Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-fscjk Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-24T18:13:27.618Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Age and cognitive functioning

from Psychology, health and illness

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  18 December 2014

David R. Shaffer
Affiliation:
University of Georgia
Gail M. Williamson
Affiliation:
University of Georgia
Susan Ayers
Affiliation:
University of Sussex
Andrew Baum
Affiliation:
University of Pittsburgh
Chris McManus
Affiliation:
St Mary's Hospital Medical School
Stanton Newman
Affiliation:
University College and Middlesex School of Medicine
Kenneth Wallston
Affiliation:
Vanderbilt University School of Nursing
John Weinman
Affiliation:
United Medical and Dental Schools of Guy's and St Thomas's
Robert West
Affiliation:
St George's Hospital Medical School, University of London
Get access

Summary

Ageing and cognitive functioning: an overview

Attitudes about ageing reflect many negative stereotypes about the intellectual prowess of our senior citizens (Center for the Advancement of Health, 1998). Are older people as cognitively deficient as is commonly assumed? Are they significantly less capable than middle-aged or younger adults of profiting from classroom instruction, solving everyday problems such as remembering to turn off the oven after removing a roast, or learning to operate such new technologies as computers, DVD players, or ATM machines?

Study of the abilities to learn, remember, and solve problems has a long history in psychology and is arguably the most thoroughly investigated aspect of adult development and ageing (Siegelman & Rider, 2003). One reason for this emphasis is that cognitive functioning in adulthood has so many important implications for the quality of ordinary people's lives. Furthermore, cognitive functioning can play a major role in how people feel about themselves as they age. Middle-aged adults occasionally claim that they have experienced a ‘senior moment’ after forgetting someone's name, their own telephone number, or a step in a well practiced procedure such as recording a programme on their VCR. Although such comments may be offered in a humorous light, they also may reflect a deep-seated concern that many middle-aged and older adults have about losing their memories (Whitbourne, 2005) (see ‘Dementias’).

Indeed, older adults have reasons for suspecting that their cognitive powers are declining.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2007

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Artistico, D., Cervone, D. & Pezzati, L. (2003). Perceived self-efficacy and everyday problem solving among young and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 18, 68–79.Google Scholar
Barrett, T. R. & Wright, M. (1981). Age-related facilitation of recall following semantic processing. Journal of Gerontology, 36, 194–9.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bowles, R. P. & Salthouse, T. A. (2003). Assessing the age-related effects of proactive interference on working memory tasks using the Rasch model. Psychology and Aging, 18, 608–15.Google Scholar
Braver, T. S., Barch, D. M., Keys, B. A.et al. (2001). Context processing in older adults: evidence for a theory relating cognitive control to neurobiology in healthy aging. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 746–63.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cattell, R. B. (1963). Theory of fluid and crystallized intelligence: a critical experiment. Journal of Educational Psychology, 54, 1–22.Google Scholar
Cattell, R. B. (1971). Abilities: their structure, growth, and action. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.
Cavanaugh, J. C. (1996). Memory self-efficacy as a moderator of memory change. In Blanchard-Fields, F. & Hess, T. M. (Eds.). Perspectives on cognitive change in adulthood and aging (pp. 488–507). New York: McGraw-Hill.
Center for the Advancement of Health (1998). Getting old: a lot of it is in your head. Facts of Life: An Issue Briefing for Health Reporters, 3.
Christensen, H., Korten, A. E., Jorm, A. F. & Henderson, A. (1997). Education and decline in cognitive performance: compensatory but not protective. International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 12, 323–30.Google Scholar
Craik, F. I. M. (1994). Memory changes in normal aging. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 3, 155–8.Google Scholar
Cregger, M. E. & Rogers, W. A. (1998). Memory for activities for young, young-old and old adults: Experimental Aging Research, 24, 195–201.Google Scholar
Deary, I. J., Leaper, S. A., Murray, A. D., Staff, R. T. & Whalley, L. J. (2003). Cerebral white matter abnormalities and lifetime cognitive change: a 67-year follow-up of the Scottish Mental Survey of 1932. Psychology and Aging, 18, 140–8.Google Scholar
Engle, R. W., Tuholski, S. W., Laughlin, J. E. & Conway, A. R. A. (1999). Working memory, short-term memory, and general fluid intelligence: a latent variable approach. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 128, 309–31.Google Scholar
Frieski, D. A. & Park, D. C. (1999). Memory for news in young and old adults. Psychology and Aging, 14, 90–8.Google Scholar
Gaeta, H., Friedman, D., Ritter, W. & Cheng, J. (2001). An event-related potential evaluation of involuntary attentional shifts in young and older adults. Psychology and Aging, 16, 55–68.Google Scholar
Hasher, L., Zacks, R. T. & May, C. P. (1999). Inhibitory control, circadian arousal, and age. In attention and performance, XVII, cognitive regulation of performance: interaction of theory and application (pp. 653–75). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Hedden, T. & Park, D. C. (2001). Aging and interference in verbal working memory. Psychology and Aging, 16, 666–81.Google Scholar
Henry, J. D., MacLeod, M. S., Phillips, L. H. & Crawford, J. R. (2004). A meta-analytic review of prospective memory and aging. Psychology and Aging, 19, 27–39.Google Scholar
Howard, D. V., Howard, J. H. Jr., Japikse, K.et al. (2004). Implicit sequence learning: Effects of level of structure, adult age, and extended practice. Psychology and Aging, 19, 79–82.Google Scholar
Hoyer, W. J., Stawski, R. S., Wasylyshyn, C. & Verhaeghen, P. (2004). Adult age and digit symbol substitution performance: A meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 19, 211–14.Google Scholar
Johansson, B., Hofer, S. M., Allaire, J. C.et al. (2004). Change in cognitive capabilities in the oldest old: the effects of proximity to death in genetically related individuals over a 6-year period. Psychology and Aging, 19, 145–56.Google Scholar
Kail, R. & Salthouse, T. A. (1994). Processing speed as a mental capacity. Acta Psychologica, 86, 199–225.Google Scholar
Li, K. Z. H., Lindenberger, U., Freund, A. M. & Baltes, P. B. (2001). Walking while memorizing: age-related differences in compensatory behavior. Psychological Science, 12, 230–7.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lindenberger, U., Marsiske, M. & Baltes, P. B. (2000). Memorizing while walking: increase in dual-task costs from young adulthood to old age. Psychology and Aging, 15, 417–36.Google Scholar
Liu, L. L. & Park, D. C. (2004). Aging and medical adherence: the use of automatic processes to achieve effortful things. Psychology and Aging, 19, 318–25.Google Scholar
Malstrom, T. & LaVoie, D. J. (2002). Age differences in inhibition of schema-activated distractors. Experimental Aging Research, 28, 281–98.Google Scholar
McGue, M. & Christensen, K. (2002). The heritability of level and rate-of-change in cognitive functioning in Danish twins aged 70 years and older. Experimental Aging Research, 28, 435–51.Google Scholar
Mitchell, D. B. & Bruss, P. J. (2003). Age differences implicit memory: conceptual, perceptual, or methodological?Psychology and Aging, 18, 807–22.Google Scholar
Naveh-Benjamin, M., Craik, F. I. M. & Ben-Shaul, L. (2002). Age-related differences in cued recall: effects of support at encoding and retrieval. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 9, 276–87.Google Scholar
Park, D. C., Lautenschlager, G., Hedden, T.et al. (2002). Models of visuospatial and verbal memory across the adult life span. Psychology and Aging, 17, 299–320.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Persad, C. C., Abeles, N., Zacks, R. T. & Denbury, N. L. (2002). Inhibitory changes after age 60 and their relationship to measures of attention and memory. Journals of Gerontology Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 57, 223–32.Google Scholar
Plemons, J. K., Willis, S. L. & Baltes, P. B. (1978). Modifiability of fluid intelligence in aging: a short-term longitudinal training approach. Journal of Gerontology, 33, 224–31.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reder, L. M., Wible, C. & Martin, J. (1986). Differential memory changes with age: exact retrieval versus plausible inference. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 12, 72–81.Google Scholar
Rogers, W. A., Fisk, A. D., Mead, S. E., Walker, N. & Cabrera, E. F. (1996). Training older adults to use automatic teller machines. Human Factors, 38, 425–33.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Saczynski, J. S., Willis, S. L. & Schaie, K. W. (2002). Strategy use in reasoning training with older adults. Aging, Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 9, 48–60.Google Scholar
Salthouse, T. A. (1996). The processing-speed theory of adult age differences in cognition. Psychological Review, 103, 403–28.Google Scholar
Schaie, K. W. (1983). The Seattle Longitudinal Study: a 21-year exploration of psychometric intelligence in adulthood. In Schaie, K. S. (Ed.). Longitudinal studies of adult psychological development (pp. 64–135). New York: Guilford.
Schaie, K. W. (1989). The hazards of cognitive aging. Gerontologist, 29, 484–93.Google Scholar
Schaie, K. W. (1994). The course of adult intellectual development. American Psychologist, 49, 304–13.Google Scholar
Schaie, K. W. (1996). Intellectual development in adulthood: the Seattle longitudinal study. New York: Cambridge University Press.
Schaie, K. W., Nguyen, H. T., Willis, S. L., Dutta, R. & Yue, G. A. (2001). Environmental factors as a conceptual framework for examining cognitive performance in Chinese adults. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 25, 193–202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schaie, K. W. & Willis, S. L. (1986). Can decline in adult intellectual functioning be reversed?Developmental Psychology, 22, 223–32.Google Scholar
Sigelman, C. K. & Rider, E. A. (2003). Life-Span Human Development (4th edn.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth.
Simensky, J. D. & Abeles, N. (2002). Decline of verbal memory performance with advancing age: the role of frontal lobe functioning. Aging and Mental Health, 6, 293–303.Google Scholar
Singer, T., Verhaeghen, P., Ghisletta, P., Lindenberger, U. & Baltes, P. B. (2003). The fate of cognition in very old age: six-year longitudinal findings in the Berlin Aging Study (BASE). Psychology and Aging, 18, 318–31.Google Scholar
Spearman, C. (1927). The abilities of man. New York: Macmillian.
Swanson, H. L. (1999). What develops in working memory: a life span perspective. Developmental Psychology, 35, 986–1000.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thurstone, L. L. (1938). Primary mental abilities. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Verhaeghen, P. (2003). Aging and vocabulary scores: a meta-analysis. Psychology and Aging, 16, 332–39.Google Scholar
Volz, T. (2000). Successful aging: The second 50. Monitor on Psychology, 31, 24–8.Google Scholar
Whitbourne, S. K. (2005). Adult Development and Aging: Biopsychosocial Perspectives (2nd edn.). New York: Wiley.
Williamson, G. M. (2002). Aging well: outlook for the 21st century. In Snyder, C. R. & Lopez, S. J. (Eds.). The Handbook of Positive Psychology (pp. 676–86). New York: Oxford University Press.
Willis, S. L., Blieszner, R. & Baltes, P. B. (1981). Intellectual training research in aging: modification of performance on the fluid ability of figural relations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 73, 41–50.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×