Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-p9bg8 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-26T13:26:51.313Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

37 - The Culture of Intelligence

from PART IV - SOCIAL SCIENCE AS DISCOURSE AND PRACTICE IN PUBLIC AND PRIVATE LIFE

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  28 March 2008

Theodore M. Porter
Affiliation:
University of California, Los Angeles
Dorothy Ross
Affiliation:
The Johns Hopkins University
Get access

Summary

Over the past two centuries, the concept of human mental ability has undergone three important transformations: from a concept referring to a general faculty to one primarily referring to an individual attribute; from a focus on talents in the plural to one on intelligence in the singular; and from a position of relatively limited cultural significance to one of considerable weight within the United States and, to a lesser extent, within various European countries. These shifts in meaning and emphasis have rendered intelligence a tool available to government, business, and the “helping professions” for the purpose of sorting, classifying, diagnosing, and justifying. Starting in the early part of the twentieth century, determinations of degree of intelligence have been used as aids in the placement of army recruits, in determing the kind of schooling a child will receive, in the hiring of job applicants, and in the decision to allow a person legal immigration. This chapter explores how intelligence has come to play these various social roles. It focuses especially on how experts in the human sciences have both created new meanings for the concept of intelligence and developed technologies that could make those meanings available and useful to a wider public.

FROM TALENTS TO INTELLIGENCE

During much of the nineteenth century, two distinct languages flourished in scientific and intellectual circles to describe the operations of the human mind. Mental philosophers and others interested primarily in what would later be called the “normal” employed a language of character and talents, emphasizing the diversity of the mental faculties and the operation of the individual mind.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2003

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adams, Mark B., ed., The Wellborn Science: Eugenics in Germany, France, Brazil, and Russia (New York: Oxford University Press, 1990)Google Scholar
Bagley, William C., “Educational Determinism; or Democracy and the I.Q.,” School and Society, 15 (1922).Google Scholar
Baritz, Loren, Servants of Power: A History of the Use of the Social Sciences in American Industry (Middletown, Conn.: Wesleyan University Press, 1960).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barkan, Elazar, The Retreat of Scientific Racism: Changing Concepts of Race in Britain and the United States between the World Wars (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1992).Google Scholar
Binet, Alfred and Simon, Théodore, The Development of Intelligence in Children, trans. Kite, Elizabeth S. (Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins, 1916).Google Scholar
Block, N. J. and Dworkin, Gerald, eds., The IQ Controversy (New York: Pantheon, 1976)Google Scholar
Boas, Franz, The Mind of Primitive Man (New York: Macmillan, 1911)Google Scholar
Brigham, Carl C., A Study of American Intelligence (Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press, 1923).Google Scholar
Brigham, , “Intelligence Tests of Immigrant Groups,” Psychological Review, 37 (1930).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carson, John, “Army Alpha, Army Brass, and the Search for Army Intelligence,” Isis, 84 (1993).Google Scholar
Carson, , “Talents, Intelligence, and the Construction of Human Difference”; Wolf, Theta H., Alfred Binet (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1973).Google Scholar
Chapman, Paul D., Schools as Sorters: Lewis M. Terman, Applied Psychology, and the Intelligence Testing Movement, 1890–1930 (New York: New York University Press, 1988).Google Scholar
Collini, Stefan, “The Idea of ‘Character’ in Victorian Political Thought,” Transactions of the Royal Historical Society, 5th ser., 35 (1985).Google Scholar
Cravens, Hamilton, Before Head Start: The Iowa Station and America’s Children (Chapel Hill: University of North Carolina Press, 1993).Google Scholar
Danziger, Kurt, Naming the Mind: How Psychology Found Its Language (London: Sage, 1997)Google Scholar
Daston, Lorraine, “The Naturalized Female Intellect,” Science in Context, 5 (1992)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Degler, Carl N., In Search of Human Nature: The Decline and Revival of Darwinism in American Social Thought (New York: Oxford University Press, 1991).Google Scholar
Fancher, Raymond E., The Intelligence Men: Makers of the IQ Controversy (New York: Norton, 1985)Google Scholar
Galton, Francis, Hereditary Genius: An Inquiry into Its Laws and Consequences (1869) (Gloucester: Peter Smith, 1972).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, Howard, Frames of Mind: The Theory of Multiple Intelligences (New York: Basic Books, 1983)Google Scholar
Hawkins, Mike, Social Darwinism in European and American Thought, 1860–1945: Nature as Model and Nature as Threat (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1997).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hearnshaw, Leslie S., Cyril Burt, Psychologist (London: Hodder and Stoughton, 1979)Google Scholar
Herman, Ellen, The Romance of American Psychology (Berkeley: University of California Press, 1995).Google Scholar
Herrnstein, Richard J. and Murray, Charles, The Bell Curve: Intelligence and Class Structure in American Life (New York: Free Press, 1994).Google Scholar
Jay Gould, Stephen, The Mismeasure of Man (New York: Norton, 1981)Google Scholar
Jensen, Arthur R., “How Much Can We Boost IQ and Scholastic Achievement?,” Harvard Educational Review, 39 (1969).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kevles, Daniel J., In the Name of Eugenics: Genetics and the Uses of Heredity (Berkeley: University of California Press, 1985).Google Scholar
Klineberg, Otto, Race Differences (New York: Harper and Brothers, 1935).Google Scholar
Kloppenberg, James T., Uncertain Victory: Social Democracy and Progressivism in European and American Thought, 1870–1920 (New York: Oxford University Press, 1986).Google Scholar
Minton, Henry L., Lewis M. Terman: Pioneer in Psychological Testing (New York: New York University Press, 1988).Google Scholar
Moody, Joseph N., French Education since Napoleon (Syracuse, N.Y.: Syracuse University Press, 1978).Google Scholar
Porter, Theodore M., Trust in Numbers: The Pursuit of Objectivity in Science and Public Life (Princeton, N.J.: Princeton University Press, 1995)Google Scholar
Rose, Nikolas, The Psychological Complex: Psychology, Politics and Society in England, 1869–1939 (London: Routledge, 1985)Google Scholar
Rose, Steven, Lewontin, R. C., and Kamin, Leon J., Not in Our Genes: Biology, Ideology and Human Nature (London: Penguin, 1984).Google Scholar
Ross, Dorothy, The Origins of American Social Science (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1991)Google Scholar
Schneider, William H., “After Binet: French Intelligence Testing, 1900–1950,” Journal of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 28 (1992).3.0.CO;2-W>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smith, Roger, The Norton History of the Human Sciences (New York: Norton, 1997)Google Scholar
Sokal, Michael M., ed., Psychological Testing and American Society, 1890–1930 (New Brunswick, N.J.: Rutgers University Press, 1987).Google Scholar
Spearman, Charles, “ ‘General Intelligence,’ Objectively Determined and Measured,” American Journal of Psychology, 15 (1904).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stanton, William, The Leopard’s Spots: Scientific Attitudes toward Race in America, 1815–59 (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1960).Google Scholar
Sternberg, Robert J., Beyond IQ: The Triarchic Theory of Human Intelligence (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1985).Google Scholar
Sutherland, Gillian, Ability, Merit and Measurement (Oxford: Oxford University Press, 1984)Google Scholar
Terman, Lewis M., The Measurement of Intelligence: An Explanation of and a Complete Guide for the Use of the Stanford Revision and Extension of the Binet–Simon Intelligence Scale (Boston: Houghton Mifflin, 1916)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walker Howe, Daniel, The Political Culture of the American Whigs (Chicago: University of Chicago Press, 1979)Google Scholar
Wayland, Francis, The Elements of Intellectual Philosophy (New York: Sheldon, 1864).Google Scholar
Wechsler, David, The Measurement of Adult Intelligence (Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins, 1939).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wiebe, Robert H., The Search for Order, 1877–1920 (New York: Hill and Wang, 1967).Google Scholar
Wilson, James Q. and Herrnstein, Richard J., Crime and Human Nature (New York: Simon and Schuster, 1985)Google Scholar
Wood, Gordon S., The Radicalism of the American Revolution (New York: Vintage, 1991).Google Scholar
Wooldridge, Adrian, Measuring the Mind: Education and Psychology in England, c. 1860–1990 (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1994).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zenderland, Leila, Measuring Minds: Henry Herbert Goddard and the Origins of American Mental Testing (Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, 1998).Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×