Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-gb8f7 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-23T20:37:31.928Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

9 - Capturing the Imagination

from Part I - Theoretical Perspectives on the Imagination

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  26 May 2020

Anna Abraham
Affiliation:
University of Georgia
Get access

Summary

To paraphrase the famous beginning of Douglas Adams’s The Hitchhiker’s Guide to the Galaxy: “Imagination is big. Really big. You just won’t believe how vastly hugely mindbogglingly big it is.” And it must be important, if even neuroscientists have noticed it. With momentum gathering to get a more comprehensive and interdisciplinary grip on the human imagination, at a time when still no one has much of a clue about this hypercomplex realm of the mind, we can expect to be treated to an initial proliferation of ideas, views, frameworks, and theories on the matter. But that is a good thing. There must be variation before there can be selection. Taking advantage of this temporarily heightened tolerance for speculation, this chapter proposes that the process of navigating an imaginary space can be described by different evolutionary algorithms that vary according to the prior knowledge we have of the topography of the imagined landscape or, put another way, the degree of sightedness we have of the fitness function of the imagined domain. Seen from this perspective, the category “novel combinatorial” of Abraham’s (2016) framework is best dissolved, with creative thinking being distributed and embedded into all other forms of the imagination.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2020

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Abraham, A. (2016). The Imaginative Mind. Human Brain Mapping, 37, 41974211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bar, M. (2007). The Proactive Brain: Using Analogies and Associations to Generate Predictions. Trends in Cognitive Science, 11, 280289.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bar, M.(2009). The Proactive Brain: Memory for Prediction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 364, 12351243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barsalou, L. W. (2009). Simulation, Situated Conceptualization, and Prediction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 364, 12811289.Google Scholar
Campbell, D. T. (1960). Blind Variation and Selective Retention in Creative Thought as in Other Knowledge Processes. Psychological Review, 67, 380400.Google Scholar
Chatterjee, A., and Vartanian, O. (2014). Neuroaesthetics. Trends in Cognitive Science, 18, 370375.Google Scholar
Christoff, K. (2012). Undirected Thought: Neural Determinants and Correlates. Brain Research, 1428, 5159.Google Scholar
Dawkins, R. (1976). The Selfish Gene. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Dennett, D. C. (1995). Darwin’s Dangerous Idea. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Dietrich, A. (2004). The Cognitive Neuroscience of Creativity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 11, 10111026.Google Scholar
Dietrich, A.(2015). How Creativity Happens in the Brain. London, UK: Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dietrich, A.(2019). Types of Creativity. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26(1), 112.Google Scholar
Dietrich, A., and Haider, H. (2017). A Neurocognitive Framework for Human Creative Thought. Frontiers in Psychology: Cognitive Science, 7, 20782085.Google Scholar
Fisher, J. C. (2006). Does Simulation Theory Really Involve Simulation? Philosophical Psychology, 19, 417432.Google Scholar
Frith, C. D. (1992). The Cognitive Neuropsychology of Schizophrenia. Hove, UK: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Gendler, T. (2013). Imagination. In Zalta, E. N. (ed.), The Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy (Fall). plato.stanford.edu/entries/imagination/.Google Scholar
Gould, S. J. (1979). Shades of Lamarck. Natural History, 88, 2228.Google Scholar
Grush, R. (2004). The Emulation Theory of Representation: Motor Control, Imagery, and Perception. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 27, 377396.Google Scholar
Kronfeldner, M. E. (2007). Is Cultural Evolution Lamarckian? Biology and Philosophy, 22, 493512.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kronfeldner, M. E.(2010). Darwinian “Blind” Hypothesis Formation Revisited. Synthese, 175, 193218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lewontin, R. C. (1970). The Units of Selection. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics, 1, 118.Google Scholar
Lewontin, R. C.(1991). Biology as Ideology. New York, NY: Harper.Google Scholar
Llinás, R. R. (2001). I of the Vortex: From Neurons to Self. Boston, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Mayr, E. (1981). The Growth of Biological Thought: Diversity, Evolution, and Inheritance. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Medawar, P. M. (1953). A Commentary on Lamarckism. Reprinted in Medawar, P (1981), The Uniqueness of the Individual. New York, NY: Dover, 6387.Google Scholar
Richerson, P. J., and Boyd, R. (2005). Not by Genes Alone: How Culture Transformed Human Evolution. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Schooler, J. W., and Dougal, S. (1999). Why Creativity Is Not like the Proverbial Typing Monkey. Psychological Inquiry, 10, 351356.Google Scholar
Shepard, R. N., and Metzler, J. (1971). Mental Rotation of Three-Dimensional Objects. Science, 171, 701703.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simonton, D. K. (1999). Creativity as Blind Variation and Selective Retention: Is the Creative Process Darwinian? Psychological Inquiry, 10, 309328.Google Scholar
Singer, J. L. (1975). Navigating the Stream of Consciousness: Research in Daydreaming and Related Inner Experiences. American Psychologist, 30, 727738.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, E. A. (2013). Agency and Adaptation: New Directions in Evolutionary Anthropology. Annual Review of Anthropology, 41, 103120.Google Scholar
Sternberg, R. J. (1998). Cognitive Mechanisms in Human Creativity: Is Variation Blind or Sighted? Journal of Creative Behavior, 32, 159176.Google Scholar
Stevenson, L. (2003). Twelve Conceptions of Imagination. The British Journal of Aesthetics, 43(3), 238259.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Weisberg, R. W., and Hass, R. (2007). We Are All Partly Right: Comment on Simonton. Creativity Research Journal, 19, 345360.Google Scholar
Wolpert, D. M., Doya, K., and Kawato, M. (2003). A Unifying Computational Framework for Motor Control and Social Interaction. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society Series B, 358, 593602.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×