Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-7cvxr Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-18T08:00:17.041Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

10 - Intelligence

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 May 2019

Robert J. Sternberg
Affiliation:
Cornell University, New York
Wade E. Pickren
Affiliation:
Ithaca College, New York
Get access

Summary

This chapter reviews the intellectual history of the study of intelligence. It provides an overview of the field today. Then it reviews the early history of the field, dating back to Sir Francis Galton and then Albert Binet, Charles Spearman, and their successors. It discusses the theory of general intelligence and competitors to the theory such as the structure-of-intellect model proposed by J. P. Guilford. The chapter also describes in some detail various approaches to testing intelligence. It further describes the work of Jean Piaget, as well as biological and cultural views on intelligence. It then describes the theory of multiple intelligences and the theory of successful intelligence. It further describes the Flynn effect. Then it draws conclusions.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2019

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ang, S., Van Dyne, L., & Tan, M. L. (Eds.). (2011). Cultural intelligence. In Sternberg, R. J. & Kaufman, S. B. (Eds.), Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence (pp. 582602). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Atran, S. (1999). Itzaj Maya folkbiological taxonomy: Cognitive universals and cultural particulars. In Medin, D. L. & Atran, S. (Eds.), Folkbiology (pp. 119213). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Baltes, P. B., Dittmann-Kohli, F., & Dixon, R. A. (1984). New perspectives on the development of intelligence in adulthood: Toward a dual-process conception and a model of selective optimization with compensation. In Baltes, P. B. & Brim, O. B. Jr. (Eds.), Life-Span Development and Behavior (Vol. 6, pp. 3376). New York: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Barnett, S. M., & Ceci, S. J. (2005). The role of transferable knowledge in intelligence. In Sternberg, R. J. & Pretz, J. E. (Eds.), Cognition and Intelligence (pp. 208224). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Benisz, M., Dumont, R., & Willis, J. O. (2015). From psychometric testing to clinical assessment: Personalities, ideas, and events that shaped David Wechsler’s views of intelligence and its assessment. In Goldstein, S., Princiotta, D., & Naglieri, J. A. (Eds.), Handbook of Intelligence (pp. 163180). New York: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Binet, A., & Simon, T. (1916). The Development of Intelligence in Children. Translated by Kite, E. S.. Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins.Google Scholar
Blackwell, L. S., Trzesniewski, K. H., & Dweck, C. S. (2007). Implicit theories of intelligence predict achievement across an adolescent transition: A longitudinal study and an intervention. Child Development, 78, 246263.Google Scholar
Boyatzis, R. E., Gaskin, J., & Wei, H. (2015). Emotional and social intelligence and behavior. In Goldstein, S., Princiotta, D., & Naglieri, J. A. (Eds.), Handbook of Intelligence (pp. 243262). New York: Springer.Google Scholar
Carraher, T. N., Carraher, D., & Schliemann, A. D. (1985). Mathematics in the streets and in the school. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 3, 2129.Google Scholar
Carroll, J. B. (1993). Human Cognitive Abilities: A Survey of Factor-Analytic Studies. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Cattell, R. B. (1971). Abilities: Their Structure, Growth, and Action. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Ceci, S. J., & Bronfenbrenner, U. (1985). Don’t forget to take the cupcakes out of the oven: Strategic time-monitoring, prospective memory and context. Child Development, 56, 175190.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ceci, S. J., & Roazzi, A. (1994). The effects of context on cognition: Postcards from Brazil. In Sternberg, R. J. & Wagner, R. K. (Eds.), Mind in Context: Interactionist Perspectives on Human Intelligence (pp. 74101). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Coley, J. D., Medin, D. L., Proffitt, J. B., Lynch, E., & Atran, S. (1999). Inductive reasoning in folkbiological thought. In Medin, D. L. & Atran, S. (Eds.), Folkbiology (pp. 205232). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Colom, R., Rebollo, I., Palacios, A., Juan-Espinosa, M., & Kyllonen, P. C. (2004). Working memory is (almost) perfectly predicted by g. Intelligence, 32, 277296.Google Scholar
Conway, A. R. A., Getz, S. J., Macnamara, B., & Engel de Abreu, P. M. J. (2011). Working memory and intelligence. In Sternberg, R. J. & Kaufman, S. B. (Eds.), Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence (pp. 394418). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Daneman, M., & Carpenter, P. A. (1983). Individual differences in integrating information between and within sentences. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 9, 561583.Google Scholar
Deary, I. J., Johnson, W., & Houlihan, L. M. (2009). Genetic foundations of human intelligence. Human Genetics, 126, 215232.Google Scholar
Dweck, C. S. (2007). Mindset: The New Psychology of Success. New York: Ballantine Books.Google Scholar
Flynn, J. R. (2012). Are We Getting Smarter? Rising IQ in the Twenty-First Century. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Galton, F. (1883). Inquiries into Human Faculty and Its Development. London: Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gardner, H. (2011). Frames of Mind: The Theory of Multiple Intelligences. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Gladwin, T. (1970). East Is a Big Bird. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goleman, D. (1995). Emotional Intelligence. New York: Bantam Books.Google Scholar
Goleman, D. (1998). Working with Emotional Intelligence. New York: Bantam Books.Google Scholar
Goleman, D. (2007). Social Intelligence. New York: Bantam Books.Google Scholar
Greenfield, P. M. (1997). You can’t take it with you: Why abilities assessments don’t cross cultures. American Psychologist, 52, 11151124.Google Scholar
Grigorenko, E. L., Geissler, P. W., Prince, R., Okatcha, F., Nokes, C., Kenny, D. A., Bundy, D. A., & Sternberg, R. J. (2001). The organization of Luo conceptions of intelligence: A study of implicit theories in a Kenyan village. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 25, 367378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grigorenko, E. L., Jarvin, L., & Sternberg, R. J. (2002). School–based tests of the triarchic theory of intelligence: Three settings, three samples, three syllabi. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 27, 167208.Google Scholar
Guilford, J. P. (1967). The Nature of Human Intelligence. New York: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Haier, R. J., & Jung, R. E. (2007). Beautiful minds (i.e., brains) and the neural basis of intelligence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 174178.Google Scholar
Haier, R. J., Siegel, B., Tang, C., Abel, L., & Buchsbaum, M. S. (1992). Intelligence and changes in regional cerebral glucose metabolic rate following learning. Intelligence, 16, 415426.Google Scholar
Hambrick, D. Z., Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2005). The role of working memory in higher-level cognition. In Sternberg, R. J. & Pretz, J. E. (Eds.), Cognition and Intelligence (pp. 104121). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Helms-Lorenz, M., Van de Vijver, F. J. R., & Poortinga, Y. H. (2003). Cross-cultural differences in cognitive performance and Spearman’s hypothesis: g or c? Intelligence, 31, 929.Google Scholar
Horn, J. L., & Knapp, J. R. (1973). On the subjective character of Guilford’s structure-of-intellect model. Psychological Bulletin, 80, 3343.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hunt, E. B. (1978). Mechanics of verbal ability. Psychological Review, 85, 109130.Google Scholar
Hunt, E. B. (1980). Intelligence as an information-processing concept. British Journal of Psychology, 71, 449474.Google Scholar
Jensen, A. R. (1982). The chronometry of intelligence. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), Advances in the Psychology of Human Intelligence (Vol. 1, pp. 255310). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Jung, R. E., & Haier, R. J. (2007). The parieto-frontal integration theory (P-FIT) of intelligence: Converging neuroimaging evidence. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 135154.Google Scholar
Kihlstrom, J. F., & Cantor, N. (2000). Social intelligence. In Sternberg, R. J. (Ed.), Handbook of Intelligence (pp. 359379). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lehman, D. R., Chiu, C. Y. P., & Schaller, M. (2004). Psychology and culture. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 689714.Google Scholar
Lohman, D. F., & Lakin, J. M. (2011). Intelligence and reasoning. In Sternberg, R. J. & Kaufman, S. B. (Eds.), Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence (pp. 419441). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Luria, A. R. (1973). The Working Brain. New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Luria, A. R. (1976). Cognitive Development: Its Cultural and Social Foundations. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Luria, A. R. (1980). Higher Cortical Functions in Man (2nd ed.). New York: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Mandelman, S. D., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2011). Intelligence: Genes, environments, and their interactions. In Sternberg, R. J. & Kaufman, S. B. (Eds.), Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence (pp. 85106). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Markus, H. R., & Conner, A. (2014). Clash! How to Survive in a Multicultural World. New York: Plume.Google Scholar
Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (1991). Culture and the self: Implications for cognition, emotion, and motivation. Psychological Review, 98, 224253.Google Scholar
Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., Caruso, D. R., & Cherkasskiy, L. (2011). Emotional intelligence. In Sternberg, R. J. & Kaufman, S. (Eds.), Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence (pp. 528549). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Medin, D. L., & Atran, S. (Eds.) (1999). Folkbiology. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Mulholland, T. M., Pellegrino, J. W., & Glaser, R. (1980). Components of geometric analogy solution. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 252284.Google Scholar
Neçka, E., & Orzechowski, J. (2005). Higher-order cognition and intelligence. In Sternberg, R. J. & Pretz, J. E. (Eds.), Cognition and Intelligence (pp. 122141). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Nettelbeck, T., & Young, R. (1996). Intelligence and savant syndrome: Is the whole greater than the sum of the fragments? Intelligence, 22, 4967.Google Scholar
Neubauer, A. C., & Fink, A. (2005). Basic information processing and the psychophysiology of intelligence. In Sternberg, R. J. & Pretz, J. E. (Eds.), Cognition and Intelligence (pp. 6887). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Neubauer, A. C., & Fink, A. (2009). Intelligence and neural efficiency: Measures of brain activation versus measures of functional connectivity in the brain. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 33, 10041023.Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E. (2003). The Geography of Thought. New York: Free Press.Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E., & Masuda, T. (2003). Culture and point of view. PNAS, 100, 1116311170.Google Scholar
Nisbett, R. E., & Miyamoto, Y. (2005). The influence of culture: Holistic versus analytic perception. Trends in Cognitive Science, 9, 467473.Google Scholar
Okagaki, L., & Sternberg, R. J. (1993). Parental beliefs and children’s school performance. Child Development, 64, 3656.Google Scholar
Pellegrino, J. W., & Glaser, R. (1979). Cognitive correlates and components in the analysis of individual differences. Intelligence, 3, 187215.Google Scholar
Piaget, J. (1972). The Psychology of Intelligence. Totowa, NJ: Littlefield, Adams.Google Scholar
Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., Knopik, V. S., & Neiderhiser, J. M. (2012). Behavioral Genetics (6th ed.). New York: Worth.Google Scholar
Roid, G. H. (2003). Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scales, Fifth Edition: Technical Manual. Itasca, IL: Riverside.Google Scholar
Schliemann, A. D., & Magalhües, V. P. (1990). Proportional reasoning: From shops, to kitchens, laboratories, and, hopefully, schools. Proceedings of the Fourteenth International Conference for the Psychology of Mathematics Education. Oaxtepec, Mexico.Google Scholar
Spearman, C. (1904). “General intelligence,” objectively determined and measured. American Journal of Psychology, 15, 201293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spearman, C. (1923). The Nature of Intelligence and the Principles of Cognition. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Spearman, C. (1927). The Abilities of Man. London: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Stern, W. (1912). Psychologische Methoden der Intelligenz-Prüfung. Leipzig: Barth.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1977). Intelligence, Information Processing, and Analogical Reasoning: The Componential Analysis of Human Abilities. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1983). Components of human intelligence. Cognition, 15, 148.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1985a). Beyond IQ: A Triarchic Theory of Human Intelligence. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1985b). Implicit theories of intelligence, creativity, and wisdom. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49, 607627.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1990). Metaphors of Mind. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (2003). Wisdom, Intelligence, and Creativity Synthesized. New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (2004). Culture and intelligence. American Psychologist, 59, 325338.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (2010). College Admissions for the 21st Century. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (2011). The theory of successful intelligence. In Sternberg, R. J. & Kaufman, S. K. (Eds.), Cambridge Handbook of Intelligence (pp. 504527). New York: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (2016). What Universities Can Be. Ithaca, NY: Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., Conway, B. E., Ketron, J. L., & Bernstein, M. (1981). People’s conceptions of intelligence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 41, 3755.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., & Grigorenko, E. L. (Eds.) (2002). The General Factor of Intelligence: How General Is It? New York: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2007). Ability testing across cultures. In Suzuki, L. (Ed.), Handbook of Multicultural Assessment (3rd ed.) (pp. 449470). New York: Jossey Bass.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., Grigorenko, E. L., Ferrari, M., & Clinkenbeard, P. (1999). A triarchic analysis of an aptitude–treatment interaction. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 15, 111.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., Jarvin, L., Birney, D., Naples, A., Stemler, S., Newman, T., Otterbach, R., Randi, J., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2014). Testing the theory of successful intelligence in teaching grade 4 language arts, mathematics, and science. Journal of Educational Psychology, 106, 881899.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., Nokes, K., Geissler, P. W., Prince, R., Okatcha, F., Bundy, D. A., & Grigorenko, E. L. (2001). The relationship between academic and practical intelligence: A case study in Kenya. Intelligence, 29, 401418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., & Smith, C. (1985). Social intelligence and decoding skills in nonverbal communication. Social Cognition, 2, 168192.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J., Torff, B., & Grigorenko, E. L. (1998). Teaching triarchically improves school achievement. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90, 374384.Google Scholar
Thomson, G. H. (1916). A hierarchy without a general factor. British Journal of Psychology, 1904–1920, 8, 271281.Google Scholar
Thorndike, E. L. (1911). Animal Intelligence: Experimental Studies. New York: Macmillan.Google Scholar
Thurstone, L. L. (1924). The Nature of Intelligence. London: Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner & Co.Google Scholar
Thurstone, L. L. (1938). Primary Mental Abilities. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Turner, M. L., & Engle, R. W. (1989). Is working-memory capacity task dependent? Journal of Memory and Language, 28, 127154.Google Scholar
Vernon, P. E. (1950). The Structure of Human Abilities. London: Methuen.Google Scholar
Wagner, D. A. (1978). Memories of Morocco: The influence of age, schooling, and environment on memory. Cognitive Psychology, 10, 128.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (2002). Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence (3rd ed.). San Antonio, TX: Psychological Corporation.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (2003). Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children—fourth edition. San Antonio, TX: Pearson.Google Scholar
Wechsler, D. (2008). Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale—fourth edition. San Antonio, TX: Pearson.Google Scholar
Wissler, C. D. (1901). The correlation of mental and physical tests. Psychological Review, Monograph Supplements, 3(6), 1162.Google Scholar
Yang, S., & Sternberg, R. J. (1997). Taiwanese Chinese people’s conceptions of intelligence. Intelligence, 25, 2136.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×