Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-2brh9 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-27T20:43:39.280Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

19 - Diversity in Multilingual Learners

How Variation in Learners and Contexts for Learning Shape the Acquisition and Processing of an L3/Ln

from Part IV - L3/Ln in Action

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  13 July 2023

Jennifer Cabrelli
Affiliation:
University of Illinois, Chicago
Adel Chaouch-Orozco
Affiliation:
The Hong Kong Polytechnic University
Jorge González Alonso
Affiliation:
Universidad Nebrija, Spain and UiT, Arctic University of Norway
Sergio Miguel Pereira Soares
Affiliation:
Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics
Eloi Puig-Mayenco
Affiliation:
King's College London
Jason Rothman
Affiliation:
UiT, Arctic University of Norway and Universidad Nebrija, Spain
Get access

Summary

In this chapter we review current research on how the experiences that multilingual learners bring to language learning may shape the trajectory and outcome of learning an L3/Ln. While there is abundant evidence that the particular languages that individuals speak contribute to the process of new language learning, and evidence that multilingualism may enhance new learning, there is now emerging research demonstrating that the cognitive and neural mechanisms that enable bilinguals and multilinguals to use two or more languages effectively may also contribute to the course and consequences of new language learning. The dynamic activation of all of the languages that a speaker uses and interactions across languages shape not only language processing but also new learning. We identify sources of variation among learners and across the contexts in which learning occurs to consider how language learning might reflect multilingual experience.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2023

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abbas, N., Degani, T., & Prior, A. (2021). Equal Opportunity Interference: Both L1 and L2 Influence L3 Morpho-syntactic Processing. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, article 673535.Google Scholar
Abutalebi, J., & Green, D. (2007). Bilingual Language Production: The Neurocognition of Language Representation and Control. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 20(3), 242275.Google Scholar
Abutalebi, J., Canini, M., Della Rosa, P. A., et al. (2014). Bilingualism Protects Anterior Temporal Lobe Integrity in Aging. Neurobiology of Aging, 35(9), 21262133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Antoniou, M., Gunasekera, G. M., & Wong, P. C. (2013). Foreign Language Training as Cognitive Therapy for Age-Related Cognitive Decline: A Hypothesis for Future Research. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 37(10), 26892698.Google Scholar
Antoniou, M., Liang, E., Ettlinger, M., & Wong, P. C. (2015). The Bilingual Advantage in Phonetic Learning. Bilingualism: Language, and Cognition, 18(4), 683.Google Scholar
Au, T. K., Knightly, L. M., Jun, S.-A., & Oh, J. S. (2002). Overhearing a Language During Childhood. Psychological Science, 13(3), 238243.Google Scholar
Bak, T. H., Long, M. R., Vega-Mendoza, M., & Sorace, A. (2016). Novelty, Challenge, and Practice: The Impact of Intensive Language Learning on Attentional Functions. PloS One, 11(4), article E0153485.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barak, L., Degani, T., & Novogrodsky, R. (2022). Influences of Bilingualism and Developmental Language Disorder on How Children Learn and Process Words. Developmental Psychology, 58(5), 821834.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bartolotti, J., Marian, V., Schroeder, S., & Shook, A. (2011). Bilingualism and Inhibitory Control Influence Statistical Learning of Novel Word Forms. Frontiers in Psychology, 2. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baum, S., & Titone, D. (2014). Moving Toward a Neuroplasticity View of Bilingualism, Executive Control, and Aging. Applied Psycholinguistics, 35(5), 857894.Google Scholar
Baus, C., Costa, A., & Carreiras, M. (2013). On the Effects of Second Language Immersion on First Language Production. Acta Psychologica, 142(3), 402409.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beatty-Martínez, A. L., Navarro-Torres, C. A., & Dussias, P. E. (2020a). Codeswitching: A Bilingual Toolkit for Opportunistic Speech Planning. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01699.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beatty-Martínez, A. L., Navarro-Torres, C. A., Dussias, P. E., et al. (2020b). Interactional Context Mediates the Consequences of Bilingualism for Language and Cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 46, 10221047.Google Scholar
Ben Zion, D., Nevat, M., Prior, A. & Bitan, T. (2019). Prior Knowledge Predicts Early Consolidation in Second Language Learning. Frontiers in Psychology, 10. http://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02312.Google Scholar
Berken, J. A., Chai, X., Chen, J.-K., Gracco, V. L., & Klein, D. (2016). Effects of Early and Late Bilingualism on Resting-State Functional Connectivity. Journal of Neuroscience, 36, 11651172.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E. (2017). The Bilingual Adaptation: How Minds Accommodate Experience. Psychological Bulletin, 143(3), 233262.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bialystok, E., & Kroll, J. F. (2017). The Neurobiology of Language: Looking Beyond Monolinguals – A Special Issue of Biolinguistics Celebrating the 50-Year Anniversary of Eric Lenneberg’s Biological Foundations of Language. Biolinguistics, 11, 339352.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Craik, F. I., & Freedman, M. (2007). Bilingualism as a Protection against the Onset of Symptoms of Dementia. Neuropsychologia, 45(2), 459464.Google Scholar
Bialystok, E., Abutalebi, J., Bak, T. H., Burke, D. M., & Kroll, J. F. (2016). Aging in Two Languages: Implications for Public Health. Ageing Research Reviews, 27, 5660.Google Scholar
Bice, K., & Kroll, J. F. (2015). Native Language Change during Early Stages of Second Language Learning. NeuroReport, 26(16), 966971.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bice, K., & Kroll, J. F. (2019). English Only? Monolinguals in Linguistically Diverse Contexts Have an Edge in Language Learning. Brain and Language, 196, article 104644.Google Scholar
Bice, K., & Kroll, J. F. (2021). Grammatical Processing in Two Languages: How Individual Differences in Language Experience and Cognitive Abilities Shape Comprehension in Heritage Bilinguals. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 58, article 100963.Google Scholar
Birdsong, D. (2005). Interpreting Age Effects in Second Language Acquisition. In Kroll, J. F. and De Groot, A. M. B. (Eds.), Handbook of Bilingualism: Psycholinguistic Approaches (pp. 109127). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Birdsong, D. (2018). Plasticity, Variability and Age in Second Language Acquisition and Bilingualism. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 81. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00081Google Scholar
Bjork, R. A., & Kroll, J. F. (2015). Desirable Difficulties in Vocabulary Learning. The American Journal of Psychology, 128(2), 241252.Google Scholar
Bogulski, C. A., Bice, K., & Kroll, J. F. (2019). Bilingualism as a Desirable Difficulty: Advantages in Word Learning Depend on Regulation of the Dominant Language. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22(5), 10521067.Google Scholar
Bordag, D., Gor, K., & Opitz, A. (2022). Ontogenesis Model of the L2 Lexical Representation. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 25, 185201.Google Scholar
Borsa, V. M., Perani, D., Della Rosa, P. A., et al. (2018). Bilingualism and Healthy Aging: Aging Effects and Neural Maintenance. Neuropsychologia, 111, 5161.Google Scholar
Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Keys, B. A., et al. (2001). Context Processing in Older Adults: Evidence for a Theory Relating Cognitive Control to Neurobiology in Healthy Aging. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130(4), 746763.Google Scholar
Brice, H., Frost, S., Bick, A. S., et al. (2021). Tracking Second Language Immersion Across Time: Evidence from a Bi-directional Longitudinal Cross-Linguistic FMRI Study. Neuropsychologia, article 107796.Google Scholar
Caffarra, S., Molinaro, N., Davidson, D., & Carreiras, M. (2015). Second Language Syntactic Processing Revealed through Event-Related Potentials: An Empirical Review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 51, 3147.Google Scholar
Degani, T., & Goldberg, M. (2019). How Individual Differences Affect Learning of Translation-Ambiguous Vocabulary? Language Learning, 69(3), 600651.Google Scholar
DeLuca, V., Rothman, J., Bialystok, E., & Pliatsikas, C. (2019). Redefining Bilingualism as a Spectrum of Experiences that Differentially Affects Brain Structure and Function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 116(15), 75657574.Google Scholar
DeLuca, V., Segaert, K., Mazaheri, A., & Krott, A. (2020). Understanding Bilingual Brain Function and Structure Changes? U Bet! A Unified Bilingual Experience Trajectory Model. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 56, article 100930.Google Scholar
Dijkstra, T. (2005). Bilingual Visual Word Recognition and Lexical Access. In Kroll, J. F. & De Groot, A. M. B. (Eds.), Handbook of Bilingualism: Psycholinguistic Approaches (pp. 179201). Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Elias, M., & Degani, T. (2022). Cross-Language Interactions during Novel Word Learning: The Contribution of Form Similarity and Participant Characteristics. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 25(4), 548565.Google Scholar
Ferjan Ramírez, N., Ramírez, R. R., Clarke, M., Taulu, S., & Kuhl, P. K. (2017). Speech Discrimination in 11-Month-Old Bilingual and Monolingual Infants: A Magnetoencephalography Study. Developmental Science, 20(1), article e12427.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Festman, J. (2021). Learning and Processing Multiple Languages: The More the Easier? Language Learning, 71, 121162.Google Scholar
Flege, J. E., Munro, M. J., & MacKay, I. R. (1995). Factors Affecting Strength of Perceived Foreign Accent in a Second Language. The Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 97(5), 31253134.Google Scholar
Fricke, M., Zirnstein, M., Navarro-Torres, C., & Kroll, J. F. (2019). Bilingualism Reveals Fundamental Variation in Language Processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22(1), 200207.Google Scholar
González Alonso, J., Alemán Bañón, J. DeLuca, V., et al. (2020). Event Related Potentials at Initial Exposure in Third Language Acquisition: Implications from an Artificial Mini-Grammar Study. Journal of Neurolinguistics, 56, article 100939.Google Scholar
Green, D. W. (1998). Mental Control of the Bilingual Lexico-Semantic System. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1(2), 6781.Google Scholar
Green, D. W., & Abutalebi, J. (2013). Language Control in Bilinguals: The Adaptive Control Hypothesis. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 515530.Google Scholar
Gullifer, J. W., & Titone, D. (2020). Characterizing the Social Diversity of Bilingualism Using Language Entropy. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(2), 283294.Google Scholar
Gullifer, J. W., Chai, X. J., Whitford, V., et al. (2018). Bilingual Experience and Resting-State Brain Connectivity: Impacts of L2 Age of Acquisition and Social Diversity of Language Use on Control Networks. Neuropsychologia, 117, 123134.Google Scholar
Guo, T., Liu, H., Misra, M., & Kroll, J.F. (2011). Local and Global Inhibition in Bilingual Word Production: fMRI Evidence from Chinese–English Bilinguals. NeuroImage, 56, 23002309.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hirosh, Z., & Degani, T. (2018). Direct and Indirect Effects of Multilingualism on Novel Language Learning: An Integrative Review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(3), 892916.Google Scholar
Hirosh, Z., & Degani, T. (2021). Novel Word Learning among Bilinguals Can Be Better through the (Dominant) First Language Than through the Second Language. Language Learning, 71(4), 10441084.Google Scholar
Hofstetter, S., Friedmann, N., & Assaf, Y. (2017). Rapid Language-Related Plasticity: microstructural Changes in the Cortex after a Short Session of New Word Learning. Brain Structure and Function, 222(3), 12311241.Google Scholar
Ingvalson, E. M., Nowicki, C., Zong, A., & Wong, P. (2017). Non-native Speech Learning in Older Adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 148.Google Scholar
Jeong, H., Li, P., Suzuki, W., Sugiura, M., & Kawashima, R. (2021). Neural Mechanisms of Language Learning from Social Contexts. Brain and Language, 212, article 104874.Google Scholar
Johnson, J. S., & Newport, E. L. (1989). Critical Period Effects in Second Language Learning: The Influence of Maturational State on the Acquisition of English as a Second Language. Cognitive Psychology, 21(1), 6099.Google Scholar
Kaushanskaya, M., & Marian, V. (2009). The Bilingual Advantage in Novel Word Learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 16(4), 705710.Google Scholar
Kaushanskaya, M., Yoo, J., & Van Hecke, S. (2013). Word Learning in Adults with Second Language Experience: Effects of Phonological and Referent Familiarity. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 56, 667678.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Understanding the Consequences of Bilingualism for Language Processing and Cognition. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25, 497514.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F., & Navarro-Torres, C. (2018). Bilingualism. In Wixted, J. (Ed.), The Stevens’ Handbook of Experimental Psychology and Cognitive Neuroscience (pp. 245274). Hoboken: Wiley/Blackwell.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F., & Stewart, E. (1994). Category Interference in Translation and Picture Naming: Evidence for Asymmetric Connection between Bilingual Memory Representations. Journal of Memory and Language, 33(2), 149174.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Bobb, S. C., & Hoshino, N. (2014). Two Languages in Mind: Bilingualism as a Tool to Investigate Language, Cognition, and the Brain. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 23(3), 159163.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Dussias, P. E., Bice, K., & Perrotti, L. (2015). Bilingualism, Mind, and Brain. Annual Review of Linguistics, 1(1), 377394.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Takahesu Tabori, A., & Navarro-Torres, C.A. (2021). Capturing the Variation in Language Experience to Understand Language Processing and Learning. In Kail, M. & Isel, F. (Eds.), Language, Plasticity and Learning: Developmental Issues. Special issue, Language, Interaction, and Acquisition, 12, 82109.Google Scholar
Kroll, J. F., Bice, K., Botezatu, M. R., & Zirnstein, M. (2022). On the Dynamics of Lexical Access in Two or More Languages. In Gleitman, L., Papafragou, A., & Trueswell, J. (Eds). Handbook of the Mental Lexicon (pp. 583597). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Kuhl, P. K. (2004). Early Language Acquisition: Cracking the Speech Code. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 5(11), 831843.Google Scholar
Lev-Ari, S. (2018). Social Network Size Can Influence Linguistic Malleability and the Propagation of Linguistic Change. Cognition, 176, 3139.Google Scholar
Lev‐Ari, S., & Sebanz, N. (2020). Interacting with Multiple Partners Improves Communication Skills. Cognitive Science, 44(4), E12.Google Scholar
Lev-Ari, S., & Shao, Z. (2017). How Social Network Heterogeneity Facilitates Lexical Access and Lexical Prediction. Memory & Cognition, 45(3), 528538.Google Scholar
Linck, J. A., Kroll, J. F., & Sunderman, G. (2009). Losing Access to the Native Language While Immersed in a Second Language: Evidence for the Role of Inhibition in Second-Language Learning. Psychological Science, 20(12), 15071515.Google Scholar
Luk, G., & Bialystok, E. (2013). Bilingualism Is Not a Categorical Variable: Interaction between Language Proficiency and Usage. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 25(5), 605621.Google Scholar
Marian, V., & Hayakawa, S. (2020). Measuring Bilingualism: The Quest for a “Bilingualism Quotient.Applied Psycholinguistics, 42, 527548.Google Scholar
Marian, V., & Sprivey, M. (2003). Competing Activation in Bilingual Language Processing: Within- and Between-Language Competition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 6(2), 97115.Google Scholar
Muscalu, L., & Smiley, P. (2019). The Illusory Benefit of Cognates: Lexical Facilitation Followed by Sublexical Interference in a Word Typing Task. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22(4), 848865.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McLaughlin, J., Osterhout, L., & Kim, A. (2004). Neural Correlates of Second-Language Word Learning: Minimal Instruction Produces Rapid Change. Nature Neuroscience, 7(7), 703704.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McLaughlin, J., Tanner, D., Pitkänen, I., et al. (2010). Brain Potentials Reveal Discrete Stages of L2 Grammatical Learning. Language Learning, 60, 123150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morgan-Short, K., Steinhauer, K., Sanz, C., & Ullman, M.T. (2012). Explicit and Implicit Second Language Training Differentially Affects the Achievement of Native-Like Brain Activation Patterns. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 24, 933947.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Oh, J. S., Au, T. K. F., Jun, S. A., & Lee, R. M. (2019). Childhood Language Memory in Adult Heritage Language (Re) Learners. In Schmid, M.S. & Köpke, B. (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Language Attrition. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pakulak, E., & Neville, H. J. (2010). Proficiency Differences in Syntactic Processing of Monolingual Native Speakers Indexed by Event-Related Potentials. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(12), 2728–44.Google Scholar
Perani, D., & Abutalebi, J. (2005). The Neural Basis of First and Second Language Processing. Cognitive Neuroscience, 15(2), 202206.Google Scholar
Petitto, L. A., Berens, M. S., Kovelman, I., et al. (2012). The “Perceptual Wedge Hypothesis” as the Basis for Bilingual Babies’ Phonetic Processing Advantage: New Insights from FNIRS Brain Imaging. Brain and Language, 121(2), 130143.Google Scholar
Pierce, L. J., Chen, J.-K., Delcenserie, A., Genesee, F., & Klein, D. (2015). Past Experience Shapes Ongoing Neural Patterns for Language. Nature Communications, 6(1), article 10073.Google Scholar
Pliatsikas, C. (2019). Understanding Structural Plasticity in the Bilingual Brain: The Dynamic Restructuring Model. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 113.Google Scholar
Pliatsikas, C., DeLuca, V., & Voits, T. (2020). The Many Shades of Bilingualism: language Experiences Modulate Adaptations in Brain Structure. Language Learning, 70(S2), 133149.Google Scholar
Polinsky, M., & Scontras, G. (2020). Understanding Heritage Languages. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(1), 420.Google Scholar
Pons, F., Bosch, L., & Lewkowicz, D. J. (2015). Bilingualism Modulates Infants’ Selective Attention to the Mouth of a Talking Face. Psychological Science, 26(4), 490498.Google Scholar
Pot, A., Keijzer, M., & De Bot, K. (2018). Intensity of Multilingual Language Use Predicts Cognitive Performance in Some Multilingual Older Adults. Brain Sciences, 8(5), 92.Google Scholar
Prat, C. S., Yamasaki, B. L., Kluender, R. A., & Stocco, A. (2016). Resting-State QEEG Predicts Rate of Second Language Learning in Adults. Brain and Language, 157, 4450.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pulido, M. F. (2021). Native Language Inhibition Predicts More Successful Second Language Learning: Evidence of Two ERP Pathways During Learning. Neuropsychologia, 152, article 107732.Google Scholar
Pulido, M. F., & Dussias, P. E. (2020). Desirable Difficulties While Learning Collocations in a Second Language: Conditions that Induce L1 Interference Improve Learning. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 23(3), 652667.Google Scholar
Roncaglia-Denissen, M. P., & Kotz, S. A. (2016). What Does Neuroimaging Tell Us About Morphosyntactic Processing in the Brain of Second Language Learners? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 19(4), 665673.Google Scholar
Rothman, J. (2011). L3 Syntactic Transfer Selectivity and Typological Determinacy: The Typological Primacy Model. Second Language Research, 27, 107128.Google Scholar
Rothman, J., González Alonso, J., & Puig-Mayenco, E. (2019). Third Language Acquisition and Linguistic Transfer (vol. 163). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Schmid, M. S. (2010). Languages at Play: The Relevance of L1 Attrition to the Study of Bilingualism. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 13(1), 17.Google Scholar
Schwartz, M., Kahn-Horwitz, J., & Share, D. L. (2014). Orthographic Learning and Self-Teaching in a Bilingual and Biliterate Context. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 117, 4558.Google Scholar
Steinhauer, K. (2014). Event-Related Potentials (ERPs) in Second Language Research: A Brief Introduction to the Technique, a Selected Review, and an Invitation to Reconsider Critical Periods in L2. Applied Linguistics, 35, 393417.Google Scholar
Surrain, S., & Luk, G. (2019). Describing Bilinguals: A Systematic Review of Labels and Descriptions Used in the Literature Between 2005–2015. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 22(2), 401415.Google Scholar
Tanner, D., & Van Hell, J. G. (2014). ERPs Reveal Individual Differences in Morphosyntactic Processing. Neuropsychologia, 56(April), 289301.Google Scholar
Veroude, K., Norris, D. G., Shumskaya, E., Gullberg, M., & Indefrey, P. (2010). Functional Connectivity Between Brain Regions Involved in Learning Words of a New Language. Brain and Language, 113(1), 2127.Google Scholar
Weikum, W. M., Vouloumanos, A., Navarra, J., et al. (2007). Visual Language Discrimination in Infancy. Science, 316(5828), 1159.Google Scholar
Werker, J. F. (2018). Perceptual Beginnings to Language Acquisition. Applied Psycholinguistics, 39(4), 703728.Google Scholar
Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (1984). Cross-Language Speech Perception: Evidence for Perceptual Reorganization During the First Year of Life. Infant Behavior and Development, 7(1), 4963.Google Scholar
Wong, P., Ou, J., Pang, C. W., et al. (2019). Foreign Language Learning as Potential Treatment for Mild Cognitive Impairment. Hong Kong Medical Journal, 7(5), S41S43.Google Scholar
Zirnstein, M., Bice, K., & Kroll, J. F. (2019). Variation in Language Experience Shapes the Consequences of Bilingualism. In Sekerina, I. A., Spradlin, L., & Valian, V. V. (Eds.), Bilingualism and Executive Function: An Interdisciplinary Approach (pp. 3548). Amsterdam: John Benjamins.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×