Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T13:37:55.821Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

26 - The Future-Directed Functions of the Imagination: From Prediction to Metaforesight

from Part III - Intentionality-Based Forms of the Imagination

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 May 2020

Anna Abraham
Affiliation:
University of Georgia
Get access

Summary

One of the fundamental roles of human imagination is to enable the representation of possible future events. Here, we survey some of the most critical abilities that this foresight supports: anticipating future emotions, setting and pursuing goals, preparing for threats, deliberately acquiring skills and knowledge, and intentionally shaping the future environment. Furthermore, we outline how metacognition bolsters human capacities even further by enabling people to reflect on and compensate for the natural limits of their foresight. For example, humans make contingency plans because they appreciate that their initial predictions may turn out to be wrong. We suggest that the processes involved in monitoring, controlling, and ultimately augmenting future-oriented imagination represent an important and understudied parallel of “metamemory” that should be called “metaforesight.”

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Ainslie, G. (1975). Specious Reward: A Behavioral Theory of Impulsiveness and Impulse Control. Psychological Bulletin, 82, 463496.Google Scholar
Balcomb, F. K., and Gerken, L. A. (2008). Three-Year-Old Children Can Access Their Own Memory to Guide Responses on a Visual Matching Task. Developmental Science, 11, 750760.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barnier, A. J., Sutton, J., Harris, C. B., and Wilson, R. A. (2008). A Conceptual and Empirical Framework for the Social Distribution of Cognition: The Case of Memory. Cognitive Systems Research, 9, 3351.Google Scholar
Barsalou, L. W. (2009). Simulation, Situated Conceptualization, and Prediction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364, 12811289.Google Scholar
Bateson, M., Brilot, B., and Nettle, D. (2011). Anxiety: An Evolutionary Approach. The Canadian Journal of Psychiatry, 56, 707715.Google Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., and Tierney, J. (2011). Willpower: Rediscovering the Greatest Human Strength. New York, NY: Penguin Press.Google Scholar
Baxter, W. T. (1989). Early Accounting: The Tally and the Checkerboard. The Accounting Historians Journal, 16, 4383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beck, S. R., Robinson, E. J., Carroll, D. J., and Apperly, I. A. (2006). Children’s Thinking about Counterfactuals and Future Hypotheticals as Possibilities. Child Development, 77, 413426.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berns, G. S., Chappelow, J., Cekic, M., et al. (2006). Neurobiological Substrates of Dread. Science, 312, 754758.Google Scholar
Bischof-Köhler, D. (1985). Zur Phylogenese menschlicher Motivation. In Eckensberger, L. H, Baltes, M. M (eds.), Emotion und Reflexivität. Munich, Germany: Urban & Schwarzenberg, 347.Google Scholar
Bjork, R. A. (1994). Memory and Metamemory Considerations in the Training of Human Beings. In Metcalfe, J and Shimamura, A (eds.), Metacognition: Knowing about Knowing. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 185205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyer, P. (2008). Evolutionary Economics of Mental Time Travel? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 219224.Google Scholar
Brandimonte, M. A., Einstein, G. O., and McDaniel, M. A. (2014). Prospective Memory: Theory and Applications. New York, NY: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Bulley, A. (2018). The History and Future of Human Prospection. Evolutionary Studies in Imaginative Culture, 1, 7593.Google Scholar
Bulley, A., Henry, J., and Suddendorf, T. (2016). Prospection and the Present Moment: The Role of Episodic Foresight in Intertemporal Choices between Immediate and Delayed Rewards. Review of General Psychology, 20, 2947.Google Scholar
Bulley, A., Henry, J., and Suddendorf, T. (2017). Thinking about Threats: Memory and Prospection in Human Threat Management. Consciousness and Cognition, 49, 5369.Google Scholar
Byrne, R. M. J. (2016). Counterfactual Thought. Annual Review of Psychology, 67, 135157.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2006). Language, Embodiment, and the Cognitive Niche. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 370374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clark, A. (2008). Supersizing the Mind: Embodiment, Action, and Cognitive Extension. Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780195333213.001.0001.Google Scholar
Clark, A. (2015). Surfing Uncertainty: Prediction, Action, and the Embodied Mind. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Clark, A., and Chalmers, D. (1998). The Extended Mind. Analysis, 58, 719.Google Scholar
Clarke, K. C. (2013). What is the World’s Oldest Map? The Cartographic Journal, 50, 136143.Google Scholar
Coffman, D. D. (1990). Effects of Mental Practice, Physical Practice, and Knowledge of Results on Piano Performance. Journal of Research in Music Education, 38(3), 187196.Google Scholar
Corballis, M. C. (2013). Wandering Tales: Evolutionary Origins of Mental Time Travel and Language. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 485.Google Scholar
Craik, K. J. W. (1943). The Nature of Explanation. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
D’Argembeau, A. (2016). The Role of Personal Goals in Future-Oriented Mental Time Travel. In Michaelian, K, Klein, S. B., and Szpunar, K. K. (eds.), Seeing the Future: Theoretical Perspectives on Future-Oriented Mental Time Travel. New York, NY: Oxford University Press. doi:10.1093/acprof:oso/9780190241537.003.0010.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (1994). Descartes’ Error: Emotion, Reason, and the Human Brain. New York, NY: Putnam Publishing.Google Scholar
Davis, J. T. M., Cullen, E., and Suddendorf, T. (2016 ). Understanding Deliberate Practice in Preschool Aged Children. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(2), 361380.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1998). Unweaving the Rainbow: Science, Delusion and the Appetite for Wonder. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin Harcourt.Google Scholar
Devereux, C. L., Whittingham, M. J., Fernández-Juricic, E., Vickery, J. A., and Krebs, J. R. (2006). Predator Detection and Avoidance by Starlings under Differing Scenarios of Predation Risk. Behavioral Ecology, 17, 303309.Google Scholar
Dohrn-van Rossum, G. (1996). History of the Hour: Clocks and Modern Temporal Orders. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Donald, M. (1991). Origins of the Modern Mind: Three Stages in the Evolution of Culture and Cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Dror, I., and Harnad, S. (2008). Offloading Cognition onto Cognitive Technology. Amsterdam, Netherlands: JohnBenjamins Publishing.Google Scholar
Duckworth, A. L., Gendler, T. S., and Gross, J. J. (2016). Situational Strategies for Self-Control. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 11, 3555.Google Scholar
Dufresne, A., and Kobasigawa, A. (1989). Children’s Spontaneous Allocation of Study Time: Differential and Sufficient Aspects. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 47, 274296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunlosky, J., and Tauber, S. K. (2016). The Oxford Handbook of Metamemory. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Eiseley, L. C. (1970). The Invisible Pyramid. Lincoln, NE: University of Nebraska Press.Google Scholar
Elster, J. (2000). Ulysses Unbound: Studies in Rationality, Precommitment, and Constraints. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Fawcett, T. W., McNamara, J. M., and Houston, A. I. (2012). When Is it Adaptive to Be Patient? A General Framework for Evaluating Delayed Rewards. Behavioural Processes, 89, 128136.Google Scholar
Finnbogadóttir, H., and Berntsen, D. (2013). Involuntary Future Projections Are as Frequent as Involuntary Memories, but More Positive. Consciousness and Cognition, 22, 272280.Google Scholar
Fischer, S. R. (2001). A History of Writing. London, UK: Reaktion Books.Google Scholar
Flavell, J. H., and Wellman, H. M. (1977). Metamemory. In Kail, R. V, Jr., and Hagen, J (eds.), Perspectives on the Development of Memory and Cognition. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum, 333.Google Scholar
Frankfurt, H. G. (1988). Freedom of the Will and the Concept of a Person. In Goodman, M. F. (ed.), What Is a Person? Clifton, NJ: Humana Press: 127144.Google Scholar
Gaesser, B., and Schacter, D. L. (2014). Episodic Simulation and Episodic Memory Can Increase Intentions to Help Others. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111, 44154420.Google Scholar
Gilbert, D. T., and Wilson, T. D. (2007). Prospection: Experiencing the Future. Science, 317, 13511354.Google Scholar
Gullo, M. J., Dawe, S., Kambouropoulos, N., Staiger, P. K., and Jackson, C. J. (2010). Alcohol Expectancies and Drinking Refusal Self‐Efficacy Mediate the Association of Impulsivity with Alcohol Misuse. Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research, 34, 13861399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hallos, J. (2005). “15 Minutes Of Fame”: Exploring the Temporal Dimension of Middle Pleistocene Lithic Technology. Journal of Human Evolution, 49(2), 155179.Google Scholar
Hassabis, D., and Maguire, E. A. (2009). The Construction System of the Brain. Philosophical Transactions: Biological Sciences, 364, 12631271.Google Scholar
Heersmink, R. (2013). A Taxonomy of Cognitive Artifacts: Function, Information, and Categories. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 4, 465481.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heersmink, R. (2016). The Cognitive Integration of Scientific Instruments: Information, Situated Cognition, and Scientific Practice. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 15, 517537.Google Scholar
Hesslow, G. (2012). The Current Status of the Simulation Theory of Cognition. Brain Research, 1428, 7179.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heyes, C. (2016). Who Knows? Metacognitive Social Learning Strategies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, xx, 110.Google Scholar
Hirst, W., and Echterhoff, G. (2012). Remembering in Conversations: The Social Sharing and Reshaping of Memories. Annual Review of Psychology, 63, 5579.Google Scholar
Hoerl, C., and McCormack, T. (2018). Thinking in and About Time: A Dual Systems Perspective on Temporal Cognition. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 177. doi:10.1017/S0140525X18002157.Google Scholar
Hutchins, E. (1999). Cognitive Artifacts. The MIT Encyclopedia of the Cognitive Sciences, 126, 127.Google Scholar
Irish, M., and Piguet, O. (2013). The Pivotal Role of Semantic Memory in Remembering the Past and Imagining the Future. Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 7, 27.Google Scholar
Jones, A. (2007). Memory and Material Culture. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kelly, L. (2015). Knowledge and Power in Prehistoric Societies: Orality, Memory, and the Transmission of Culture. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Kelly, L. (2017). The Memory Code: The Secrets of Stonehenge, Easter Island and Other Ancient Monuments. New York, NY: Pegasus Books. books.google.com.au/books?id=dfa8DAEACAAJ.Google Scholar
Kidd, C., Palmeri, H., and Aslin, R. N. (2013). Rational Snacking: Young Children’s Decision-Making on the Marshmallow Task Is Moderated by Beliefs about Environmental Reliability. Cognition, 126, 109114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kirby, K. N., Petry, N. M., and Bickel, W. K. (1999). Heroin Addicts Have Higher Discount Rates for Delayed Rewards than Non-Drug-Using Controls. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 7887.Google Scholar
Klein, S. B. (2013). The Temporal Orientation of Memory: It’s Time for a Change of Direction. Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 2, 222234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koffarnus, M. N., Jarmolowicz, D. P., Mueller, E. T., and Bickel, W. K. (2013). Changing Delay Discounting in the Light of the Competing Neurobehavioral Decision Systems Theory: A Review. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 99, 3257.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Köhler, W. (1925). The Mentality of Apes. New York, NY: Read Books Ltd.Google Scholar
Kohn, M., and Mithen, S. (1999). Handaxes: Products of Sexual Selection? Antiquity, 73, 518526.Google Scholar
Legare, C. H., and Nielsen, M. (2015). Imitation and Innovation: The Dual Engines of Cultural Learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 19, 688699.Google Scholar
Lepre, C. J., Roche, H., Kent, D. V., et al. (2011). An Earlier Origin for the Acheulian. Nature, 477(7362), 8285.Google Scholar
Loewenstein, G. F., and Lerner, J. (2003). The Role of Affect in Decision-Making. In Davidson, R, Goldsmith, H, and Scherer, K (ed.), Handbook of Affective Science. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 619642.Google Scholar
Loewenstein, G. F., Read, D., and Baumeister, R. F. (2003). Time and Decision: Economic and Psychological Perspectives of Intertemporal Choice. New York, NY: Russell Sage Foundation. books.google.com.au/books?id=iHJcQJcg3BICx.Google Scholar
Machin, A. J. (2008). Why Handaxes Just Aren’t That Sexy: A Response to Kohn & Mithen (1999). Antiquity, 82(317), 761766.Google Scholar
Meece, S. (2006). A Bird’s Eye View – of a Leopard’s Spots: The Çatalhöyük “Map” and the Development of Cartographic Representation in Prehistory. Anatolian Studies, 56, 116.Google Scholar
Mellaart, J. (1967). Çatal Hüyük: a Neolithic Town in Anatolia. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.Google Scholar
Michaelian, K., and Sutton, J. (2013). Distributed Cognition and Memory Research: History and Current Directions. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 4, 124.Google Scholar
Miloyan, B., Bulley, A., and Suddendorf, T. (2016). Episodic Foresight and Anxiety: Proximate and Ultimate Perspectives. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 55(1), 422.Google Scholar
Miloyan, B., Bulley, A., and Suddendorf, T. (2019). Anxiety: Here and Beyond. Emotion Review, 11(1), 3949.Google Scholar
Mischel, W. (2014). The Marshmallow Test: Understanding Self-Control and How to Master it. New York, NY: Random House.Google Scholar
Mischel, W., Shoda, Y., and Rodriguez, M. I. (1989). Delay of Gratification in Children. Science, 244, 933938.Google Scholar
Mithen, S. J. (1996). The Prehistory of the Mind: a Search for the Origins of Art, Religion and Science. London, UK: Weidenfeld & Nicholson.Google Scholar
Müller, C. P., and Schumann, G. (2011). Drugs as Instruments: A New Framework for Non-Addictive Psychoactive Drug Use. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 34, 293310.Google Scholar
Neldner, K., Collier-Baker, E., and Nielsen, M. (2015). Chimpanzees (Pan Troglodytes) and Human Children (Homo Sapiens) Know When They Are Ignorant about the Location of Food. Animal Cognition, 18, 683699.Google Scholar
Nelson, T., and Narens, L. (1990). Metamemory: A Theoretical Framework and New Findings. Psychology of Learning and Motivation, 26, 125173.Google Scholar
Nestojko, J. F., Finley, J. R., and Roediger, H. L. (2013). Extending Cognition to External Agents. Psychological Inquiry, 24, 321325.Google Scholar
Oakley, K. P. (1981). Emergence of Higher Thought 3.0-0.2 Ma B.P. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 292, 205211.Google Scholar
Palermos, S. O. (2016). The Dynamics of Group Cognition. Minds and Machines, 26, 409440.Google Scholar
Parfit, D. (1971). Personal Identity. The Philosophical Review, 80, 327.Google Scholar
Perner, J. (1991). Learning, Development, and Conceptual Change. Understanding the Representational Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Peters, J., and Büchel, C. (2010). Episodic Future Thinking Reduces Reward Delay Discounting through an Enhancement of Prefrontal-Mediotemporal Interactions. Neuron, 66, 138148.Google Scholar
Pezzulo, G. (2008). Coordinating with the Future: The Anticipatory Nature of Representation. Minds and Machines, 18, 179225.Google Scholar
Pezzulo, G., and Rigoli, F. (2011). The Value of Foresight: How Prospection Affects Decision-Making. Frontiers in Neuroscience, 5, 79.Google Scholar
Popper, K. (1934). The Logic of Scientific Discovery. London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Pylyshyn, Z. W. (1978). When Is Attribution of Beliefs Justified? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(4), 592593.Google Scholar
Rachlin, H. (1995). Self-Control: Beyond Commitment. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18(1), 109121.Google Scholar
Rachlin, H. (2016). Self-Control Based on Soft Commitment. Behavior Analyst, 39, 259268.Google Scholar
Rafetseder, E., Cristi-Vargas, R., and Perner, J. (2010). Counterfactual Reasoning: Developing a Sense of “Nearest Possible World”. Child Development, 81, 376389.Google Scholar
Redshaw, J. (2014). Does Metarepresentation Make Human Mental Time Travel Unique? Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 5(5), 519531.Google Scholar
Redshaw, J., and Bulley, A. (2018). Future-Thinking in Animals Capacities and Limits. In Oettingen, G, Sevincer, A. T., and Gollwitzer, P (eds.), The Psychology of Thinking about the Future. New York, NY: Guilford Press, 3151.Google Scholar
Redshaw, J., and Suddendorf, T. (2016). Children’s and Apes’ Preparatory Responses to Two Mutually Exclusive Possibilities. Current Biology, 26(13), 17581762.Google Scholar
Redshaw, J., Suddendorf, T., Neldner, K., et al. (2019). Young Children From Three Diverse Cultures Spontaneously and Consistently Prepare for Alternative Future Possibilities. Child Development, 90(1), 5161.Google Scholar
Redshaw, J., Vandersee, J., Bulley, A., and Gilbert, S. J. (2018). Development of Children’s Use of External Reminders for Hard-to-Remember Intentions. Child Development, 89( 6), 2099–2118.Google Scholar
Risko, E. F., and Gilbert, S. J. (2016). Cognitive Offloading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20, 676688.Google Scholar
Rorty, A. O. (1980). Where Does the Akratic Break Take Place? Australasian Journal of Philosophy, 58, 333346.Google Scholar
Santos, L. R., and Rosati, A. G. (2015). The Evolutionary Roots of Human Decision-Making. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 321347.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., and Buckner, R. L. (2008). Episodic Simulation of Future Events: Concepts, Data, and Applications. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1124, 3960.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Addis, D. R., Hassabis, D., et al. (2012). The Future of Memory: Remembering, Imagining, and the Brain. Neuron, 76, 677694.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Benoit, R. G., De Brigard, F., and Szpunar, K. K. (2015). Episodic Future Thinking and Episodic Counterfactual Thinking: Intersections between Memory and Decisions. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 117, 1421.Google Scholar
Schacter, D. L., Benoit, R. G., and Szpunar, K. K. (2017). Episodic Future Thinking: Mechanisms and Functions. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 17, 4150.Google Scholar
Schelling, T. C. (1960). The Strategy of Conflict. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Schelling, T. C. (1984). Self-Command in Practice, in Policy, and in a Theory of Rational Choice. The American Economic Review, 74(2), 111.Google Scholar
Seli, P., Kane, M. J., Smallwood, J., et al. (2018). Mind-Wandering as a Natural Kind: A Family-Resemblances View. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 22, 479490.Google Scholar
Shipton, C., and Nielsen, M. (2015). Before Cumulative Culture: The Evolutionary Origins of Overimitation and Shared Intentionality. Human Nature, 26, 331345.Google Scholar
Smith, C. D. (1987). Cartography in the Prehistoric Period in the Old World: Europe, the Middle East, and North Africa. The History of Cartography, 1, 54102.Google Scholar
Sterelny, K. (2010). Minds: Extended or Scaffolded? Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 9, 465481.Google Scholar
Stevens, J. R., and Stephens, D. (2008). Patience. Current Biology: CB, 18, R11–2.Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T. (1994). Discovery of the Fourth Dimension: Mental Time Travel and Human Evolution. Waikato, New Zealand: University of Waikato, PhD thesis. cogprints.org/729/1/THESIS.txt.Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T. (1999). The Rise of the Metamind. In Corballis, M and Lee, S (eds.), The Descent of Mind: Psychological Perspectives on Hominid Evolution. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press, 218260.Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T. (2010). Episodic Memory versus Episodic Foresight: Similarities and Differences. Wiley Interdisciplinary Reviews: Cognitive Science, 1, 99107.Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T. (2013). The Gap: The Science of What Separates us from Other Animals. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T., Brinums, M., and Imuta, K. (2016). Shaping One’s Future Self – The Development of Deliberate Practice. In Michaelian, K, Klein, S. B., and Szpunar, K. K. (eds.), Seeing the Future: Theoretical Perspectives on Future-Oriented Mental Time Travel. New York, NY: Oxford University Press, 343366.Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T., and Corballis, M. C. (1997). Mental Time Travel and the Evolution of the Human Mind. Genetic Social and General Psychology Monographs, 123, 133167.Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T., and Corballis, M. C. (2007). The Evolution of Foresight: What Is Mental Time Travel, and Is it Unique to Humans? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30, 299351.Google Scholar
Suddendorf, T., Crimston, J., and Redshaw, J. (2017). Preparatory Responses to Socially Determined, Mutually Exclusive Possibilities in Chimpanzees and Children. Biology Letters, 13.Google Scholar
Sutton, J. (2006). Exaograms and Interdisciplinarity: History, the Extended Mind, and the Civilizing Process. In Menary, R (ed.), The Extended Mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press, 189225.Google ScholarPubMed
Szpunar, K. K. (2010). Episodic Future Thought: An Emerging Concept. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5, 142162.Google Scholar
Thaler, R. H., and Shefrin, H. (1981). An Economic Theory of Self-Control. Journal of Political Economy, 89(2), 392406.Google Scholar
Utrilla, P., Mazo, C., Sopena, M. C., Martínez-Bea, M., and Domingo, R. (2009). A Palaeolithic Map from 13,660 calBP: Engraved Stone Blocks from the Late Magdalenian in Abauntz Cave (Navarra, Spain). Journal of Human Evolution, 57, 99111.Google Scholar
Whiten, A., and Erdal, D. (2012). The Human Socio-Cognitive Niche and its Evolutionary Origins. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 367, 21192129.Google Scholar
Woodward, D., and Harley, J. B. (1987). The History of Cartography, Volume 1: Cartography in Prehistoric, Ancient, and Medieval Europe and the Mediterranean. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×