Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-586b7cd67f-t7fkt Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-11-24T22:33:40.117Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

18 - The Worked Example Principle in Multimedia Learning

from Part IV - Principles for Reducing Extraneous Processing in Multimedia Learning

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  19 November 2021

Richard E. Mayer
Affiliation:
University of California, Santa Barbara
Logan Fiorella
Affiliation:
University of Georgia
Get access

Summary

This chapter outlines the tight connections between multimedia learning and worked examples in that worked examples can make multimedia learning more effective, and applying instructional multimedia guidelines makes learning from worked examples more effective. The to-be-expected effect sizes when using worked examples are discussed, and the theoretical rationale of the worked-example effect is explained (e.g., example study replaces unproductive learning by problem-solving). In addition, a set of instructional guidelines for optimizing learning from worked examples is derived from findings on factors that moderate the worked-example effect, such as fading worked steps or including incorrectly worked examples. Furthermore, the theoretical implications of the general pattern of findings on worked examples are discussed (e.g., with respect to the role of generative learning or extraneous processing). Finally, important questions to be addressed in further research are proposed.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2021

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Atkinson, R. K., Renkl, A., & Merrill, M. M. (2003). Transitioning from studying examples to solving problems: Combining fading with prompting fosters learning. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 774783.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barbieri, C. A., & Booth, J. L. (2020). Mistakes on display: Incorrect examples refine equation solving and algebraic feature knowledge. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 34, 862878.Google Scholar
Bentley, B., & Yates, G. C. R. (2017). Facilitating proportional reasoning through worked examples: Two classroom-based experiments. Cogent Education, 4, 1297213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berthold, K., & Renkl, A. (2009). Instructional aids to support a conceptual understanding of multiple representations. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 7087.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Catrambone, R. (1996). Generalizing solution procedures learned from examples. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 22, 10201031.Google Scholar
Cooper, G., Tindall-Ford, S., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2001). Learning by imagining procedures and concepts. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 7, 6882.Google Scholar
Eitel, A., Endres, T., & Renkl, A. (2020). Self-management as a bridge between cognitive load and self-regulated learning: The illustrative case of seductive details. Educational Psychology Review, 32, 10731087.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiorella, L., & Mayer, R. E. (2016). Eight ways to promote generative learning. Educational Psychology Review, 28, 717741.Google Scholar
Gentner, D., Loewenstein, J., & Thompson, L. (2003). Learning and transfer: A general role for analogical encoding. Journal of Educational Psychology, 95, 393408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ginns, P., Chandler, P., & Sweller, J. (2003). When imagining information is effective. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 28, 229251.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ginns, P., Hu, F.-T., & Bobis, J. (2020). Tracing enhances problem‐solving transfer, but without effects on intrinsic or extraneous cognitive load. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 34, 15221529.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glogger-Frey, I., Fleischer, C., Grüny, L., Kappich, J., & Renkl, A. (2015). Inventing a solution and studying a worked solution prepare differently for learning from direct instruction. Learning & Instruction, 39, 7287.Google Scholar
Glogger-Frey, I., Gaus, K., & Renkl, A. (2017). Learning from direct instruction: Best prepared by several self-regulated or guided invention activities? Learning & Instruction, 51, 2635.Google Scholar
Große, C. S., & Renkl, A. (2007). Finding and fixing errors in worked examples: Can this foster learning outcomes? Learning & Instruction, 17, 612634.Google Scholar
Hallinen, N. R., & Booth, J. L. (2018). Don’t just do it, explain it: A 5th grade worked examples curriculum supports transfer to algebra content. Proceedings of International Conference of the Learning Sciences (pp. 16471648). London: ICLS.Google Scholar
Hartmann, C., van Gog, T., & Rummel, N. (2020). Do examples of failure effectively prepare students for learning from subsequent instruction? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 34, 879889.Google Scholar
Hattie, J. A. C. (2009). Visible Learning: A Synthesis of over 800 Meta-Analyses Relating to Achievement. London: Routledge.Google Scholar
Hefter, M. H., Renkl, A., Riess, W., Schmid, S., Fries, S., & Berthold, K. (2018). Training interventions to foster skill and will of argumentative thinking. Journal of Experimental Education, 86, 325343.Google Scholar
Hilbert, T. S., Renkl, A., Schworm, S., Kessler, S., & Reiss, K. (2008). Learning to teach with worked-out examples: A computer-based learning environment for teachers. Journal of Computer-Assisted Learning, 24, 316332.Google Scholar
Hiller, S., Rumann, S., Berthold, K., & Roelle, J. (2020). Example‑based learning: Should learners receive closed‑book or open‑book self‑explanation prompts? Instructional Science, 48, 623649.Google Scholar
Holyoak, K. J. (2012). Analogy and relational reasoning. In Holyoak, K. J., & Morrison, R. G. (eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Thinking and Reasoning (pp. 234259). New York: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hoogerheide, V., Van Wermeskerken, M., Loyens, S. M. M., & Van Gog, T. (2016). Learning from video modeling examples: Content kept equal, adults are more effective models than peers. Learning & Instruction, 44, 2230.Google Scholar
Kapur, M. (2008). Productive failure. Cognition & Instruction, 26, 379424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krebs, M.-C., Schüler, A., & Scheiter, K. (2019). Just follow my eyes: The influence of model-observer similarity on eye movement modeling examples. Learning & Instruction, 61, 126137.Google Scholar
Mason, L., Pluchino, P., & Tornatora, M. C. (2015). Eye-movement modeling of integrative reading of an illustrated text: Effects on processing and learning. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 41, 172187.Google Scholar
Mayer, R. E. (2021). Multimedia Learning (3rd ed.). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Newman, P. M., & DeCaro, M. S. (2019). Learning by exploring: How much guidance is optimal? Learning & Instruction, 62, 4963.Google Scholar
Nokes-Malach, T. J., VanLehn, K., Belenky, D., Lichtenstein, M., & Cox, G. (2013). Coordinating principles and examples through analogy and self-explanation. European Journal of Education of Psychology, 28, 12371263.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Paas, F., & van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (1994). Variability of worked examples and transfer of geometrical problem-solving skills: A cognitive-load approach. Journal of Educational Psychology, 86, 122133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pant, H. A. (2014). Aufbereitung von Evidenz für bildungspolitische und pädagogische Entscheidungen: Metaanalysen in der Bildungsforschung [Synthesizing evidence for educational policy and educational decisions: Meta-analyses in educational sciences]. Zeitschrift für Erziehungswissenschaft, 17, 7999.Google Scholar
Quilici, J. L., & Mayer, R. E. (1996). Role of examples in how students learn to categorize statistics word problems. Journal of Educational Psychology, 88, 144161.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Reiss, K., Heinze, A., Renkl, A., & Groß, C. (2008). Reasoning and proof in geometry: Effects of a learning environment based on heuristic worked-out examples. ZDM – Mathematics Education, 40, 455467.Google Scholar
Renkl, A. (1997). Learning from worked-out examples: A study on individual differences. Cognitive Science, 21, 129.Google Scholar
Renkl, A. (2014). Towards an instructionally-oriented theory of example-based learning. Cognitive Science, 38, 137.Google Scholar
Renkl, A. (2015). Different roads lead to Rome: The case of principle-based cognitive skills. Learning: Research & Practice, 1, 7990.Google Scholar
Renkl, A. (2017). Learning from worked examples in mathematics: Students relate procedures to principles. ZDM Mathematics Education, 49, 571584.Google Scholar
Renkl, A., & Atkinson, R. K. (2003). Structuring the transition from example study to problem solving in cognitive skills acquisition: A cognitive load perspective. Educational Psychologist, 38, 1522.Google Scholar
Renkl, A., Hilbert, T., & Schworm, S. (2009). Example-based learning in heuristic domains: A cognitive load theory account. Educational Psychology Review, 21, 6778.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Richey, J. E., & Nokes-Malach, T. J. (2013). How much is too much? Explanatory text effects on conceptual learning and motivation. Learning & Instruction, 25, 104121.Google Scholar
Rittle-Johnson, B., Star, J. R., & Durkin, K. (2009). The importance of prior knowledge when comparing examples: Influences on conceptual and procedural knowledge of equation solving. Journal of Educational Psychology, 101, 836852.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roelle, J., Hiller, S., Berthold, K., & Rumann, S. (2017). Example-based learning: The benefits of prompting organization before providing examples. Learning and Instruction, 49, 112.Google Scholar
Roelle, J., & Renkl, A. (2020). Does an option to review instructional explanations enhance example-based learning? It depends on learners’ academic self-concept. Journal of Educational Psychology, 112, 131147.Google Scholar
Rummel, N., Spada, H., & Hauser, S. (2009). Learning to collaborate while being scripted or by observing a model. International Journal of Computer-Supported Collaborative Learning, 4, 6992.Google Scholar
Salden, R., Aleven, V., Schwonke, R., & Renkl, A. (2010). The expertise-reversal effect and worked examples in tutored problem solving. Instructional Science, 38, 289307.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Salden, R., Koedinger, K. R., Renkl, A., Aleven, V., & McLaren, B. M. (2010). Accounting for beneficial effects of worked examples in tutored problem solving. Educational Psychology Review, 22, 379392.Google Scholar
Scheiter, K. (2020). Embracing complexity in research on learning from examples and from problem solving. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 34, 906911.Google Scholar
Schwonke, R., Berthold, K., & Renkl, A. (2009). How multiple external representations are used and how they can be made more useful. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 23, 12271243.Google Scholar
Schwonke, R., Ertelt, A., Otieno, C., Renkl, A., Aleven, V., & Salden, R. (2013). Metacognitive support promotes an effective use of instructional resources in intelligent tutoring. Learning & Instruction, 23, 136150.Google Scholar
Schwonke, R., Renkl, A., Krieg, K., Wittwer, J., Aleven, V., & Salden, R. (2009). The worked-example effect: Not an artefact of lousy control conditions. Computers in Human Behavior, 25, 258266.Google Scholar
Spanjers, I. A. E., van Gog, T., & van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2012). Segmentation of worked examples: Effects on cognitive load and learning. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 26, 352358.Google Scholar
Sweller, J., & Cooper, G. A. (1985). The use of worked examples as a substitute for problem solving in learning algebra. Cognition & Instruction, 2, 5989.Google Scholar
Sweller, J., van Merriënboer, J. J. G., & Paas, F. (2019). Cognitive architecture and instructional design: 20 years later. Educational Psychology Review, 31, 261292.Google Scholar
Tarmizi, R. A., & Sweller, J. (1988). Guidance during mathematical problem solving. Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 424436.Google Scholar
Tindall-Ford, S., Agostinho, S., Bokosmaty, S., Paas, F., & Chandler, P. (2015). Computer-based learning of geometry from integrated and split-attention worked examples: The power of self-management. Educational Technology & Society, 18, 8999.Google Scholar
van Gog, T., Rummel, N., & Renkl, A. (2019). Learning how to solve problems by studying examples. In Dunlosky, J., & Rawson, K. (eds.), Cambridge Handbook on Cognition and Education (pp. 183208). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
van Harsel, M., Hoogerheide, V., Verkoeijen, P., & van Gog, T. (2019). Effects of different sequences of examples and problems on motivation and learning. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 58, 260275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wittrock, M. C. (1989). Generative processes of comprehension. Educational Psychologist, 24, 345376.Google Scholar
Wittwer, J., & Renkl, A. (2010). How effective are instructional explanations in example-based learning? A meta-analytic review. Educational Psychology Review, 22, 393409.Google Scholar
Wouters, P., Paas, F., & van Merriënboer, J. J. G. (2009). Observational learning from animated models: Effects of modality and reflection on transfer. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 34, 18.Google Scholar
Yeo, L., & Tzeng, Y. (2020). Cognitive effect of tracing gesture in the learning from mathematics worked examples. International Journal of Science and Mathematics Education, 18, 733751.Google Scholar
Ziegler, E., & Stern, E. (2014). Delayed benefits of learning elementary algebraic transformations through contrasted comparisons. Learning & Instruction, 33, 131146.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×