntactic persistence in Dutch’, Language and Speech 41(2): 143–84CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haselow, Alexander 2011. ‘Discourse marker and modal particle: The functions of utterance-final then in spoken English’, Journal of Pragmatics 43(14): 3603–23CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haselow, Alexander 2012. ‘Discourse organization and the rise of final then in the history of English’, in Hegedüs, and Fodor, (eds.), pp. 153–75CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haspelmath, Martin 1998. ‘Does grammaticalization need reanalysis?’, Studies in Language 22(2): 315–51CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haspelmath, Martin 1999. ‘Why is grammaticalization irreversible?’, Linguistics 37(6): 1043–68CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, John A. (ed.) 1988. Explaining language universals. Oxford: Basil BlackwellGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, John A. 1994. A performance theory of order and constituency. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, John A. 2004. Efficiency and complexity in grammars. Oxford: Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, John A. 2012. ‘The drift of English towards invariable word order from a typological and Germanic perspective’, in Nevalainen, and Traugott, (eds.), pp. 622–32CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hawkins, John A. 2014. Cross-linguistic variation and efficiency. Oxford: Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hay, Jennifer 2001. ‘Lexical frequency in morphology: Is everything relative?’, Linguistics 39(6): 1041–70CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hebb, Donald O. 1949. The organization of behaviour. New York: John Wiley and SonsGoogle Scholar
Hegedüs, Irén, and Fodor, Alexandra (eds.) 2012. English historical linguistics 2010. Amsterdam: BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heine, Bernd 1993. Auxiliaries: Cognitive forces and grammaticalization. Oxford: Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heine, Bernd 2002. ‘On the role of context in grammaticalization’, in Wischer, and Diewald, (eds.), pp. 83101CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heine, Bernd 2014. ‘Grammaticalization, metaphor, and explanation: What accounts for unidirectionality?’. Plenary paper presented at the Workshop de Gramaticalização II, Universidade Federal Fluminense, Niterói, May 7, 2014Google Scholar
Heine, Bernd, Claudi, Ulrike, and Hünnemeyer, Friederike 1991. Grammaticalization: A conceptual framework. Chicago: University of Chicago PressGoogle Scholar
Heine, Bernd, and Narrog, Heiko (eds.) 2011. The Oxford handbook of grammaticalization. Oxford: Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hendriks, Petra, Englert, Christina, Wubs, Ellis, and Hoeks, John 2008. ‘Age differences in adults’ use of referring expressions’, Journal of Logic, Language and Information 17(4): 443–66CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hendrix, Peter 2015. Experimental explorations of a discrimination learning approach to language processing. Doctoral dissertation. University of TübingenGoogle Scholar
Herron, Daniel, and Bates, Elizabeth 1997. ‘Sentential and acoustic factors in the recognition of open- and closed-class words’, Journal of Memory and Language 37(2): 217–39CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hickey, Raymond (ed.) 2003. Motives for language change. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hickey, Raymond 2013. ‘English as a contact language in Ireland and Scotland’, in Schreier, and Hundt, (eds.), pp. 206–46CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hill, Robin L., and Murray, Wayne S. 2000. ‘Commas and spaces: Effects of punctuation on eye movements and sentence parsing’, in Kennedy, , Radach, , Heller, and Pynte, (eds.), pp. 565–89CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hilpert, Martin 2008. Germanic future constructions: A usage-based approach to language change. Amsterdam: John BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hilpert, Martin 2011. ‘Dynamic visualizations of language change: Motion charts on the basis of bivariate and multivariate data from diachronic corpora’, International Journal of Corpus Linguistics 16(4): 435–61CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hilpert, Martin 2013. Constructional change in English: Developments in allomorphy, word formation, and syntax. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hilpert, Martin, and Gries, Stefan Th. 2009. ‘Assessing frequency changes in multi-stage diachronic corpora: Applications for historical corpus linguistics and the study of language acquisition’, Literary and Linguistic Computing 34(4): 385401CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Himmelmann, Nikolaus P. 2004. ‘Lexicalization and grammaticization: Opposite or orthogonal?’, in Bisang, , Himmelmann, and Wiemer, (eds.), pp. 2142Google Scholar
Hinskens, Frans, Auer, Peter, and Kerswill, Paul 2005. ‘The study of dialect convergence and divergence. Conceptual and methodological considerations’, in Auer, , Hinskens, and Kerswill, (eds.), pp. 150CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hock, Hans Heinrich 1991. Principles of historical linguistics. Berlin and New York: Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hodge, Carleton 1970. ‘The linguistic cycle’, Linguistic Sciences 13(7): 17Google Scholar
Hoffmann, Sebastian 2005. Grammaticalization and English complex prepositions: A corpus-based study. London and New York: RoutledgeGoogle Scholar
Hoffmann, Thomas, and Trousdale, Graeme (eds.) 2013. Oxford handbook of construction grammar. Oxford: Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hofstadter, Douglas 1995. Fluid concepts and creative analogies. Computer models of the fundamental mechanisms of thought. New York: Basic BooksGoogle Scholar
Hofstadter, Douglas, and Sander, Emmanuel 2013. Surfaces and essences: Analogy as the fuel and fire of thinking. New York: Basic BooksGoogle Scholar
Hogg, Richard M. 1988. ‘Snuck: The development of irregular preterite forms’, in Nixon, and Honey, (eds.), pp. 3140CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hogg, Richard M., and Denison, David (eds.) 2006. A history of the English language. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hollmann, Willem B. 2003. Synchrony and diachrony of English periphrastic causatives: A cognitive perspective. Doctoral dissertation. University of Manchester.Google Scholar
Hollmann, Willem, and Siewierska, Anna 2006. ‘Corpora and (the need for) other methods in a study of a Lancashire dialect to require independent factors’, Zeitschrift für Anglistik und Amerikanistik 53: 203–16.Google Scholar
Holmes, Virginia, Stowe, Laurie, and Cupples, Linda 1989. ‘Lexical expectations in parsing complement-verb sentences’, Journal of Memory and Language 28(6): 668–89CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holyoak, Keith J., and Thagard, Paul 1995. Mental leaps: Analogy in creative thought. Cambridge, MA: The MIT PressGoogle Scholar
Hooper, Celia R., and Cralidis, Ann 2009. ‘Normal changes in the speech of older adults: You’ve still got what it takes; it just takes a little longer!’, SIG 15 Perspectives on Gerontology 14(2): 4756CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hooper, Joan B. 1976. ‘Word frequency in lexical diffusion and the source of morphophonological change’, in Christie, (ed.), pp. 96105Google Scholar
Hooper, Joan B. 1980. ‘Child morphology and morphophonemic change’, in Fisiak, (ed.), pp. 157–87CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopkins, Gerald M. 1918. ‘Pied Beauty’: Poems of Gerald Manley Hopkins. London: Humphrey MilfordGoogle Scholar
Hopper, Paul J. (ed.) 1982. Tense-aspect: Between semantics and pragmatics. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopper, Paul J. 1998. ‘Emergent grammar’, in Tomasello, (ed.), pp. 155–75CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopper, Paul J., and Thompson, Sandra A. 1980. ‘Transitivity in grammar and discourse’, Language 56(2): 251–99CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopper, Paul J., and Traugott, Elizabeth C. 2003. Grammaticalization. 2nd edition. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horn, Laurence R. 1984. ‘Toward a new taxonomy for pragmatic inference: Q-based and R-based implicature’, in Schiffrin, (ed.), pp. 1142Google Scholar
Huang, Yi Ting, and Pinker, Steven 2010. ‘Lexical semantics and irregular inflection’, Language and Cognitive Processes 25(10): 1411–61CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huddleston, Rodney D., and Pullum, Geoffrey K. 2002. The Cambridge grammar of the English language. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hume, Elizabeth, Johnson, Keith, Seo, Misun, and Tserdanelis, Georgios 1999. ‘A cross-linguistic study of stop place perception’, in Ohala, , Hasegawa, , Ohala, , Granville, and Bailey, (eds.), pp. 2069–72Google Scholar
Hundt, Marianne (ed.) 2014a. Late Modern English syntax. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hundt, Marianne 2014b. ‘The demise of the being to V construction’, Transactions of the Philological Society 112(2): 167–87CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hundt, Marianne, and Leech, Geoffrey 2012. ‘“Small is beautiful”: On the value of standard reference corpora for observing recent grammatical change’, in Nevalainen, and Traugott, (eds.), pp. 175–88CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, Janellen, Vasilyeva, Marina, Cymerman, Elina, and Levine, Susan 2002. ‘Language input and child syntax’, Cognitive Psychology 45(3): 337–74CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huttenlocher, Janellen, Vasilyeva, Marina, and Shimpi, Priya 2004. ‘Syntactic priming in young children’, Journal of Memory and Language 50(2): 182–95CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ibbotson, Paul 2013. ‘The scope of usage-based theory’, Frontiers in Psychology 4: 255CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ibbotson, Paul, and Tomasello, Michael 2009. ‘Prototype constructions in early language acquisition’, Language and Cognition 1(1): 5985CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Itkonen, Esa 2005. Analogy as structure and process. Amsterdam: BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ivanova, Iva, Pickering, Martin J., Branigan, Holly P., McLean, Janet F., and Costa, Albert 2012. ‘The comprehension of anomalous sentences: Evidence from structural priming’, Cognition 122(2): 193209CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jackendoff, Ray 2002. Foundations of language. Oxford: Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacob, Gunnar, and Felser, Claudia 2015. ‘Reanalysis and semantic persistence in native and non-native garden-path recovery’, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 69(5): 119Google ScholarPubMed
Jaeger, T. Florian, and Snider, Neal E. 2013. ‘Alignment as a consequence of expectation adaptation: Syntactic priming is affected by the prime’s prediction error given both prior and recent experience’, Cognition 127(1): 5783CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jäger, Gerhard, and Rosenbach, Anette 2008. ‘Priming and unidirectional language change’, Theoretical Linguistics 34(2): 85113CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, William 1890a. The principles of psychology, vol. 1. New York: DoverGoogle Scholar
James, William 1890b. The principles of psychology, vol. 2. New York: HoltGoogle Scholar
Jankowski, Bridget 2004. ‘A transatlantic perspective of variation and change in English deontic modality’, Toronto Working Papers in Linguistics 23: 85113Google Scholar
Jared, Debra, McRae, Ken, and Seidenberg, Mark S. 1990. ‘The basis of consistency effects in word naming’, Journal of Memory and Language 29(6): 687715CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jescheniak, Jörg D., and Levelt, Willem J. M. 1994. ‘Word frequency effects in speech production: Retrieval of syntactic information and of phonological form’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 20(4): 824–43Google Scholar
Jespersen, Otto 1909–49. A Modern English grammar on historical principles. 7 vols. Heidelberg: WinterGoogle Scholar
Jespersen, Otto 1922. Language: Its nature, development and origin. London: Allen and UnwinGoogle Scholar
Jiang, Nan, and Nekrasova, Tatiana M. 2007. ‘The processing of formulaic sequences by second language speakers’, The Modern Language Journal 91(3): 433–45CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, Christopher R. 1999. Constructional grounding: The role of interpretational overlap in lexical and constructional acquisition. Doctoral dissertation. University of California, BerkeleyGoogle Scholar
Johnson, Christopher R. 2001. ‘Constructional grounding: On the relation between deictic and existential there constructions in acquisition’, in Cienki, , Luka, and Smith, (eds.), pp. 123–36Google Scholar
Johnson, Christopher R. 2005. ‘Developmental reinterpretation in first language acquisition’. Paper presented at the Symposium ‘Exemplar-based models in linguistics, 79th meeting of the Linguistic Society of America’, Oakland, January 9Google Scholar
Jonas, Dianne, Whitman, John, and Garrett, Andrew (eds.) 2011. Grammatical change: Origins, nature, outcomes. Oxford: Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, Benjamin F. 2005. ‘The burden of knowledge and the “death of the Renaissance man”: Is innovation getting harder?’ Working Paper 11360.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, Gregory V. 1985. ‘Deep dyslexia, imageability, and ease of predication’, Brain and Language 24(1): 119CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jones, Mari C., and Esch, Edith (eds.) 2002. Language change: The interplay of internal, external, and extra-linguistic factors. Berlin and New York: Mouton de GruyterCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Joseph, Brian D., and Janda, Richard D. (eds.) 2003. The handbook of historical linguistics. Oxford: BlackwellCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jucker, Andreas H. 1995. Historical pragmatics: Pragmatic developments in the history of English. Amsterdam: BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jurafsky, Daniel 2002. ‘Probabilistic modeling in psycholinguistics: Linguistic comprehension and production’, in Bod, , Hay, and Jannedy, (eds.), pp. 3996CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jurafsky, Daniel, Bell, Alan, Gregory, Michelle, and Raymond, William D. 2001. ‘Probabilistic relations between words: Evidence from reduction in lexical production’, in Bybee, and Hopper, (eds.), pp. 229–54CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kamide, Yuki, and Mitchell, Don 1999. ‘Incremental pre-head attachment in Japanese parsing’, Language and Cognitive Processes 14(5–6): 631–32CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kamin, Leon J. 1969. ‘Predictability, surprise, attention, and conditioning’, in Campbell, and Church, (eds.), pp. 279–96Google Scholar
Kantola, Leila, and van Gompel, Roger P. G. 2011. ‘Between- and within-language priming is the same: Evidence for shared bilingual syntactic representations’, Memory and Cognition 39(2): 276–90CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaschak, Michael P. 2006. ‘What this construction needs is generalized’, Memory and Cognition 34(2): 368–79CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaschak, Michael P., and Borreggine, Kristin L. 2008. ‘Is long-term structural priming affected by patterns of experience with individual verbs?’, Journal of Memory and Language 58(3): 862–78CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaschak, Michael P., and Glenberg, Arthur M. 2004. ‘This construction needs learned’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: General 133(3): 450–67Google ScholarPubMed
Kaschak, Michael P., Kutta, Timothy J., and Coyle, Jacqueline M. 2014. ‘Long and short term cumulative structural priming effects’, Language, Cognition and Neuroscience 29(6): 728–43CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaschak, Michael P., Kutta, Timothy J., and Schatschneider, Chris 2011. ‘Long-term cumulative structural priming persists for (at least) one week’, Memory and Cognition 39(3): 381–88CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kastovsky, Dieter (ed.) 1994. Studies in Modern English. Berlin: Mouton de GruyterGoogle Scholar
Kay, Paul 1997a. ‘The kind of/sort of construction’, in Kay, (ed.), pp. 145–58Google Scholar
Kay, Paul (ed.) 1997b. Words and the grammar of context. Stanford, CA: CSLI PublicationsGoogle Scholar
Ke, Jinyun 2006. ‘A cross-linguistic quantitative study of homophony’, Journal of Quantitative Linguistics 13(1): 129–59CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kecskes, Istvan 2012. ‘Sociopragmatics and cross-cultural and intercultural studies’, in Allan, and Jaszczolt, (eds.), pp. 599616CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kecskes, Istvan 2013. Intercultural pragmatics. Oxford: Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keizer, Evelien 2007. The English noun phrase: The nature of linguistic categorization. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keller, Rudi 1994. On language change: The invisible hand in language. London: Routledge [Trans. Nerlich, Brigitte; first published 1990 in German]Google Scholar
Kello, Christopher T., Brown, Gordon D. A., Ferrer-i-Cancho, Ramon, Holden, John G., Linkenkaer-Hansen, Klaus, Rhodes, Theo, and van Orden, Guy C. 2010. ‘Scaling laws in cognitive sciences’, Trends in Cognitive Science 14(5): 223–32CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kemps, Rachel, Ernestus, Mirjam, Schreuder, Robert, and Baayen, R. Harald 2005a. ‘Prosodic cues for morphological complexity: The case of Dutch noun plurals’, Memory and Cognition 33(3): 430–46CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kemps, Rachel, Wurm, Lee H., Ernestus, Mirjam, Schreuder, Robert, and Baayen, R. Harald 2005b. ‘Prosodic cues for morphological complexity in Dutch and English’, Language and Cognitive Processes 20(1–2): 4373CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kennedy, Alan, Radach, Ralph, Heller, Dieter, and Pynte, Joël (eds.) 2000. Reading as a perceptual process. Amsterdam: North-Holland/Elsevier Science PublishersCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kepser, Stephan, and Reis, Marga (eds.) 2005. Linguistic evidence. Empirical, theoretical, and computational perspectives. Berlin and New York: Mouton de GruyterCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kerswill, Paul 1996. ‘Children, adolescents and language change’, Language Variation and Change 8(2): 177202CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kerswill, Paul, and Williams, Ann 2002. ‘“Salience” as an explanatory factor in language change: Evidence from dialect levelling in urban England’, in Jones, and Esch, (eds.), pp. 81110CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Keuleers, Emmanuel, Stevens, Michaël, Mandera, Pawel, and Brysbaert, Marc 2015. ‘Word knowledge in the crowd: Measuring vocabulary size and word prevalence in a massive online experiment’, The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 66(8): 1665–92Google Scholar
Kiparsky, Paul 1968. ‘Linguistic universals and linguistic change’, in Bach, and Harms, (eds.), pp. 170202Google Scholar
Kiparsky, Paul 2014. ‘New perspectives in historical linguistics’, in Bowern, and Evans, (eds.), pp. 64102Google Scholar
Kirby, Simon 1999. Function, selection and innateness. Oxford: Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirjavainen, Minna, Lieven, Elena V. M., and Theakston, Anna L. 2016. ‘Can infinitival to omissions and provisions be primed? An experimental investigation into the role of constructional competition in infinitival to omission errors’, Cognitive Science: 132 [DOI: 10.1111/cogs.12407]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirjavainen, Minna, and Theakston, Anna L. 2011. ‘Are infinitival to omission errors primed by prior discourse? The case of WANT constructions’, Cognitive Linguistics 22(4): 629–57CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirjavainen, Minna, Theakston, Anna L., and Lieven, Elena V. M. 2009. ‘Can input explain children’s me-for-I errors?’, Journal of Child Language 36(5): 1091–114CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirjavainen, Minna, Theakston, Anna L., Lieven, Elena V. M., and Tomasello, Michael 2009. ‘“I want hold Postman Pat”: An investigation into the acquisition of infinitival marker to’, First Language 29(3): 313–39CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klein, Wolfgang 1998. ‘The contribution of second language acquisition research’, Language Learning 48(4): 527–50CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Köhler, Reinhard 1986. Zur linguistischen Synergetik: Struktur und Dynamik der Lexik. Bochum: BrockmeyerGoogle Scholar
Köhler, Reinhard, Altmann, Gabriel, and Piotrowski, Rajmund G. (eds.) 2005. Quantitative linguistics. An international handbook. Berlin and New York: Mouton de GruyterGoogle Scholar
Kohnen, Thomas, and Mair, Christian 2012. ‘Technologies of communication’, in Nevalainen, and Traugott, (eds.), pp. 261–84CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Konopka, Agnieszka E., and Bock, J. Kathryn 2005. ‘Helping syntax out: What do words do?’ Paper presented at the CUNY Sentence Processing Conference, Tucson, ArizonaGoogle Scholar
Konopka, Agnieszka E., and Bock, J. Kathryn 2009. ‘Lexical or syntactic control of sentence formulation? Structural generalizations from idiom production’, Cognitive Psychology 58(1): 68101CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Köpcke, Klaus-Michael 1993. Schemata bei der Pluralbildung des Deutschen: Versuch einer kognitiven Morphologie. Tübingen: NarrGoogle Scholar
Köpcke, Klaus-Michael 1998. ‘The acquisition of plural marking in English and German revisited: Schemata vs. rules’, Journal of Child Language 25(2): 293319CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kortmann, Bernd, and Lunkenheimer, Kerstin (eds.) 2013. The Mouton world atlas of variation in English. Berlin and New York: Mouton de GruyterGoogle Scholar
Krasnegor, Norman A., Rumbaugh, Duane M., Schiefelbusch, Richard L., and Studdert-Kennedy, Michael (eds.) 1991. Biological and behavioral determinants of language development. Mahwah, NJ: ErlbaumGoogle Scholar
Krause, Anne 2017. Frequency effects on entrenchment: converging evidence from ongoing language change. Doctoral dissertation, University of FreiburgGoogle Scholar
Kroch, Anthony 2001. ‘Syntactic change’, in Baltin, and Collins, (eds.), pp. 699729CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krug, Manfred G. 1998. ‘String frequency. A cognitive motivating factor in coalescence, language processing, and linguistic change’, Journal of English Linguistics 26(4): 286320CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krug, Manfred G. 2000. Emerging English modals: A corpus-based study of grammaticalization. Berlin: Mouton de GruyterCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kruschke, John K. 2006. ‘Learned attention’. Fifth International Conference on Development and Learning, June 2006. Indiana UniversityGoogle Scholar
Kruschke, John K., and Blair, Nathaniel J. 2000. ‘Blocking and backward blocking involve learned inattention’, Psychonomic Bulletin and Review 7(4): 636–45CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuiper, Koenraad 1996. Smooth talkers: The linguistic performance of auctioneers and sportscasters. Mahwah, NJ: ErlbaumGoogle Scholar
Küntay, Aylin C., and Slobin, Dan I. 2002. ‘Putting interaction back into child language: Examples from Turkish’, Psychology of Language and Communication 6(1): 514Google Scholar
Kuperman, Victor, Pluymaekers, Mark, Ernestus, Mirjam, and Baayen, R. Harald 2007. ‘Morphological predictability and acoustic duration of interfixes in Dutch compounds’, Journal of the Acoustical Society of America 121(4): 2261–71CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuryłowicz, Jerzy 1949. ‘La nature des procès dits “analogiques”’, Acta Linguistica 5(1): 1537CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kytö, Merja 1991. Manual to the diachronic part of the Helsinki Corpus of English Texts: Coding conventions and lists of source texts. 3rd edition. Helsinki: Department of English, University of HelsinkiGoogle Scholar
Kytö, Merja (ed.) 2012. English corpus linguistics: Crossing paths. Amsterdam: RodopiCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kytö, Merja, and Pahta, Päivi (eds.) 2016. The Cambridge handbook of English historical linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Labov, William 1982. ‘Building on empirical foundations’, in Lehmann, and Malkiel, (eds.), pp. 1792Google Scholar
Laird, John E., Newell, Allen, and Rosenbloom, Paul S. 1987. ‘Soar: An architecture for general intelligence’, Artificial intelligence 33(1): 164CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laird, John E., Rosenbloom, Paul S., and Newell, Allen 1986. ‘Chunking in Soar: The anatomy of a general learning mechanism’, Machine learning 1(1): 1146CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lakoff, George 1987. Women, fire, and dangerous things: What categories reveal about the mind. Chicago: University of Chicago PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lamb, Michael E., and Brown, Ann L. (eds.) 1982. Advances in developmental psychology, vol. 2. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence ErlbaumGoogle Scholar
Lamendella, John T. 1976. ‘Relations between the ontogeny and phylogeny of language: A neo-recapitulationist view’, in Harnad, , Steklis, and Lancaster, (eds.), pp. 396412CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Landauer, Thomas K., and Dumais, Susan T. 1997. ‘A solution to Plato’s problem: The latent semantic analysis theory of acquisition, induction, and representation of knowledge’, Psychological Review 104.2: 211–40.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lang, Jürgen, and Neumann-Holzschuh, Ingrid (eds.) 1999. Reanalyse und Grammatikalisierung in den Romanischen Sprachen. Tübingen: Max Niemeyer VerlagCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langacker, Ronald W. 1977. ‘Syntactic reanalysis’, in Li, (ed.), pp. 57139CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langacker, Ronald W. 1987. Foundations of cognitive grammar, vol. 1: Theoretical prerequisites. Stanford, CA: Stanford University PressGoogle Scholar
Langacker, Ronald W. 1991. Foundations of cognitive grammar, vol. 2: Descriptive application. Stanford, CA: Stanford University PressGoogle Scholar
Langacker, Ronald W. 2000. ‘A dynamic usage-based model’, in Barlow, and Kemmer, (eds.), pp. 163Google Scholar
Langacker, Ronald W. 2008. Cognitive grammar: A basic introduction. Oxford: Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langer, Jonas 2000. ‘The descent of cognitive development’, Developmental Science 3(4): 361–78CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Larsen-Freeman, Diane 1997. ‘Chaos/complexity science and second language acquisition’, Applied Linguistics 18(2): 141–65CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lass, Roger (ed.) 1999. The Cambridge history of the English language, vol. 3: 1476–1776. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Laxon, Veronica, Masterson, Jacqueline, and Coltheart, Veronika 1991. ‘Some bodies are easier to read: The effect of consistency and regularity on children’s reading’, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental Psychology 43(4): 793824CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Łęcki, Andrzej M. 2010. Grammaticalisation paths of have in English. Bern: Peter LangCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leech, Geoffrey 1981. Semantics: The study of meaning. 2nd edition. Harmondsworth: PenguinGoogle Scholar
Leech, Geoffrey, Hundt, Marianne, Mair, Christian, and Smith, Nicholas 2009. Change in contemporary English: A grammatical study. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leech, Geoffrey, Rayson, Paul, and Wilson, Andrew 2001. Word frequencies in written and spoken English: Based on the British National Corpus. London: LongmanGoogle Scholar
Legate, Julie A., and Yang, Charles 2007. ‘Morphosyntactic learning and the development of tense’, Language Acquisition 14(3): 315–44CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lehmann, Christian 2002. Thoughts on grammaticalization. 2nd edition. Seminar für Sprachwissenschaft der Universität Erfurt (ASSidUE, 9) [under Schriftenverzeichnis, 2002] www.christianlehmann.eu/ [accessed April 19, 2016]Google Scholar
Lehmann, Winfred P., and Malkiel, Yakov (eds.) 1968. Directions for historical linguistics: A symposium. Austin: University of Texas PressGoogle Scholar
Lehmann, Winfred P., and Malkiel, Yakov (eds.) 1982. Perspectives on historical linguistics. Amsterdam: BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lenker, Ursula 2010. Argument and rhetoric. Adverbial connectors in the history of English. Berlin and New Work: Mouton de GruyterCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levelt, Willem J. M., and Kelter, Stephanie 1982. ‘Surface form and memory in question answering’, Cognitive Psychology 14(1): 78106CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levelt, Willem J. M., Roelofs, Ardi, and Meyer, Antje S. 1999. ‘A theory of lexical access in speech production’, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22(1): 138CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levinson, Stephen C. 1995. ‘Three levels of meaning’, in Palmer, (ed.), pp. 90115CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Li, Charles N. (ed.) 1977. Mechanisms of syntactic change. Austin, TX and London: University of Texas PressGoogle Scholar
Liberman, Mark 2010. ‘“Begging the question”: We have answers’. http://languagelog.ldc.upenn.edu/nll/?p=2290 [accessed April 19, 2016]Google Scholar
Lieberman, Erez, Michel, Jean-Baptiste, Jackson, Joe, Tang, Tina, and Nowak, Martin A. 2007. ‘Quantifying the evolutionary dynamics of language’, Nature 449(7163): 713–16CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieven, Elena V. M. 2008. ‘Learning the English auxiliary: A usage-based approach’, in Behrens, (ed.), pp. 6198CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieven, Elena V. M. 2010. ‘Input and first language acquisition: Evaluating the role of frequency’, Lingua 120(11): 2546–56CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieven, Elena V. M., Behrens, Heike, Speares, Jennifer, and Tomasello, Michael 2003. ‘Early syntactic creativity: A usage-based approach’, Journal of Child Language 30(1): 333–70CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lieven, Elena V. M., Pine, Julian M., and Dresner Barnes, Helen 1992. ‘Individual differences in early vocabulary development: Redefining the referential expressive dimension’, Journal of Child Language 19(2): 287310CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lieven, Elena V. M., and Tomasello, Michael 2008. ‘Children’s first language acquisition from a usage-based perspective’, in Robinson, and Ellis, (eds.), pp. 168–96Google Scholar
Lightbown, Patsy M., and Spada, Nina 1999. How languages are learned. Oxford: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
Lightfoot, David 1979. Principles of diachronic syntax. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Lightfoot, David 1991. How to set parameters: Arguments from language change. Cambridge, MA: The MIT PressGoogle Scholar
Lightfoot, David 1997. ‘Catastrophic change and learning theory’, Lingua 100(1): 171–92CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lightfoot, David 1999. The development of language: Acquisition, change, and evolution. Malden, MA and Oxford: BlackwellGoogle Scholar
Lightfoot, David 2006. How new languages emerge. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lightfoot, David 2010. ‘Language acquisition and language change’, Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science 1(5): 677–84Google ScholarPubMed
Lima, Susan D., Corrigan, Roberta L., and Iverson, Gregory K. (eds.) 1994. The reality of linguistic rules. Amsterdam: John BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindquist, Hans, and Mair, Christian (eds.) 2004. Corpus approaches to grammaticalization in English. Amsterdam: BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lipka, Leonhard 1985. ‘Inferential features in historical semantics’, in Fisiak, (ed.), pp. 339–54CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lively, Scott E., Pisoni, David B., and Goldinger, Stephen D. 1994. ‘Spoken word recognition: Research and theory’, in Gernsbacher, (ed.), pp. 265301Google Scholar
Loebell, Helga, and Bock, J. Kathryn 2003. ‘Structural priming across languages’, Linguistics 41(5): 791824CrossRefGoogle Scholar
López-Couso, María José 2007. ‘Frequency effects: Middle English nis as a case in point’, in Mazzon, (ed.), pp. 165–78Google Scholar
López-Couso, María José 2011. ‘Developmental parallels in diachronic and ontogenetic grammaticalization: Existential there as a test case’, Folia Linguistica 45(1): 81102CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lorenz, David 2013. Contractions of English semi-modals: The emancipating effect of frequency. Freiburg: RombachGoogle Scholar
Los, Bettelou 2005. The rise of the to-infinitive. Oxford: Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loudermilk, Brandon C. 2013. ‘Psycholinguistic approaches’, in Bayley, , Cameron, and Lucas, (eds.), pp. 132–52CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luce, Paul A. 1986. ‘A computational analysis of uniqueness points in auditory word recognition’, Perception and Psychophysics 39(3): 155–58CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lüdeling, Anke and Evert, Stefan 2005. ‘The emergence of non-medical -itis. Corpus evidence and qualitative analysis’, in Kepser, and Reis, (eds.), pp. 315–33CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luka, Barbara J., and Barsalou, Lawrence W. 2005. ‘Structural facilitation: Mere exposure effects for grammatical acceptability as evidence for syntactic priming in comprehension’, Journal of Memory and Language 52(3): 436–59CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Luka, Barbara J., and Choi, Heidi 2012. ‘Dynamic grammar in adults: Incidental learning of natural syntactic structures extends over 48 h’, Journal of Memory and Language 66(2): 345–60CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lyons, John 1995. Linguistic semantics: An introduction. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacDonald, Maryellen C. 1994. ‘Probabilistic constraints and syntactic ambiguity resolution’, Language and Cognitive Processes 9(2): 157201CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacDonald, Maryellen C., Pearlmutter, Neal J., and Seidenberg, Mark S. 1994. ‘The lexical nature of syntactic ambiguity resolution’, Psychological Review 101(4): 676703CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacDonald, Maryellen C., and Seidenberg, Mark S. 2006. ‘Constraint satisfaction accounts of lexical and sentence comprehension’, in Traxler, and Gernsbacher, (eds.), pp. 581611CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mackintosh, Nicholas J. 1975. ‘A theory of attention: Variations in the associability of stimuli with reinforcement’, Psychological Review 82(4): 276–98CrossRefGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, Brian 1987a. ‘The competition model’, in MacWhinney, (ed.), pp. 249308Google Scholar
MacWhinney, Brian (ed.) 1987b. Mechanisms of language acquisition. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence ErlbaumGoogle Scholar
MacWhinney, Brian 2000. The CHILDES-Project: Tools for analyzing talk. 2 vols. 3rd edition. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
MacWhinney, Brian 2004. ‘New directions in the competition model’, in Tomasello, and Slobin, (eds.), pp. 81110Google Scholar
MacWhinney, Brian 2012. ‘The logic of the unified model’, in Gass, and Mackey, (eds.), pp. 211–27Google Scholar
MacWhinney, Brian, and O’Grady, William (eds.) 2015. The handbook of language emergence. Oxford: Wiley-BlackwellCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mair, Christian 2004. ‘Corpus linguistics and grammaticalisation theory: Statistics, frequencies, and beyond’, in Lindquist, and Mair, (eds.), pp. 121–50CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mair, Christian 2012. ‘From opportunistic to systematic use of the web as corpus: Do-Support with got (to) in contemporary American English’, in Nevalainen, and Traugott, (eds.), pp. 245–55CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mair, Christian 2014. ‘Do we got a difference? – Divergent developments of semi-auxiliary (have) got (to) in BrE and AmE’, in Hundt, (ed.), pp. 5676CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Majid, Asifa, Bowerman, Melissa, Kita, Sotaro, Haun, Daniel B. M., and Levinson, Stephen C. 2004. ‘Can language restructure cognition? The case for space’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences 8(3): 108–14CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malchukov, Andrej, and Siewierska, Anna (eds.) 2011. Impersonal constructions. A cross-linguistic perspective. Amsterdam: BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Manabe, Kazumi 1989. The syntactic and stylistic development of the infinitive in Middle English. Fukuoka: Kyushu University PressGoogle Scholar
Mańczak, Witold 1958. ‘Tendences générales des changements analogiques’, Lingua 7: 298325CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mandler, Jean M. 2008. ‘On the birth and growth of concepts’, Philosophical Psychology 21(2): 207–30CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marcus, Gary F., Brinkmann, Ursula, Clahsen, Harald, Wiese, Richard, and Pinker, Steven 1995. ‘German inflection: The exception that proves the rule’, Cognitive Psychology 29(3): 189256CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcus, Gary F., Pinker, Steven, Ullman, Michael, Hollander, Michelle, Rosen, T. John, Xu, Fei, and Clahsen, Harald 1992. ‘Overregularization in language acquisition’, Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 57(4): 1178CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marcus, Gary F., Vijayan, S., Bandi Rao, Shoba, and Vishton, Peter M. 1999. ‘Rule learning by seven-month-old infants’, Science 283(5398): 7780CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Markman, Arthur B., and Gentner, Dedre 1993. ‘Structural alignment during similarity comparisons’, Cognitive Psychology 25(4): 431–67CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marslen-Wilson, William 1973. ‘Linguistic structure and speech shadowing at very short latencies’, Nature 244: 522–33CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marslen-Wilson, William 1990. ‘Activation, competition, and frequency in lexical access’, in Altmann, (ed.), pp. 148–72CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marsolek, Chad J. 2008. ‘What antipriming reveals about priming’, Trends in Cognitive Science 12(5): 176–81CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Maslen, Robert J. C., Theakston, Anna L., Lieven, Elena V. M., and Tomasello, Michael 2004. ‘A dense corpus study of past tense and plural overregularization in English’, Journal of Speech, Language and Hearing Research 47(6): 1319–33CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matthews, Peter H. 2014. The positions of adjectives in English. Oxford: Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Matthey, Marinette 2001. ‘Le changement linguistique: Évolution, variation, hétérogénéité. Actes du colloque de Neuchâtel Université, Neuchâtel, Suisse 2–4 Octobre 2000 [Linguistic change: Evolution, variation, heterogeneity. Proceedings of the University of Neuchâtel colloquium, Neuchâtel, Switzerland, October 2–4, 2000]’, Travaux Neuchâtelois de Linguistique 34: 273303Google Scholar
Mayerthaler, Willi 1981. Morphologische Natürlichkeit. Wiesbaden: AthenaionGoogle Scholar
Mazzon, Gabriella (ed.) 2007. Studies in Middle English forms and meanings. Frankfurt am Main, etc.: Peter LangGoogle Scholar
Menn, Lise, and Stoel-Gammon, Carol 1994. ‘Phonological development’, in Fletcher, and MacWhinney, (eds.), pp. 335–59CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCauley, Stewart M., and Christiansen, Morten H. 2014. ‘Acquiring formulaic language: A computational model’, Mental Lexicon 9(3): 419–36CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClelland, James L., and Elman, Jeffrey L. 1986. ‘The TRACE model of speech perception’, Cognitive Psychology 18(1): 186CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCullough, Gretchen 2012. ‘Because reasons’, All Things Linguistic, July 4, 2012. http://allthingslinguistic.com/post/26522214342/because-reasons [accessed December 27, 2014]Google Scholar
McCullough, Gretchen 2013. ‘Where “because noun” probably came from’, All Things Linguistic, 19 November 2013. http://allthingslinguistic.com/post/67507311833/where-because-noun-probably-came-from [accessed December 27, 2014]Google Scholar
McDaniel, Dana, McKee, Cecile, Cowart, Wayne, and Garrett, Merrill F. 2015. ‘The role of the language production system in shaping grammars’, Language 91(2): 415–41CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McDonald, Scott A., and Shillcock, Richard C. 2003a. ‘Eye-movements reveal the on-line computation of lexical probabilities during reading’, Psychological Science 14(6): 648–52CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDonald, Scott A., and Shillcock, Richard C. 2003b. ‘Low-level predictive inference in reading: The influence of transitional probabilities on eye movements’, Vision Research 43(16): 1735–51CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McDonough, Kim, and Trofimovich, Pavel 2008. Using priming methods in second language research. London: RoutledgeGoogle Scholar
McRae, Ken, and Matsuki, Kazunaga 2013. ‘Constraint-based models of sentence processing’, in van Gompel, (ed.), pp. 5177Google Scholar
McRae, Ken, Spivey-Knowlton, Michael J., and Tanenhaus, Michael K. 1998. ‘Modeling the influence of thematic fit (and other constraints) in on-line sentence comprehension’, Journal of Memory and Language 38(3): 283312CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McWhorter, John 2001. ‘The world’s simplest grammars are creole grammars’, Language Typology 5(2–3): 125–66Google Scholar
McWhorter, John 2002. The power of Babel: A natural history of language. San Franciso, CA: W. H. Freeman and Co.Google Scholar
McWhorter, John 2004. The story of human language. Lecture 24: Language interrupted. The Great Courses. The Teaching Company Limited PartnershipGoogle Scholar
Meibauer, Jörg, Guttropf, Anja, and Scherer, Carmen 2004. ‘Dynamic aspects of German -er-nominals: A probe into the interrelation of language change and language acquisition’, Linguistics 42(1): 155–93CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meillet, Antoine (ed.) 1951/1958. Linguistique historique et linguistique générale. 2 vols. Paris: Klincksieck/ChampionGoogle Scholar
Meillet, Antoine 1958. ‘L’évolution des formes grammaticales’, in Meillet, (ed.), pp. 130–48 [First published 1912 in Scientia [Rivista di Scienza]: 12]Google Scholar
Meisel, Jürgen 2011. ‘Bilingual language acquisition and theories of diachronic change: Bilingualism as cause and effect of grammatical change’, Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 14(2): 121–45CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meisel, Jürgen, Elsig, Martin, and Rinke, Esther 2013. Language acquisition and change: A morphosyntactic perspective. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University PressGoogle Scholar
Meurman-Solin, Anneli, López-Couso, María José, and Los, Bettelou (eds.) 2012. Information structure and syntactic change in the history of English. Oxford: Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meyerhoff, Miriam 1992. ‘“A sort of something” – Hedging strategies on nouns’, Working Papers on Language, Gender and Sexism 2(1): 5973Google Scholar
Meylan, Stephan C., and Gahl, Susanne 2014. ‘The divergent lexicon: Lexical overlap decreases with age in a large corpus of conversational speech’, in Bello, , Guarini, , McShane, and Scassellati, (eds.), pp. 1006–11Google Scholar
Michel, Jean-Baptiste, Shen, Yuan Kui, Presser Aiden, Aviva, Veres, Adrian, Gray, Matthew K., The Google Books Team, Pickett, Joseph P., Hoiberg, Dale, Clancy, Dan, Norvig, Peter, Orwant, Jon, Pinker, Steven, Nowak, Martin A., and Lieberman, Aiden, Erez 2011. ‘Quantitative analysis of culture using millions of digitized books’, Science 331(6014): 176–82CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milin, Petar, Feldman, Laurie B., Ramscar, Michael, Hendrix, P., Baayen, R. Harald (2017). ‘Discrimination in lexical decision.’ PLoS ONE, 12(2), e0171935CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, George A. 1956. ‘The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information’, Psychological Review 63(2): 8197CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, George A. 1958. ‘Free recall of redundant strings of letters’, Journal of Experimental Psychology 56(6): 485CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, George A. 1990. ‘Linguists, psychologists and the cognitive sciences’, Language 66(2): 317–22CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, George A., Bruner, Jerome S., and Postman, Leo 1954. ‘Familiarity of letter sequences and tachistoscopic identification’, Journal of General Psychology 50(1): 129–39CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, George A., and Selfridge, Jennifer A. 1950. ‘Verbal context and the recall of meaningful material’, The American Journal of Psychology 63(2): 176–85CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Milroy, James 1992. Linguistic variation and change: On the historical sociolinguistics of English. Oxford: BlackwellGoogle Scholar
Mintz, Toben H. 2003. ‘Frequent frames as a cue for grammatical categories in child directed speech’, Cognition 90(1): 91117CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mitchell, Tom M., Shinkareva, Svetlana V., Carlson, Andrew, Chang, Kai-Min, Malave, Vicente L., Mason, Robert A., and Just, Marcel A. 2008. ‘Predicting human brain activity associated with the meanings of nouns’, Science 320(5880): 1191–95CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Molnar, Monika, and Sabastián-Galles, Núria (eds.) 2014. The roots of language learning: Infant language acquisition. Hoboken: WileyGoogle Scholar
Montermini, Fabio, Boyé, Gilles, and Hathout, Nabil 2007. Selected proceedings of the 5th Décembrettes: Morphology in Toulouse. Somerville, MA: Cascadilla Proceedings ProjectGoogle Scholar
Montgomery, Chris 2012. ‘Perceptions of dialects: Changing attitudes and ideologies’, in Nevalainen, and Traugott, (eds.), pp. 457–69CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morton, John 1969. ‘Interaction of information in word recognition’, Psychological Review 76(2): 165–78CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mufwene, Salikoko S. 2001. The ecology of language evolution. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mufwene, Salikoko S. 2008. Language evolution: Contact, competition and change. London: Continuum International Publishing GroupCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mugdan, Joachim 1977. Flexionsmorphologie und Psycholinguistik. Tübingen: NarrGoogle Scholar
Müller, Max 1890. The science of language. New York: Charles ScribnerGoogle Scholar
Naigles, Letitia R., Hoff, Erika, Vear, Donna, Tomasello, Michael, Brandt, Silke, Waxman, Sandra R., Childers, Jane B., and Collins, W. Andrew 2009. ‘Flexibility in early verb use: Evidence from a multiple-N diary study’, Monographs for the Society for Research in Child Development 74(2): 1144Google ScholarPubMed
Narrog, Heiko 2005. ‘Modality, mood, and change of modal meanings: A new perspective’, Cognitive Linguistics 16(4): 677731CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Narrog, Heiko, and Heine, Bernd (eds.) 2011. The Oxford handbook of grammaticalization. Oxford: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
Nesselhauf, Nadja 2010. ‘The development of future time expressions in Late Modern English: Redistribution of forms or change in discourse?’, English Language and Linguistics 14(2): 163–86CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nevala, Minna 2016. ‘Processes of sociolinguistic and sociopragmatic change’, in Kytö, and Pahta, (eds.), pp. 286300CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nevalainen, Terttu, and Raumolin-Brunberg, Helena 2003. Historical sociolinguistics. London: LongmanGoogle Scholar
Nevalainen, Terttu, and Traugott, Elizabeth C. (eds.) 2012. The Oxford handbook of the history of English. Oxford: Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newell, Allen 1990. Unified theories of cognition. Cambridge, MA: The MIT PressGoogle Scholar
Ninio, Anat 2006. Language and the learning curve. A new theory of syntactic development. Oxford: Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ninio, Anat 2011. Syntactic development, its input and output. Oxford: Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nixon, Graham, and Honey, John (eds.) 1988. An historic tongue: Studies in English linguistics in memory of Barbara Strang. London and New York: RoutledgeGoogle Scholar
Noël, Dirk 2012. ‘Grammaticalization in diachronic construction grammar’. Lecture held at the Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Norte, Natal, Brasil, November 27, 2012Google Scholar
Nowak, Martin A., Komaraova, Natalia L., and Niyogi, Partha 2002. ‘Computational and evolutionary aspects of language’, Nature 417(6889): 611–17CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nunberg, Geoffrey, Sag, Ivan A., and Wasow, Thomas 1994. ‘Idioms’, Language 70(3): 491538CrossRefGoogle Scholar
O’Donnell, Matthew B., Römer, Ute, and Ellis, Nick C. 2013. ‘The development of formulaic sequences in first and second language writing: Investigating effects of frequency, association, and native norm’, International Journal of Corpus Linguistics 18(1): 83108CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ohala, John J., Hasegawa, Yoko, Ohala, Manjari, Granville, Daniel, and Bailey, Ashlee C. (eds.) 1999. 14th International Congress of Phonetic Sciences (ICPhS-14), San Francisco, CA, USA, August 1–7, 1999Google Scholar
Oldfield, Richard C., and Wingfield, Arthur 1965. ‘Response latencies in naming objects’, Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 17(4): 273–81CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Osgood, Charles E. 1957. The measurement of meaning. Illinois: University of Illinois PressGoogle Scholar
Pagliuca, William (ed.) 1994. Perspectives on grammaticalization. Amsterdam: BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paivio, Allan 1971. Imagery and verbal processes. New York: Holt, Rinehart and WinstonGoogle Scholar
Paivio, Allan 1986. Mental representations: A dual coding approach. Oxford: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
Paivio, Allan, Yuille, John C., and Madigan, Stephen A. 1968. ‘Concreteness, imagery, and meaningfulness values for 925 nouns’, Journal of Experimental Psychology 76(1): Suppl.: 125CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Palmer, Frank R. (ed.) 1995. Grammar and meaning. Essays in honour of Sir John Lyons. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Palmore, Erdman B., and Manton, Kenneth 1973. ‘Ageism compared to racism and sexism’, Journal of Gerontology 28(3): 363–69CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Papadopoulou, Despina, and Clahsen, Harald 2003. ‘Parsing strategies in L1 and L2 sentence processing: A study of relative clause attachment in Greek’, Studies in Second Language Acquisition 25: 501–28CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Papafragou, Anna 2001. ‘Linking early linguistic and conceptual capacities: The role of Theory of Mind’, in Cienki, , Luka, and Smith, (eds.), pp. 169–84Google Scholar
Parker, Frank 1976. ‘Language change and the passive voice’, Language 52(4): 449–60CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Partington, Alan 2011. ‘Phrasal irony: Its form, function and exploitation’, Journal of Pragmatics 43(6): 1786–800CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paul, Hermann 1880/1960. Prinzipien der Sprachgeschichte. 6th edition. Halle: NiemeyerGoogle Scholar
Payne, Doris L. 2011. ‘The Maa (Eastern Nilotic) impersonal construction’, in Malchukov, and Siewierska, (eds.), pp. 257–84CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perdue, Clive (ed.) 1993. Adult language acquisition: Crosslinguistic perspectives. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Pérez, Aveline 1990. ‘Time in motion: Grammaticalization of the be going to construction in English’, La Trobe Working Papers in Linguistics 3: 4964Google Scholar
Perruchet, Pierre, and Pacton, Sebastien 2006. ‘Implicit learning and statistical learning: One phenomenon, two approaches’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences 10(5): 233–38CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Peter, Michelle, Chang, Franklin, Pine, Julian M., Blything, Ryan, and Rowland, Caroline F. 2015. ‘When and how do children develop knowledge of verb argument structure? Evidence from verb bias effects in a structural priming task’, Journal of Memory and Language 81: 115CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Peters, Ann M. 1983. The units of language acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Peterson, Robert R., Burgess, Curt, Dell, Gary S., and Eberhard, Kathleen M. 2001. ‘Disassociation between syntactic and semantic processing during idiom comprehension’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 27(5): 1223–37Google Scholar
Petré, Peter 2009. ‘Leuven English Old to New (LEON): Some ideas on a new corpus for longitudinal diachronic studies’. Paper presented at the Middle and Modern English Corpus Linguistics (MMECL) Conference, University of Innsbruck, July 5–9, 2009Google Scholar
Petré, Peter, and Cuyckens, H. 2008. ‘Bedusted, yet not beheaded: The role of be-’s constructional properties in its conservation’, in Bergs, and Diewald, (eds.), pp. 133–69CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pexman, Penny M., Siakaluk, Paul D., and Yap, Melvin J. 2013. ‘Introduction to the research topic meaning in mind: Semantic richness effects in language processing’, Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7: 723CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pexman, Penny M., Siakaluk, Paul D., and Yap, Melvin J. 2014. Meaning in mind: Semantic richness effects in language processing. Frontiers E-BooksCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phillips, Colin 2013. ‘Some arguments and non-arguments for reductionist accounts of syntactic phenomena’, Language and Cognitive Processes 28(1–2): 156–87CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, Jean 1969. ‘Genetic epistemology’, Columbia Forum 12: 411Google Scholar
Piantadosi, Steven T., Tily, Harry, and Gibson, Edward 2011. ‘Word lengths are optimized for efficient communication’. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences 108(9): 3526–9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piantadosi, Steven T., Tily, Harry, and Gibson, Edward 2012. ‘The communicative function of ambiguity in language’, Cognition 122(3): 280–91CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pica, Teresa 1983. ‘Adult acquisition of English as a second language under different conditions of exposure’, Language Learning 33(4): 465–97CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pickering, Martin J. 2006. ‘The dance of dialogue’, The Psychologist 19(12): 734–37Google Scholar
Pickering, Martin J., and Branigan, Holly P. 1998. ‘The representation of verbs: Evidence from syntactic priming in language production’, Journal of Memory and Language 39(4): 633–51CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pickering, Martin J., and Ferreira, Victor S. 2008. ‘Structural priming: A critical review’, Psychological Bulletin 134(3): 427–59CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pickering, Martin J., and Garrod, Simon C. 2004. ‘Toward a mechanistic psychology of dialogue’, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 27(2): 169225CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pickering, Martin J., and Garrod, Simon C. 2006. ‘Alignment as the basis for successful communication’, Research on Language and Computation 4(2–3): 203–28CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pickering, Martin J., and Garrod, Simon C. 2013. ‘An integrated theory of language production and comprehension’, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 36(4): 329–47CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pickering, Martin J., McElree, Brian, Frisson, Steven, Chen, Lillian, and Traxler, Matthew 2006. ‘Underspecification and aspectual coercion’, Discourse Processes 42(2): 131–55CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pickering, Martin J., and Traxler, Matthew 1998. ‘Plausibility and recovery from garden paths: An eye-tracking study’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition 24(4): 940–61Google Scholar
Pierrehumbert, Janet B. 2001. ‘Exemplar dynamics: Word frequency, lenition, and contrast’, in Bybee, and Hopper, (eds.), pp. 137–57CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pierrehumbert, Janet B. 2012. ‘Burstiness of verbs and derived nouns’, in Santos, , Linden, and Ng’ang’a, (eds.), pp. 99116CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pinker, Steven 1989. Learnability and cognition: The Acquisition of argument structure. Cambridge: The MIT PressGoogle Scholar
Pinker, Steven 1999. Words and rules: The ingredients of language. New York: Basic BooksGoogle Scholar
Pinker, Steven, and Prince, Alan 1988. ‘On language and connectionism: Analysis of a parallel distributed processing model of language acquisition’, Cognition 28(1): 59102CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pinker, Steven, and Ullman, Michael T. 2002. ‘The past and future of the past tense’, Trends in Cognitive Sciences 6(11): 456–63CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pintzuk, Susan, Tsoulas, George, and Warner, Anthony (eds.) 2001. Diachronic syntax: Models and mechanisms. Oxford: Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Plag, Ingo, Homann, Julia, and Kunter, Gero 2014. ‘Homophony and morphology: The acoustics of word-final s in English’. Manuscript. University of Düsseldorf.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Podesva, Robert J., and Sharma, Devyani (eds.) 2014. Research methods in linguistics. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Polikarpov, Anatoliy A. 2006. Cognitive model of lexical system evolution and its verification. www.philol.msu.ru/~lex/articles/cogn_ev.htm [accessed April 19, 2016]Google Scholar
Preston, Dennis R. 2004. ‘Three kinds of sociolinguistics: A psycholinguistic perspective’, in Fought, (ed.), pp. 140–58CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pritchett, Bradley 1992. Grammatical competence and parsing performance. Chicago: University of Chicago PressGoogle Scholar
Pullum, Geoffrey K. 1997. ‘The morpholexical nature of English to-contraction’, Language 73: 79102CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pulvermüller, Friedemann 1999. ‘Words in the brain’s language’, Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22: 253336CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pye, Clifton 1980. ‘The acquisition of person markers in Quich Mayan’, Papers and Reports on Child Language Development 19: 53–9Google Scholar
Quirk, Randolph 1965. ‘Descriptive statement and serial relationship’, Language 41(2): 205–17CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Quirk, Randolph, Greenbaum, Sidney, Leech, Geoffrey, and Svartvik, Jan 1985. A comprehensive grammar of the English language. London and New York: LongmanGoogle Scholar
Rács, Péter 2013. Salience in sociolinguistics: A quantitative approach. Berlin and New York: Mouton de GruyterCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Radford, Andrew, Felser, Claudia, and Boxell, Oliver 2012. ‘Preposition copying and pruning in present-day English’, English Language and Linguistics 16(3): 403–26CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramachandran, Vilayanur S. (ed.) 2012. Encyclopedia of human behavior. Oxford: ElsevierGoogle Scholar
Ramat, Paolo, and Ricca, Davide 1994. ‘Prototypical adverbs: On the scalarity/radiality of the notion ADVERB’, Rivista di Linguistica 6.2: 289326.Google Scholar
Ramscar, Michael, and Baayen, R. Harald 2013. ‘Production, comprehension, and synthesis: A communicative perspective on language’, Frontiers in Psychology 4: 233CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramscar, Michael, Dye, Melody, and McCauley, Stewart M. 2013a. ‘Error and expectation in language learning: The curious absence of mouses in adult speech’, Language 89(4): 760–93CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramscar, Michael, Hendrix, Peter, Love, Bradley, and Baayen, R. Harald 2013b. ‘Learning is not decline: The mental lexicon as a window into cognition across the lifespan’, The Mental Lexicon 8: 450–81CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramscar, Michael, Hendrix, Peter, Shaoul, Cyrus, Milin, Petar, and Baayen, R. Harald 2014. ‘The myth of cognitive decline: Non-linear dynamics of lifelong learning’, Topics in Cognitive Science 6(1): 542CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ramscar, Michael, Smith, Asha Halima, Dye, Melody, Futrell, Richard, Hendrix, Peter, Baayen, R. Harald, and Starr, Rebecca 2013c. ‘The “universal” structure of name grammars and the impact of social engineering on the evolution of natural information systems’. Proceedings of the 35th Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, Berlin, Germany, July 31–August 3, 2013.Google Scholar
Ramscar, Michael, Yarlett, Daniel, Dye, Melody, Denny, Katie, and Thorpe, Kirsten 2010. ‘The effects of feature-label order and their implications for symbolic learning’, Cognitive Science 34(6): 909–57CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rasmussen, Nicolas 1991. ‘The decline of recapitulationism in early twentieth-century biology: Disciplinary conflict and consensus on the battleground of theory’, Journal of the History of Biology 24(1): 5189Google Scholar
Ravid, Dorit 1995. Language change in child and adult Hebrew: A psycholinguistic perspective. Oxford: Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ravid, Dorit, Dressler, Wolfgang U., Nir-Sagiv, Bracha, Korecky-Kröll, Katharina, Souman, Agnita, Rehfeldt, Katja, Laaha, Sabine, Bertl, Johannes, Basbøll, Hans, and Gillis, Steven 2008. ‘Core morphology in child directed speech: Crosslinguistic corpus analyses of noun plurals’, in Behrens, (ed.), pp. 2560CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reali, Florencia, and Christiansen, Morten H. 2007. ‘Processing of relative clauses is made easier by frequency of occurrence’, Journal of Memory and Language 57(1): 123CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reber, Arthur S. 1993. Implicit learning and tacit knowledge: An essay on the cognitive unconscious. Oxford: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
Reber, Arthur S., Kassin, Saul M., Lewis, Selma, and Cantor, Gary W. 1980. ‘On the relationship between implicit and explicit modes in the learning of a complex rule structure’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 6(5): 492502Google Scholar
Reber, Paul J., and Squire, Larry R. 1998. ‘Encapsulation of implicit and explicit memory in sequence learning’, Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 10(2): 248–63CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rebuschat, Patrick (ed.) 2015. Implicit and explicit learning of language. Amsterdam: John BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rebuschat, Patrick, and Williams, John N. (eds.) 2012. Statistical learning and language acquisition. Berlin: Mouton de GruyterGoogle Scholar
Redington, Martin, and Chater, Nick 1998. ‘Connectionist and statistical approaches to language acquisition: A distributional perspective’, Language and Cognitive Processes 13(2–3): 129–92CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reitter, David, and Moore, Johanna D. 2014. ‘Alignment and task success in spoken dialogue’, Journal of Memory and Language 76: 2946CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, Robert A. 1968. ‘Probability of shock in the presence and absence of CS in fear conditioning’, Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology 66(1): 15CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rescorla, Robert A. 1988. ‘Pavlovian conditioning. It’s not what you think it is’, American Psychologist 43(3): 151–60CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rescorla, Robert A., and Wagner, Allan R. 1972. ‘A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement’, in Black, and Prokasy, (eds.), pp. 6499Google Scholar
Richards, Brian J. 1990. Language development and individual differences: A study of auxiliary verb learning. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rissanen, Matti 1999. ‘Syntax’, in Lass, (ed.), pp. 187331CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robenalt, Clarice, and Goldberg, Adele E. 2015. ‘Judgment evidence for statistical preemption: It is relatively better to vanish than to disappear a rabbit, but a lifeguard can equally well backstroke or swim children to shore’, Cognitive Linguistics 26(3): 467503CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, Ian 1993. Verbs and diachronic syntax: A comparative history of English and French. Dordrecht: KluwerGoogle Scholar
Roberts, Ian 2015. ‘Formal and functional explanations: New perspective on an old debate’. Paper presented at the 48th Annual Meeting of the Societas Linguistica Europaea, Leiden, September 2–5Google Scholar
Roberts, Ian, and Roussou, Anna 2003. Syntactic change: A minimalist approach to grammaticalization. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roberts, Leah, and Felser, Claudia 2011. ‘Plausibility and recovery from garden-paths in second-language sentence processing’, Applied Psycholinguistics 32(2): 299331CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, Peter (ed.) 2001. Cognition and second language instruction. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robinson, Peter, and Ellis, Nick C. (eds.) 2008. Handbook of cognitive linguistics and second language acquisition. New York and London: RoutledgeCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rohdenburg, Günter, and Schlüter, Julia (eds.) 2009. One language, two grammars? Differences between British and American English. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Romaine, Suzanne (ed.) 1998. The Cambridge history of the English language, vol. 4: 1776 to the present day. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Rosemeyer, Malte 2016. ‘Modeling frequency effects in language change’, in Behrens, and Pfänder, (eds.), pp. 179207CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rowland, Caroline F. 2007. ‘Explaining errors in children’s questions’, Cognition 104(1): 106–34CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowland, Caroline F., Chang, Franklin, Ambridge, Ben, Pine, Julian M., and Lieven, Elena V. M. 2012. ‘The development of abstract syntax: Evidence from structural priming and the lexical boost’, Cognition 125(1): 4963CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowland, Caroline F., and Pine, Julian M. 2000. ‘Subject-auxiliary inversion errors and wh-question acquisition: What children do know?’, Journal of Child Language 27(1): 157–81CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowland, Caroline F., Pine, Julian M., Lieven, Elena V. M., and Theakston, Anna L. 2003. ‘Determinants of the order of acquisition of wh-questions: Re-evaluating the role of caregiver speech’, Journal of Child Language 30(3): 609–35CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rumelhart, David E., Hinton, Geoffrey E., and Williams, Ronald J. 1986. ‘Learning representations by back-propagating errors’, Nature Reviews Neuroscience 323(9): 533–36Google Scholar
Rumelhart, David E., and McClelland, James L. (eds.) 1986. Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition. Cambridge, MA: The MIT PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saffran, Eleanor M., and Martin, Nadine 1997. ‘Effects of structural priming on sentence production in aphasics’, Language and Cognitive Processes 12(5–6): 877–82CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saffran, Jenny R., Aslin, Richard N., and Newport, Elissa L. 1996. ‘Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants’, Science 274(5294): 1926–28CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saffran, Jenny R., Johnson, Elizabeth K., Aslin, Richard N., and Newport, Elissa L. 1999. ‘Statistical learning of tone sequences by human infants and adults’, Cognition 70(1): 2752CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Säily, Tanja 2014. Sociolinguistic variation in English derivational productivity: Studies and methods in diachronic corpus linguistics. Helsinki: Société NéophilologiqueGoogle Scholar
Sampson, Geoffrey 1997. Educating Eve: The ‘Language Instinct’ debate. London: ContinuumGoogle Scholar
Santelmann, Lynn, Berk, Stephanie, Austin, Jennifer, Somashekar, Shamitha, and Lust, Barbara 2002. ‘Continuity and development in the acquisition of inversion in yes/no questions: Dissociating movement and inflection’, Journal of Child Language 29(4): 813–42CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Santos, Diana, Linden, Krister, and Ng’ang’a, Wanjiju (eds.) 2012. Shall we play the Festschrift game? Essays on the occasion of Lauri Carlson’s 60th birthday. Berlin: SpringerCrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Saussure, Ferdinand 1916/1983. Cours de linguistique générale [Course in general linguistics]. Translated and annotated by Roy Harris. London: DuckworthGoogle Scholar
Savage, Ceri, Lieven, Elena V. M., Theakston, Anna, and Tomasello, Michael 2003. ‘Testing the abstractness of children’s linguistic representation: Lexical and structural priming of syntactic constructions in young children’, Developmental Science 6(5): 557–67CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savage, Ceri, Lieven, Elena V. M., Theakston, Anna, and Tomasello, Michael 2006. ‘Structural priming as implicit learning in language acquisition: The persistence of lexical and structural priming in 4-year-olds’, Language Learning and Development 2(1): 2750CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savin, Harris B. 1963. ‘Word-frequency effects and errors in the perception of speech’, Journal of the Acoustic Society of America 35(2): 200–6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scalise, Sergio, and Vogel, Irene (eds.) 2010. Cross-disciplinary issues in compounding. Amsterdam: John BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schaner-Wolles, Chris, Rennison, John R., and Neubarth, Friedrich (eds.) 2001. Naturally! Linguistic studies in honour of Wolfgang Ulrich Dressler. Torino: Rosenberg and SellierGoogle Scholar
Scheibman, Joanne 2000. ‘I dunno: A usage-based account of the phonological reduction of don’t in American English conversation’, Journal of Pragmatics 32(1): 105–24CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scherer, Carmen 2005. Wortbildungswandel und Produktivität: Eine empirische Studie zur nominalen ‘-er’-Derivation im Deutschen. Berlin and New York: Mouton de GruyterCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schiffrin, Deborah (ed.) 1984. Meaning, form, and use in context: Linguistic applications; Georgetown University Round Table ’84. Washington, DC: Georgetown University PressGoogle Scholar
Schleicher, August 1861. ‘Einige Beobachtungen an Kindern’, Beiträge zur vergleichenden Sprachforschung 2: 497–98Google Scholar
Schlüter, Julia 2005. Rhythmic grammar: The influence of rhythm on grammatical variation and change in English. Berlin and New York: Mouton de GruyterCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmid, Hans-Jörg 2007. ‘Entrenchment, salience, and basic levels’, in Geeraerts, and Cuyckens, (eds.), pp. 117–38Google Scholar
Schmid, Hans-Jörg 2010. ‘Does frequency in text really instantiate entrenchment in the cognitive system?’, in Glynn, and Fischer, (eds.), pp. 101–33CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmid, Hans-Jörg (ed.). to appear. Entrenchment, memory and automaticity. The psychology of linguistic knowledge and language learning. Berlin and New York: Mouton de GruyterGoogle Scholar
Schmid, Hans-Jörg, and Küchenhoff, Helmut 2013. ‘Collostructional analysis and other ways of measuring lexicogrammatical attraction: Theoretical premises, practical problems and cognitive underpinnings’, Cognitive Linguistics 24(3): 531–77CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, Richard 1984. ‘The strengths and limitations of acquisition: A case study of an untutored language learner’, Language, Learning, and Communication 3: 116Google Scholar
Schmidtke-Bode, Karsten 2009. ‘Going-to-V and gonna-V in child language: A quantitative approach to constructional development’, Cognitive Linguistics 20(3): 509–38CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schneider, Edgar W. 2011. English around the world: An introduction. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Schoenemann, P. Thomas 2005. ‘Conceptual complexity and the brain: Understanding language origins’, in Wang, and Minett, (eds.), pp. 4783Google Scholar
Schönle, Paul W., Gräbe, Klaus, Wenig, Peter, Höhne, Jörg, Schrader, Jörg, and Conrad, Bastian 1987. ‘Electromagnetic articulography: Use of alternating magnetic fields for tracking movements of multiple points inside and outside the vocal tract’, Brain and Language 31(1): 2635CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schooler, Lael J. 1993. Memory and the statistical structure of the environment. Doctoral dissertation. Carnegie Mellon UniversityGoogle Scholar
Schoonbaert, Sofie, Hartsuiker, Robert J., and Pickering, Martin J. 2007. ‘The representation of lexical and syntactic information in bilinguals: Evidence from syntactic priming’, Journal of Memory and Language 56(2): 153–71CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schreier, Daniel, and Hundt, Marianne (eds.) 2013. English as a contact language. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schumann, John H. 1978. The pidginisation process: A model for second language acquisition. Rowley, MA: Newbury HouseGoogle Scholar
Schumann, John H. 1987. ‘The expression of temporality in Basilang speech’, Studies in Second Language Acquisition 9(1): 2141CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schütze, Carson 1996. The empirical base of linguistics: Grammaticality judgments and linguistic methodology. Chicago: University of Chicago PressGoogle Scholar
Segalowitz, Norman 2010. The cognitive bases of second language fluency. New York: RoutledgeCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seidenberg, Mark S. 1997. ‘Language acquisition and use: Learning and applying probabilistic constraints’, Science 275(5306): 1599–603CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Seidenberg, Mark S., and Bruck, Maggie 1990. ‘Consistency effects in the generation of past tense morphology’, Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 28(6): 522Google Scholar
Seidenberg, Mark S., and MacDonald, Maryellen C. 1999. ‘A probabilistic constraints approach to language acquisition and processing’, Cognitive Science 23(4): 569–88CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seidenberg, Mark S., and McClelland, J. L. 1989. ‘Visual word recognition and pronunciation: A computational model of acquisition, skilled performance, and dyslexia’, in Galaburda, (ed.), pp. 255303Google Scholar
Seidenberg, Mark S., Waters, Gloria S., Barnes, Marcia A., and Tanenhaus, Michael K. 1984. ‘When does irregular spelling or pronunciation influence word recognition?’, Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior 23(3): 383404CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seidlhofer, Barbara 2004. ‘Research perspectives on teaching English as a Lingua Franca’, Annual Review of Applied Linguistics 24: 209–39CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shanks, David R. 1995. The psychology of associative learning. New York: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shannon, Claude E. 1949. The mathematical theory of communication. Champaign, IL: University of Illinois PressGoogle Scholar
Shaoul, Cyrus 2013. ‘The consequences of accumulating experience for lexical processing’. Paper presented at the 8th International Morphology MeetingGoogle Scholar
Shaoul, Cyrus, Schilling, Natalie A., Bitschnau, S., Arppe, Antti, Hendrix, Peter, and Baayen, R. Harald 2014. NDL2: Naive Discriminative Learning. R package version 1.901 [Development version available upon request]Google Scholar
Shaoul, Cyrus, Willits, Jon, Ramscar, Michael, Milin, Petar, and Baayen, R. Harald under revision. ‘A discrimination-driven model for the acquisition of lexical knowledge in auditory comprehension’ [Under revision]Google Scholar
Shepherd, Susan C. 1982. ‘From deontic to epistemic: An analysis of modals in the history of English, creoles, and language acquisition’, in Ahlqvist, (ed.), pp. 316–23CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shiffrin, Richard M., and Schneider, Walter 1977. ‘Controlled and automatic human information processing: II. Perceptual learning, automatic attending and a general theory’, Psychological Review 84(2): 127–90CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shimpi, Priya M., Gámez, Perla B., Huttenlocher, Janellen, and Vasilyeva, Marina 2007. ‘Syntactic priming in 3- and 4-year-old children: Evidence for abstract representations of transitive and dative forms’, Developmental Psychology 43(6): 1334–46CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simon, Herbert A. 1962. The sciences of the artificial. Cambridge, MA: The MIT PressGoogle Scholar
Simpson-Vlach, Rita, and Ellis, Nick C. 2010. ‘An Academic Formulas List: New methods in phraseology research’, Applied Linguistics 31(4): 487512CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sinclair, John M. 1996. ‘The search for units of meaning’, Textus 9: 75106Google Scholar
Singh-Manoux, Archana, Kivimaki, Mika, Glymour, M. Maria, Elbaz, Alexis, Berr, Claudine, Ebmeier, Klaus P., Ferrie, Jane E., and Dugravot, Aline 2012. ‘Timing of onset of cognitive decline: Results from Whitehall II prospective cohort study’, British Medical Journal 344: d7622CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Skarabela, Barbora, Fish, Sarah, and Do, Anna H.-J. (eds.) 2002. Proceedings of the 26th Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development (BUCLD 26). Somerville, MA: Cascadilla PressGoogle Scholar
Skousen, Royal 1989. Analogical modeling of language. Boston, MA: KluwerGoogle Scholar
Skousen, Royal 2002. ‘Introduction’, in Skousen, , Lonsdale, and Parkinson, (eds.), pp. 18CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Skousen, Royal, Lonsdale, Deryle, and Parkinson, Dilworth B. (eds.) 2002. Analogical modeling: An exemplar-based approach to language. Amsterdam: BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slattery, Timothy J., Sturt, Patrick, Christianson, Kiel, Yoshida, Masaya, and Ferreira, Fernanda 2013. ‘Lingering misinterpretations of garden path sentences arise from competing syntactic representations’, Journal of Memory and Language 69(2): 104–20CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slobin, Dan I. 1973. ‘Cognitive prerequisites for the development of grammar’, in Ferguson, and Slobin, (eds.), pp. 175208Google Scholar
Slobin, Dan I. 1985a. ‘Crosslinguistic evidence for the language-making capacity’, in Slobin, (ed.), pp. 1157–249Google Scholar
Slobin, Dan I. (ed.) 1985b. The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, vol. 2: Theoretical issues. Mahwah, NJ: ErlbaumGoogle Scholar
Slobin, Dan I. 1992. Psycholinguistics. Glenview, IL: Scott, Foresman and CompanyGoogle Scholar
Slobin, Dan I. 1994. ‘Talking perfectly. Discourse origins of the present perfect’, in Pagliuca, (ed.), pp. 119–33CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slobin, Dan I. 1997a. ‘The universal, the typological, and the particular in language acquisition’, in Slobin, (ed.), pp. 139Google Scholar
Slobin, Dan I. (ed.) 1997b. The crosslinguistic study of language acquisition, vol. 5: Expanding the contexts. Mahwah, NJ: ErlbaumGoogle Scholar
Slobin, Dan I. 2002. ‘Language evolution, acquisition and diachrony: Probing the parallels’, in Givón, and Malle, (eds.), pp. 375–92CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slobin, Dan I., and Aksu, Ayhan A. 1982. ‘Tense, aspect and modality in the use of the Turkish evidential’, in Hopper, (ed.), pp. 185200CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, Jeremy J. 2012. ‘History of English historical linguistics: The historiography of the English language’, in Bergs, and Brinton, (eds.), pp. 1295–312CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, Nathaniel J., and Levy, Roger 2013. ‘The effect of word predictability on reading time is logarithmic’, Cognition 128(3): 302–19CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sommerer, Lotte 2012. ‘Investigating the emergence of the definite article in Old English: About categorization, gradualness and constructions’, Folia Linguistica Historica 33: 175213Google Scholar
Spufford, Margaret 1981. Small books and pleasant histories: Popular fiction and its readership in seventeenth-century England. Athens, GA: University of Georgia PressGoogle Scholar
Stadler, Michael A., and Frensch, Peter A. (eds.) 1998. Implicit learning handbook. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage PublicationsGoogle Scholar
Stampe, David 1969. ‘The acquisition of phonetic representation’, Chicago Linguistics Society 5: 443–54Google Scholar
Stanford, James N. 2014. ‘Language acquisition and language change’, in Bowern, and Evans, (eds.), pp. 466–83Google Scholar
Stede, Manfred, Chiarcos, Christian, Grabski, Michael, and Lagerwerf, Luuk (eds.) 2005. ‘Salience in discourse: Multidisciplinary approaches to discourse 2005’. Proceedings of the 6th Workshop on Multidisciplinary Approaches to Discourse (MAD-05), Chorin/Berlin, Oct. 2005. Münster: Nordus PublikationenGoogle Scholar
Stefanowitsch, Anatol, and Gries, Stefan Th. 2003. ‘Collostructions: Investigating the interaction of words and constructions’, International Journal of Corpus Linguistics 8(2): 209–43CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stemberger, Joseph P., and MacWhinney, Brian 1986. ‘Frequency and the lexical storage of regularly inflected forms’, Memory and Cognition 14(1): 1726CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stephany, Ursula 1986. ‘Modality’, in Fletcher, and Garman, (eds.), pp. 375400CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Street, James A., and Dąbrowska, Ewa 2010. ‘More individual differences in language attainment: How much do adult native speakers of English know about passives and quantifiers?’, Lingua 120(8): 2080–94CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stromswold, Karin J. 1990. Learnability and the acquisition of auxiliaries. Doctoral dissertation. Massachusetts Institute of Technology [Distributed by MIT Working Papers in Linguistics]Google Scholar
Studdert-Kennedy, M. 1991. ‘Language development from an evolutionary perspective’, in Krasnegor, , Rumbaugh, , Schiefelbusch, and Studdert-Kennedy, (eds.), pp. 528Google Scholar
Stumper, Barbara, and Lieven, Elena V. M. 2011. ‘Pronoun case errors in the acquisition of German are not rare, but systematic’. Poster presented at the 12th Triennial Conference of the International Association for the Study of Child Language (UQAM, Montreal)Google Scholar
Sturt, Patrick 2007. ‘Semantic re-interpretation and garden path recovery’, Cognition 105(2): 477–88CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sturt, Patrick, Pickering, Martin J. and Crocker, Matthew W. 1999. ‘Structural change and reanalysis difficulty in language comprehension’, Journal of Memory and Language 40(1): 136–50CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sturt, Patrick, Pickering, Martin J., Scheepers, Christoph, and Crocker, Matthew W. 2001. ‘The preservation of structure in language comprehension: Is reanalysis the last resort?’, Journal of Memory and Language 45(2): 283307CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suttle, Laura, and Goldberg, Adele E. 2011. ‘The partial productivity of constructions as induction’, Linguistics 49(6): 1237–69CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swan, Toril, and Westvik, Olaf Jansen (eds.) 1996. Modality in Germanic languages: Historical and comparative perspectives. Berlin and New York: Mouton de GruyterGoogle Scholar
Sweet, Henry 1888. A history of English sounds. Oxford: Clarendon PressGoogle Scholar
Sweetser, Eve E. 1988. ‘Grammaticalization and semantic bleaching’, in Axmaker, , Jaisser, and Singmaster, (eds.), pp. 389405CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Swets, Benjamin, Desmet, Timothy, Clifton, Charles, and Ferreira, Fernanda 2008. ‘Underspecification of syntactic ambiguities: Evidence from self-paced reading’, Memory and Cognition 36(1): 201–16CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Szagun, Gisela 2001. ‘Learning different regularities: The acquisition of noun plurals by German-speaking children’, First Language 21(62): 109–41CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Szagun, Gisela 2006. Sprachentwicklung beim Kind: Ein Lehrbuch. 3rd edition. Weinheim and Basel: BeltzGoogle Scholar
Szagun, Gisela, Stumper, Barbara, Sondag, Nina, and Franik, Melanie 2007. ‘The acquisition of gender marking by young German-speaking children: Evidence for learning guided by phonological regularities’, Journal of Child Language 34(3): 445–71CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Szmrecsanyi, Benedikt 2006. Morphosyntactic persistence in spoken English. A corpus study at the intersection of variationist sociolinguistics, psycholinguistics, and discourse analysis. Berlin and New York: Mouton de GruyterCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Szmrecsanyi, Benedikt 2013. ‘Diachronic probabilistic grammar’, English Language and Linguistics [Journal of the English Linguistics Society of Korea] 19(3): 4168Google Scholar
Tabak, Wieke, Schreuder, Robert, and Baayen, R. Harald 2010. ‘Producing inflected verbs: A picture naming study’, The Mental Lexicon 5(1): 2246CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tabor, Whitney 1993. ‘The gradual development of degree modifier sort of and kind of: A corpus proximity model’, in Beals, (ed.), pp. 451–65Google Scholar
Tagliamonte, Sali A., and D’Arcy, Alexandra 2007. ‘The modals of obligation/necessity in Canadian perspective’, English World-Wide 28(1): 4787CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tagliamonte, Sali A., D’Arcy, Alexandra, and Jankowski, Bridget 2010. ‘Social work and linguistic systems: Marking possession in Canadian English’, Language Variation and Change 22(1): 149–73CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tannen, Deborah 1989. Talking voices: Repetition, dialogue, and imagery in conversational discourse. New York: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Taraban, Roman, and McClelland, James L. 1987. ‘Conspiracy effects in word pronunciation’, Journal of Memory and Language 26(6): 608–31CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Terkourafi, Marina 2011. ‘The pragmatic variable: Toward a procedural interpretation’, Language in Society 40(3): 343–72CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Terrell, Tracy 1991. ‘The role of grammar instruction in a communicative approach’, The Modern Language Journal 75(1): 5263CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Theakston, Anna L. 2004. ‘The role of entrenchment in constraining children’s verb argument structure overgeneralisations: A grammaticality judgment study’, Cognitive Development 19(1): 1534CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Theakston, Anna L., Lieven, Elena V. M., and Tomasello, Michael 2003. ‘The role of input in the acquisition of third-person singular verbs in English’, Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research 46(4): 863–77CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomason, Sarah G. 2013. ‘Innovation and contact: The role of adults (and children)’, in Schreier, and Hundt, (eds.), pp. 283–97CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thompson, Sandra A., and Mulac, Anthony J. 1991. ‘A quantitative perspective on the grammaticalization of epistemic parentheticals in English’, in Traugott, and Heine, (eds.), pp. 213329CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thurner, Stefan, Hanel, Rudolf, Liu, Bo, and Corominas-Murtra, Bernat 2015. ‘Understanding Zipf’s law of word frequencies through sample-space collapse in sentence formation’, Journal of The Royal Society Interface 12(108): 20150330CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Timberlake, Alan 1977. ‘Reanalysis and actualization in syntactic change’, in Li, (ed.), pp. 141–77CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomaschek, Fabian, Tucker, Benjamin V., Wieling, Martijn, and Baayen, R. Harald 2014. ‘Vowel articulation affected by word frequency’, in Fuchs, , Grice, , Hermes, , Lancia, and Mücke, (eds.), pp. 429–32Google Scholar
Tomasello, Michael 1992. First verbs: A case study of early grammatical development. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, Michael (ed.) 1998. The new psychology of language: Cognitive and functional approaches to language structure. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence ErlbaumGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, Michael 1999. The cultural origins of human cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University PressGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, Michael 2003. Constructing a language: A usage-based theory of language acquisition. Boston, MA: Harvard University PressGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, Michael 2008. Origins of human communication. Cambridge, MA: The MIT PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, Michael, and Call, Josep 1997. Primate cognition. Oxford: Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, Michael, and Slobin, Dan I. (eds.) 2004. Beyond nature-nurture: Essays in honor of Elizabeth Bates. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence ErlbaumCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tooley, Kirsten, and Bock, J. Kathryn 2014. ‘On the parity of structural persistence in language production and comprehension’, Cognition 132(2): 101–36CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Toupin, Fabienne, and Lowrey, Brian (eds.) 2015. Studies in linguistic variation and change: From Old to Middle English. Newcastle: Cambridge Scholars PublishingGoogle Scholar
Townsend, David, and Bever, Thomas 2001. Sentence comprehension: The integration of habits and rules. Cambridge, MA: The MIT PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Traugott, Elizabeth C. 1989. ‘On the rise of epistemic meaning in English: An example of subjectification in semantic change’, Language 65(1): 3155CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Traugott, Elizabeth C. 1995. ‘The role of the development of discourse markers in a theory of grammaticalization’. Paper presented at the 12th International Conference on Historical Linguistics (ICHL 12), Manchester, UK, August 13–18, 1995. www.stanford.edu/~traugott/papers/discourse.pdf [accessed April 19, 2016]Google Scholar
Traugott, Elizabeth C. 1999. ‘The role of pragmatics in semantic change’, in Verschueren, (ed.), pp. 93102Google Scholar
Traugott, Elizabeth C. 2008a. ‘Testing the hypothesis that priming is a motivation for change’, Theoretical Linguistics 34: 135–42CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Traugott, Elizabeth C. 2008b. ‘The grammaticalization of NP of NP patterns’, in Bergs, and Diewald, (eds.), pp. 2345CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Traugott, Elizabeth C. 2011. ‘Grammaticalization and mechanisms of change’, in Narrog, and Heine, (eds.), pp. 1930CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Traugott, Elizabeth C. 2012a. ‘Pragmatics and language change’, in Allan, and Jaszczolt, (eds.), pp. 549–65CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Traugott, Elizabeth C. 2012b. ‘The status of onset contexts in analysis of micro-changes’, in Kytö, (ed.), pp. 221–55CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Traugott, Elizabeth C., and Dasher, Richard B. 2002. Regularity in semantic change. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Traugott, Elizabeth C., and Heine, Bernd (eds.) 1991. Approaches to grammaticalization. 2 vols. Amsterdam: BenjaminsGoogle Scholar
Traugott, Elizabeth C., and König, Ekkehard 1991. ‘The semantics-pragmatics of grammaticalization revisited’, in Traugott, and Heine, (eds.), pp. 189218CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Traugott, Elizabeth C., Labrum, Rebecca, and Shepherd, Susan (eds.) 1980. Papers from the Fourth International Conference on Historical Linguistics. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: John BenjaminsGoogle Scholar
Traugott, Elizabeth C., and Trousdale, Graeme 2010a. ‘Gradience, gradualness and grammaticalization: How do they intersect?’, in Traugott, and Trousdale, (eds.), pp. 1944CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Traugott, Elizabeth C., and Trousdale, Graeme (eds.) 2010b. Gradience, gradualness and grammaticalization. Amsterdam: BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Traugott, Elizabeth C., and Trousdale, Graeme 2013. Constructionalization and constructional changes. Oxford: Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Traxler, Matthew 2002. ‘Plausibility and subcategorization preference in children’s processing of temporarily ambiguous sentences: Evidence from self-paced reading’, The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 55(1): 7596CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Traxler, Matthew J., and Gernsbacher, Morton A. (eds.) 2006. Handbook of Psycholinguistics. 2nd edition. London: ElsevierGoogle Scholar
Treisman, Anne M., and Gelade, Garry 1980. ‘A feature-integration theory of attention’, Cognitive Psychology 12: 97136CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tremblay, Antoine, Derwing, Bruce, Libben, Gary, and Westbury, Chris 2011. ‘Processing advantages of lexical bundles: Evidence from self-paced reading and sentence recall tasks’, Language Learning 61(2): 569613CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trimmer, Pete C., McNamara, John M., Houston, Alasdair I., and Marshall, James A. R. 2012. ‘Does natural selection favour the Rescorla–Wagner rule?’, Journal of Theoretical Biology 302: 3952CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trousdale, Graeme 2013. ‘Gradualness in language change: A constructional perspective’, in Giacalone Ramat, , Mauri, , and Molinelli, (eds.), pp. 2742CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trudgill, Peter 1983. On dialect: Social and geographical perspectives. Oxford: BlackwellGoogle Scholar
Trudgill, Peter 2002a. ‘Linguistic and social typology’, in Chambers, , Trudgill, and Schilling-Estes, (eds.), pp. 707–28CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trudgill, Peter 2002b. Sociolinguistic variation and change. Edinburgh: Edinburgh University Press and Washington, DC: Georgetown University PressGoogle Scholar
Trueswell, John, Sekerina, Irina, Hill, Nicole, and Logrip, Marian 1999. ‘The kindergarden-path effect: Studying on-line sentence processing in children’, Cognition 73(2): 89134CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trueswell, John, Tanenhaus, Michael, and Garnsey, Susan 1994. ‘Semantic influences on parsing: Use of thematic role information in syntactic ambiguity resolution’, Journal of Memory and Language 33(3): 285–18CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trueswell, John, Tanenhaus, Michael, and Kello, Christopher 1993. ‘Verb-specific constraints in sentence processing: Separating effects of lexical preference from garden-paths’, Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition 19(3): 528–53Google ScholarPubMed
Tulving, Endel, and Thomson, Donald M. 1973. ‘Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory’, Psychological Review 80(5): 352–73CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Underwood, Geoffrey (ed.) 1978. Strategies of information processing. San Diego, CA: Academic PressGoogle Scholar
van Bergen, Linda 2013. ‘Early progressive passives’, Folia Linguistica Historica 34(1): 173207Google Scholar
van der Auwera, Johan, and Plungian, Vladimir A. 1998. ‘Modality’s semantic map’, Linguistic Typology 2(1): 79124CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van der Horst, Joop 2008. Geschiedenis van de Nederlandse syntaxis. Leuven: Leuven University PressGoogle Scholar
van de Velde, Freek 2015. ‘Schijnbare syntactische feniksen’, Nederlandse Taalkunde 20: 69107CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Geert, Paul, and Steenbeek, Henderien 2005. ‘Explaining after by before: Basic aspects of a dynamic systems approach to the study of development’, Developmental Review 25(3–4): 408–42CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Gelderen, Elly 2004. Grammaticalization as economy. Amsterdam: BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Gelderen, Elly 2011. The linguistic cycle: Language change and the language faculty. Oxford: Oxford University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Gompel, Roger (ed.) 2013. Sentence processing. New York: Psychology PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Halteren, Hans, Baayen, R. Harald, Tweedie, Fiona, Haverkort, Marco, and Neijt, Anneke 2005. ‘New machine learning methods demonstrate the existence of a human stylome’, Journal of Quantitative Linguistics 12(1): 6577CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Kemenade, Ans, and de Haas, Nynke (eds.) 2012. Historical linguistics 2009: Selected papers from the 19th international conference on historical linguistics, Nijmegen, 10–14 August 2009. Amsterdam and Philadelphia: BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Kemenade, Ans, and Los, Bettelou (eds.) 2006. The handbook of the history of English. Oxford: BlackwellCrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Kemenade, Ans, and Westergaard, Marit 2012. ‘Syntax and information structure: V2 variation in Middle English’, in Meurman-Solin, , López-Couso, and Los, (eds.), pp. 87118CrossRefGoogle Scholar
van Marle, Jaap 1985. On the paradigmatic dimension of morphological creativity. Dordrecht: ForisGoogle Scholar
van Petten, Cyma, and Luka, Barbara J. 2012. ‘Prediction during language comprehension: Benefits, costs, and ERP components’, International Journal of Psychophysiology 83(2): 176–90CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
van Steenis, Lindsey 2013. The grammaticalization of have to and hebben te: A comparative study between English and Dutch. MA thesis. University of Amsterdam: Faculty of HumanitiesGoogle Scholar
Vasilyeva, Marina, Waterfall, Heidi, Gámez, Perla B., Gómez, Ligia E., Bowers, Edmond, and Shimpi, Priya 2010. ‘Cross-linguistic syntactic priming in bilingual children’, Journal of Child Language 37(5): 1047–64CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vennemann, Theo 1972. ‘Phonetic detail in assimilation: Problems in Germanic phonology’, Language 48(4): 863–92CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veronique, Daniel 1999. ‘L’émergence de catégories grammaticales dans les langues créoles: Grammaticalisation et réanalyse’, in Lang, and Neumann-Holzschuh, (eds.), pp. 181203CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Veronique, Daniel 2001. ‘Genèse(s) et changement(s) grammaticaux: Quelques modestes leçons tirés de l’émergence des créoles et de l’acquisition des langues étrangères’, in Matthey, (ed.), pp. 273303CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Verschueren, Jef (ed.) 1999. Pragmatics in 1998: Selected papers from the 6th international pragmatics conference, vol. 2. Antwerp and Amsterdam: International Pragmatics AssociationGoogle Scholar
Vihman, Marilyn May 1980. ‘Sound change and child language’, in Traugott, , Labrum, and Shepherd, (eds.), pp. 303–20CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vincent, David 1993. Literacy and popular culture: England 1750–1914. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Visser, Frederikus Th. 1963–1973. An historical syntax of the English language. 3 vols. Leiden: BrillGoogle Scholar
Visser, Frederikus Th. 1984. An historical syntax of the English language, vol. 2: Syntactical units with one verb (continued). Leiden: Brill [First edition 1966]Google Scholar
von Eye, Alexander 1990. Introduction to configurational frequency analysis. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Wagner, Michael, Breen, Mara, Flemming, Edward, Shattuck-Hufnagel, Stefanie, and Gibson, Edward 2010. ‘Prosodic effects of discourse salience and association with focus’. Proceedings of the Fifth International Conference on Speech Prosody, Chicago, May 11–14, 2010. http://prosodylab.org/~chael/www/papers/wagneretal10only.pdf [accessed April 19, 2016]CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wales, Roger J., and Walker, Edward (eds.) 1976. New approaches to language mechanisms: A collection of psycholinguistics studies. Amsterdam: North HollandGoogle Scholar
Walling, Anne D., and Dickson, Gretchen M. 2012. ‘Hearing loss in older adults’, American Family Physician 85(12): 1150–56Google ScholarPubMed
Waltereit, Richard 2006. ‘The rise of discourse markers in Italian: A specific type of language change’, in Fischer, (ed.), pp. 6176CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wang, William S.-Y., and Minett, James W. (eds.) 2005. Language acquisition, change and emergence: Essays in evolutionary linguistics. Hong Kong: City University of Hong Kong PressGoogle Scholar
Warner, Anthony A. 1993. English auxiliaries: Structure and history. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Warrington, Elizabeth K. 1975. ‘The selective impairment of semantic memory’, The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology 27(4): 635–57CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waterfall, Heidi R. 2006. A little change is a good thing: Feature theory, language acquisition and variation sets. Doctoral dissertation. University of Chicago: Department of LinguisticsGoogle Scholar
Wegener, Heide 1999. ‘Die Pluralbildung im Deutschen – ein Versuch im Rahmen der Optimalitätstheorie’, Linguistik online 4.3/1999CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weinreich, Uriel, Labov, William, and Herzog, Marvin 1968. ‘Empirical foundations for a theory of language change’, in Lehmann, and Malkiel, (eds.), pp. 95198Google Scholar
Wexler, Ken 1998. ‘Very early parameter setting and the unique checking constraint: A new explanation of the optional infinitive stage’, Lingua 106(1): 2379CrossRefGoogle Scholar
White, Katherine S., Yee, Eiling, Blumstein, Sheila E., and Morgan, James L. 2013. ‘Adults show less sensitivity to phonetic detail in unfamiliar words, too’, Journal of Memory and Language 68(4): 362–78CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whitman, John 2001. ‘Relabelling’, in Pintzuk, , Tsoulas, and Warner, (eds.), pp. 220–38CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whitman, John 2012. ‘Misparsing and syntactic reanalysis’, in van Kemenade, and de Haas, (eds.), pp. 6987CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiechmann, Daniel 2015. Understanding relative clauses – A usage-based view on the processing of complex constructions. Berlin and New York: Mouton de GruyterCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiechmann, Daniel, Kerz, Elma, Snider, Neal, and Jaeger, T. Florian 2013. ‘Introduction to the Special Issue: Parsimony and redundancy in models of language’, Language and Speech 0(0): 18Google Scholar
Wiemer, Björn 2015. ‘Quo vadis grammaticalization theory? Why complex language change is like words’, Folia Linguistica 48(2): 425–67Google Scholar
Wiese, Richard 1996. The phonology of German. Oxford: Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
Wijnen, Frank, Kempen, Masja, and Gillis, Steven 2001. ‘Root infinitives in Dutch early child language’, Journal of Child Language 28(3): 629–60CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Williams, Jake R., Lessard, Paul R., Desu, Suma, Clark, Eric M., Bagrow, James P., Danforth, Christopher M., and Dodds, Peter S. 2014. ‘Zipf’s law holds for phrases, not words’, Scientific Reports 5: 12209CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wills, Andy J. 2005. New directions in human associative learning. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence ErlbaumCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wills, Andy J. 2009. ‘Prediction errors and attention in the presence and absence of feedback’, Current Directions in Psychological Science 18(2): 95100CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winters, Margaret E. 2010. ‘Introduction: On the emergence of diachronic cognitive linguistics’, in Winters, , Tissari, and Allan, (eds.), pp. 327CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Winters, Margaret E., Tissari, Heli, and Allan, Kathryn (eds.) 2010. Historical cognitive linguistics. Cognitive Linguistics Research 47. Berlin: Mouton de GruyterCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wischer, Ilse, and Diewald, Gabriele (eds.) 2002. New reflections on grammaticalization. Amsterdam: John BenjaminsCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wolk, Christoph, Bresnan, Joan, Rosenbach, Anette, and Szmrecsanyi, Benedikt 2013. ‘Dative and genitive variability in Late Modern English: Exploring cross-constructional variation and change’, Diachronica 30(3): 382419CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood, Frederick T. 1955–6. ‘Verb-adverb combinations: The position of the adverb’, English Language Teaching 10(1): 1827CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wood, Simon N. 2006. Generalized Additive Models: An introduction with R. CRC Texts in Statistical Science. New York: Chapman and Hall/CRCCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Xu, Fei, and Tennenbaum, Joshua B. 2007. ‘Word learning as Bayesian inference’, Psychological Review 114(2): 245–72CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Yáñez-Bouza, Nuria, Moore, Emma, Hollmann, Willem, and van Bergen, Linda (eds.) forthcoming. Analysing English syntax. Cambridge: Cambridge University PressGoogle Scholar
Yanovich, Igor 2013. Four pieces for modality, context and usage. Doctoral dissertation. Massachusetts Institute of Technology.Google Scholar
Ziegeler, Debra 1997. ‘Retention in ontogentic and diachronic grammaticalization’, Cognitive Linguistics 8(3): 207–41CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zipf, George K. 1935. The psycho-biology of language: An introduction to dynamic philology. Cambridge, MA: The MIT PressGoogle Scholar
Zipf, George K. 1949. Human behaviour and the principle of least effort: An introduction to human ecology. Cambridge, MA: Addison-Wesley and New York: HafnerGoogle Scholar
Zuraw, Kie 2003. ‘Probability in language change’, in Bod, , Hay, and Jannedy, (eds.), pp. 139–76CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zwaan, Rolf A., and Radvansky, Gabriel A. 1998. ‘Situation models in language comprehension and memory’, Psychological Bulletin 123(2): 162–85CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×