Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-g8jcs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-30T20:33:10.372Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

5.8 - Habit Formation

from 5 - Neural Circuits

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  08 November 2023

Mary-Ellen Lynall
Affiliation:
University of Cambridge
Peter B. Jones
Affiliation:
University of Cambridge
Stephen M. Stahl
Affiliation:
University of California, San Diego
Get access

Summary

To understand the most sophisticated human capacities, such as analytical inquiry, social interaction or creativity, it is important to first understand the most basic. This chapter aims to introduce one of the most fundamental brain processes there is – the ability to form habits, to automate the routine and to ignore things that have become predictable. Habits are brilliant in their simplicity and the poetic irony is that we notice least these behaviours that we know the best. If you take one thing away from this chapter, let it be this: without our ability to develop habits, to automate tasks we have performed 100 times or more, our brains would lack capacity to support any of the advanced functions that we prize as humans. At every level of complex thought, habits provide the essential scaffolding. Aside from the fundamental importance of understanding habits for gaining a broader concept of how the brain processes information and arrives at decisions, habits are of extra interest to those in the field of psychiatry. As you will learn, habits play a key role in a range of compulsive disorders, including obsessive–compulsive disorder and addiction.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2023

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Dickinson, A. Actions and habits: the development of behavioural autonomy. Philos Trans R Soc London B 1985; 308(1135): 6778.Google Scholar
Adams, CD. Variations in the sensitivity of instrumental responding to reinforcer devaluation. Q J Exp Psychol B 1982; 34: 7798.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yin, HH, Ostlund, SB, Knowlton, BJ, Balleine, BW. The role of the dorsomedial striatum in instrumental conditioning. Eur J Neurosci 2005; 22(2): 513–523.Google ScholarPubMed
Yin, HH, Knowlton, BJ, Balleine, BW. Lesions of dorsolateral striatum preserve outcome expectancy but disrupt habit formation in instrumental learning. Eur J Neurosci 2004; 19(1): 181189.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, KS, Graybiel, AM. Habit formation coincides with shifts in reinforcement representations in the sensorimotor striatum. J Neurophysiol 2016; 115(3): 14871498.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Wit, S, Watson, P, Harsay, HA et al. Corticostriatal connectivity underlies individual differences in the balance between habitual and goal-directed action control. J Neurosci 2012; 32(35): 1206612075.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tricomi, E, Balleine, BW, O’Doherty, JP. A specific role for posterior dorsolateral striatum in human habit learning. Eur J Neurosci 2009; 29(11): 22252232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valentin, VV, Dickinson, A, O’Doherty, JP. Determining the neural substrates of goal-directed learning in the human brain. J Neurosci 2007; 27(15): 4019–4026.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Killcross, S, Coutureau, E. Coordination of actions and habits in the medial prefrontal cortex of rats. Cereb Cortex 2003; 13(4): 400408.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vikbladh, OM, Meager, MR, King, J et al. Hippocampal contributions to model-based planning and spatial memory. Neuron 2019; 102(3): 683693.e4.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schreiner, DC, Renteria, R, Gremel, CM. Fractionating the all-or-nothing definition of goal-directed and habitual decision-making. J Neurosci Res 2020; 98(6): 9981006.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cushman, F, Morris, A. Habitual control of goal selection in humans. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2015; 112(45): 1381713822.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Daw, ND, Gershman, SJ, Seymour, B, Dayan, P, Dolan, RJ. Model-based influences on humans’ choices and striatal prediction errors. Neuron 2011; 69(6): 12041215.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gillan, CM, Otto, AR, Phelps, EA, Daw, ND. Model-based learning protects against forming habits. Cogn Affect Behav Neurosci 2015; 15(3): 523536.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Decker, JH, Otto, AR, Daw, ND, Hartley, CA. From creatures of habit to goal-directed learners: tracking the developmental emergence of model-based reinforcement learning. Psychol Sci 2016; 27(6): 848858.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eppinger, B, Walter, M, Heekeren, HR, Li, SC. Of goals and habits: age-related and individual differences in goal-directed decision-making. Front Neurosci 2013; 7: 253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Otto, AR, Skatova, A, Madlon-Kay, S, Daw, ND. Cognitive control predicts use of model-based reinforcement learning. J Cogn Neurosci 2015; 27(2): 319333.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gillan, CM, Kosinski, M, Whelan, R, Phelps, EA, Daw, ND. Characterizing a psychiatric symptom dimension related to deficits in goal-directed control. elife 2016; 5.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
de Wit, S, Kindt, M, Knot, SL et al. Shifting the balance between goals and habits: five failures in experimental habit induction. J Exp Psychol Gen 2018; 147(7): 10431065.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hardwick, R, Forrence, A, Krakauer, J, Haith, A. Time-dependent competition between habitual and goal-directed response preparation. Nature Hum Behav 2019; 3(12): 12521262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luque, D, Molinero, S, Watson, P, López, FJ, Le Pelley, ME. Measuring habit formation through goal-directed response switching. J Exp Psychol Gen 2020; 149(8): 14491459.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graybiel, AM, Rauch, SL. Toward a neurobiology of obsessive–compulsive disorder. Neuron 2000; 28(2): 343347.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gillan, CM, Papmeyer, M, Morein-Zamir, S et al. Disruption in the balance between goal-directed behavior and habit learning in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2011; 168(7): 718726.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gillan, CM, Morein-Zamir, S, Urcelay, GP et al. Enhanced avoidance habits in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Biol Psychiatry 2014; 75(8): 631638.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Voon, V, Derbyshire, K, Rück, C et al. Disorders of compulsivity: a common bias towards learning habits. Mol Psychiatry 2015; 20(3): 345352.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gillan, CM, Apergis-Schoute, AM, Morein-Zamir, S et al. Functional neuroimaging of avoidance habits in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Am J Psychiatry 2015; 172(3): 284293.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Banca, P, Voon, V, Vestergaard, MD et al. Imbalance in habitual versus goal directed neural systems during symptom provocation in obsessive–compulsive disorder. Brain 2015; 138(Pt 3): 798811.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morris, RW, Quail, S, Griffiths, KR, Green, MJ, Balleine, BW. Corticostriatal control of goal-directed action is impaired in schizophrenia. Biol Psychiatry 2015; 77(2), 187195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alvares, GA, Balleine, BW, Guastella, AJ. Impairments in goal-directed actions predict treatment response to cognitive–behavioral therapy in social anxiety disorder. PLoS One 2014; 9(4): e94778.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 5th ed. (DSM-5). American Psychiatric Association, 2013.Google Scholar
Gillan, C, Kalanthroff, E, Evans, M et al. Comparison of the association between goal-directed planning and self-reported compulsivity vs obsessive–compulsive disorder diagnosis. JAMA Psychiatry 2020; 77(1): 7785.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×