Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-6bf8c574d5-b4m5d Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-02-22T19:18:59.715Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Part II - Thinking and Feeling

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 February 2025

Bertram Malle
Affiliation:
Brown University, Rhode Island
Philip Robbins
Affiliation:
University of Missouri
Get access
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2025

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Baldassano, C., Chen, J., Zadbood, A., Pillow, J. W., Hasson, U., & Norman, K. A. (2017). Discovering event structure in continuous narrative perception and memory. Neuron, 95(3), 709721.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bandura, A., Barbaranelli, C., Caprara, G. V., & Pastorelli, C. (1996). Mechanisms of moral disengagement in the exercise of moral agency. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(2), 364374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron, J. (1994). Nonconsequentialist decisions. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 17(1), 110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron, J., & Spranca, M. (1997). Protected values. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 70(1), 116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Batson, C. D., Kobrynowicz, D., Dinnerstein, J. L., Kampf, H. C., & Wilson, A. D. (1997). In a very different voice: Unmasking moral hypocrisy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72(6), 13351348.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Batson, C. D., Thompson, E. R., & Chen, H. (2002). Moral hypocrisy: Addressing some alternatives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(2), 330339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Białek, M., Terbeck, S., & Handley, S. (2014). Cognitive psychological support for the ADC model of moral judgment. AJOB Neuroscience, 5(4), 2123.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Binmore, K. (2011). Natural justice. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Blake, P. R., McAuliffe, K., Corbit, J., Callaghan, T. C., Barry, O., Bowie, A., Kleutsch, L., Kramer, K. L., Ross, E., Vongsachang, H., Wrangham, R., & Warneken, F. (2015). The ontogeny of fairness in seven societies. Nature, 528(7581), 258261.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloom, P. (2017). Empathy and its discontents. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(1), 2431.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buckholtz, J. W., & Marois, R. (2012). The roots of modern justice: Cognitive and neural foundations of social norms and their enforcement. Nature Neuroscience, 15(5), 655661.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chakroff, A., Thomas, K. A., Haque, O. S., & Young, L. (2015). An indecent proposal: The dual functions of indirect speech. Cognitive Science, 39(1), 199211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Conway, P., & Peetz, J. (2012). When does feeling moral actually make you a better person? Conceptual abstraction moderates whether past moral deeds motivate consistency or compensatory behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(7), 907919.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cushman, F. (2013). Action, outcome, and value: A dual-system framework for morality. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 17(3), 273292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cushman, F., & Young, L. (2011). Patterns of moral judgment derive from nonmoral psychological representations. Cognitive Science, 35(6), 10521075.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Decety, J., Chen, C., Harenski, C., & Kiehl, K. A. (2013). An fMRI study of affective perspective taking in individuals with psychopathy: Imagining another in pain does not evoke empathy. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, Article 489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Deutsch, M. (1975). Equity, equality, and need: What determines which value will be used as the basis of distributive justice? Journal of Social Issues, 31(3), 137149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Duff, R. A. (1993). Choice, character, and criminal liability. Law and Philosophy, 12(4), 345383.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dunham, Y., Baron, A. S., & Carey, S. (2011). Consequences of “minimal” group affiliations in children. Child Development, 82(3), 793811.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ericsson, K. A., & Simon, H. A. (1984). Protocol analysis: Verbal reports as data. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fischbacher, U., & Föllmi-Heusi, F. (2013). Lies in disguise—an experimental study on cheating. Journal of the European Economic Association, 11(3), 525547.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gaus, J. & Thrasher, J. (2022). Philosophy, politics, and economics. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Gawronski, B., Armstrong, J., Conway, P., Friesdorf, R., & Hütter, M. (2017). Consequences, norms, and generalized inaction in moral dilemmas: The CNI model of moral decision-making. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 113(3), 343376.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glenn, A. L., Koleva, S., Iyer, R., Graham, J., & Ditto, P. H. (2010). Moral identity in psychopathy. Judgment and Decision Making, 5(7), 497505.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gollwitzer, P. M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54(7), 493503.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graham, J., Haidt, J., & Nosek, B. A. (2009). Liberals and conservatives rely on different sets of moral foundations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(5), 10291046.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Graham, J., Meindl, P., Koleva, S., Iyer, R., & Johnson, K. M. (2015). When values and behavior conflict: Moral pluralism and intrapersonal moral hypocrisy. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 9(3), 158170.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Graham, J., Nosek, B. A., Haidt, J., Iyer, R., Koleva, S., & Ditto, P. H. (2011). Mapping the moral domain. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(2), 366385.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greene, J. D. (2008). The secret joke of Kant’s soul. In Sinnott-Armstrong, W. (Ed.), Moral psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 3580). MIT Press.Google Scholar
Greene, J. D., Cushman, F. A., Stewart, L. E., Lowenberg, K., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D. (2009). Pushing moral buttons: The interaction between personal force and intention in moral judgment. Cognition, 111(3), 364371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greene, J. D., & Haidt, J. (2002). How (and where) does moral judgment work? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(12), 517523.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haidt, J. (2012). The righteous mind: Why good people are divided by politics and religion. Vintage.Google Scholar
Hannikainen, I. R., Miller, R. M., & Cushman, F. A. (2017). Act versus impact: Conservatives and liberals exhibit different structural emphases in moral judgment. Ratio, 30(4), 462493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henne, P., Niemi, L., Pinillos, Á., De Brigard, F., & Knobe, J. (2019). A counterfactual explanation for the action effect in causal judgment. Cognition, 190, 157164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hooker, B. (2000). Ideal code, real world: A rule-consequentialist theory of morality. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Janoff-Bulman, R., Sheikh, S., & Hepp, S. (2009). Proscriptive versus prescriptive morality: Two faces of moral regulation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(3), 521537.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, E. J., & Payne, J. W. (1985). Effort and accuracy in choice. Management Science, 31(4), 395414.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. Macmillan.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., Slovic, S. P., Slovic, P., & Tversky, A. (Eds.). (1982). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kimbrough, E. O., & Vostroknutov, A. (2018). A portable method of eliciting respect for social norms. Economics Letters, 168, 147150.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klar, S., & McCoy, A. (2021). Partisan‐motivated evaluations of sexual misconduct and the mitigating role of the #MeToo movement. American Journal of Political Science, 65(4), 777789.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kurby, C. A., & Zacks, J. M. (2008). Segmentation in the perception and memory of events. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(2), 7279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
MacLeod, C. M. (1991). Half a century of research on the Stroop effect: An integrative review. Psychological Bulletin, 109(2), 163203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marsh, A. A. (2019). The caring continuum: Evolved hormonal and proximal mechanisms explain prosocial and antisocial extremes. Annual Review of Psychology, 70, 347371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McAuliffe, K., Blake, P. R., Steinbeis, N., & Warneken, F. (2017). The developmental foundations of human fairness. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(2), 19.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moll, J., de Oliveira-Souza, R., Bramati, I. E., & Grafman, J. (2002). Functional networks in emotional moral and nonmoral social judgments. NeuroImage, 16(3, Part A), 696703.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Moore, A. B., Stevens, J., & Conway, A. R. (2011). Individual differences in sensitivity to reward and punishment predict moral judgment. Personality and Individual Differences, 50(5), 621625.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newman, G. E., Bloom, P., & Knobe, J. (2014). Value judgments and the true self. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40(2), 203216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nichols, S. (2021). Rational rules: Towards a theory of moral learning. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Niemi, L., Doris, J. M., & Graham, J. (2023). Who attributes what to whom? Moral values and relational context shape causal attributions to the person or the situation. Cognition, 232, Article 105332.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Niemi, L., Wasserman, E., & Young, L. (2017). The behavioral and neural signatures of distinct conceptions of fairness. Social Neuroscience, 13(4), 399415.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Niemi, L., & Young, L. (2017). Who sees what as fair? Mapping individual differences in valuation of reciprocity, charity, and impartiality. Social Justice Research, 30(4), 438449.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Orwell, G. (1946). Politics and the English language. Penguin Classics.Google Scholar
Petrinovich, L., & O’Neill, P. (1996). Influence of wording and framing effects on moral intuitions. Ethology and Sociobiology, 17(3), 145171.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phillips, J., & Cushman, F. (2017). Morality constrains the default representation of what is possible. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(18), 46494654.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rawls, J. (1971). A theory of justice. Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rawls, J. (2001). Justice as fairness: A restatement. Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ritov, I., & Baron, J. (1992). Status-quo and omission biases, Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 5, 4961.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singer, P. (2005). Ethics and intuitions. The Journal of Ethics, 9(3), 331352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spranca, M., Minsk, E., & Baron, J. (1991). Omission and commission in judgment and choice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 27(1), 76105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tajfel, H. (1982). Social psychology of intergroup relations. Annual Review of Psychology, 33, 139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Trivers, R. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism. Quarterly Review of Biology, 46, 3557.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Valdesolo, P., & DeSteno, D. (2007). Moral hypocrisy: Social groups and the flexibility of virtue. Psychological Science, 18(8), 689690.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Valdesolo, P., & DeSteno, D. (2008). The duality of virtue: Deconstructing the moral hypocrite. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 44(5), 13341338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vallacher, R. R., & Wegner, D. M. (1989). Levels of personal agency: Individual variation in action identification. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 57(4), 660671.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Lange, P. A. M. (1999). The pursuit of joint outcomes and equality in outcomes: An integrative model of social value orientation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(2), 337349.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Van Lange, P. A. M., Otten, W., De Bruin, E. M. N., & Joireman, J. A. (1997). Development of prosocial, individualistic, and competitive orientations: Theory and preliminary evidence. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(4), 733746.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Lange, P. A. M., Schippers, M., & Balliet, D. (2011). Who volunteers in psychology experiments? An empirical review of prosocial motivation in volunteering. Personality and Individual Differences, 51(3), 279284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Willemsen, P., & Reuter, K. (2016). Is there really an omission effect? Philosophical Psychology, 29(8), 11421159.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yoder, K. J., & Decety, J. (2014). The good, the bad, and the just: Justice sensitivity predicts neural response during moral evaluation of actions performed by others. Journal of Neuroscience, 34(12), 41614166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, L., Camprodon, J. A., Hauser, M., Pascual-Leone, A., & Saxe, R. (2010). Disruption of the right temporoparietal junction with transcranial magnetic stimulation reduces the role of beliefs in moral judgments. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(15), 67536758.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Young, L., Cushman, F., Hauser, M., & Saxe, R. (2007). The neural basis of the interaction between theory of mind and moral judgment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(20), 82358240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zacks, J. M., & Swallow, K. M. (2007). Event segmentation. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16(2), 8084.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zaki, J., & Cikara, M. (2015). Addressing empathic failures. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(6), 471476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zenon, A., Solopchuk, O., & Pezzulo, G. (2019). An information-theoretic perspective on the costs of cognition. Neuropsychologia, 123, 518.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

References

Adelson, J. (1971). The political imagination of the young adolescent. Daedalus, 100, 10131050.Google Scholar
Asch, D., Baron, J., Hershey, J. C., Kunreuther, H. C., Meszaros, J., Ritov, I., & Spranca, M. (1994). Omission bias and pertussis vaccination. Medical Decision Making, 14(2), 118123.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bago, B., & De Neys, W. (2019). The intuitive greater good: Testing the corrective dual process model of moral cognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148(10), 17821801.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron, J. (1992). The effect of normative beliefs on anticipated emotions. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 63(2), 320330.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron, J. (1993). Why teach thinking? – An essay. Applied Psychology: An International Review, 42(3), 191237.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron, J. (1995). Blind justice: Fairness to groups and the do-no-harm principle. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 8(2), 7183.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron, J. (2012a). Parochialism as a result of cognitive biases. In Goodman, R., Jinks, D., & Woods, A. K. (Eds.), Understanding social action, promoting human rights (pp. 203243). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron, J. (2012b). The “culture of honor” in citizens’ concepts of their duty as voters. Rationality and Society, 24(1), 3772.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron, J., & Gürçay, B. (2017). A meta-analysis of response-time tests of the sequential two-systems model of moral judgment. Memory and Cognition, 45(4), 566575.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Baron, J., Isler, O., & Yilmaz, O. (2023). Actively open-minded thinking and the political effects of its absence. In Ottati, V. & Stern, C. (Eds.), Divided: Open-mindedness and dogmatism in a polarized world (pp. 162182). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron, J., & Jurney, J. (1993). Norms against voting for coerced reform. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64(3), 347355.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron, J., & Leshner, S. (2000). How serious are expressions of protected values. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 6(3), 183194.Google ScholarPubMed
Baron, J., & Ritov, I. (1993). Intuitions about penalties and compensation in the context of tort law. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 7, 1733.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron, J., & Ritov, I. (1994). Reference points and omission bias. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 59(3), 475498.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baron, J., & Ritov, I. (2009). Protected values and omission bias as deontological judgments. In Bartels, D. M., Bauman, C. W., Skitka, L. J., & Medin, D. L. (Eds.), Moral judgment and decision making, Vol. 50 in B. H. Ross (Series Ed.), The psychology of learning and motivation (pp. 133167). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Baron, J., Ritov, I., & Greene, J. D. (2013). The duty to support nationalistic policies. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 26(2), 128138.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bell, D. E., Raiffa, H., & Tversky, A. (Eds.). (1988). Decision making: Descriptive, normative, and prescriptive interactions. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bhatia, S., Walasek, L., Slovic, P., & Kunreuther, H. (2021). The more who die, the less we care: Evidence from natural language analysis of online news articles and social media posts. Risk Analysis, 41(1), 179203.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bornstein, G., & Ben-Yossef, M. (1994). Cooperation in intergroup and single-group social dilemmas. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 30(1), 5257.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Breyer, S. (1993). Breaking the vicious circle: Toward effective risk regulation. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Bruner, J. S., Goodnow, J. J., & Austin, G. A. (1956). A study of thinking. Wiley.Google Scholar
Buchan, N. R., Brewer, M., Grimalda, G., Wilson, R., Fatas, E., & Foddy, M. (2011). Global social identity and global cooperation. Psychological Science, 22(6), 821828.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buchan, N. R., Grimalda, G., Wilson, R., Brewer, M., Fatas, E., & Foddy, M. (2009). Globalization and human cooperation. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 106(11), 41384142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Camerer, C. F. (2000). Prospect theory in the wild: Evidence from the field. In Kahneman, D. & Tversky, A. (Eds.), Choices, values, and frames (pp. 288300). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Caplan, B. (2007). The myth of the rational voter: Why democracies choose bad policies. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Carlsmith, K. M., Darley, J. M., & Robinson, P. H. (2002). Why do we punish? Deterrence and just deserts as motives for punishment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(2), 284299.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chapman, L. J., & Chapman, J. P. (1969). Illusory correlation as an obstacle to the use of valid psychodiagnostic signs. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 74(3), 271280.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cushman, F. (2008). Crime and punishment: Distinguishing the roles of causal and intentional analyses in moral judgment. Cognition, 108(2), 353380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cushman, F., & Young, L. (2011). Patterns of moral judgment derive from nonmoral psychological representations. Cognitive Science, 35(6), 10521075.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deutsch, M. (1975). Equity, equality, and need: What determines which value will be used as the basis of distributive justice? Journal of Social Issues, 31(3), 137149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Edlin, A., Gelman, A., & Kaplan, N. (2007). Voting as a rational choice: Why and how people vote to improve the well-being of others. Rationality and Society, 19(3), 293314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellsberg, D. (1961). Risk, ambiguity, and the Savage axioms. Quarterly Journal of Economics, 75(4), 643699.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Foot, P. (1978). The problem of abortion and the doctrine of the double effect. In Foot, P., Virtues and vices and other essays in moral philosophy (pp. 1932). University of California Press. (Originally published 1967 in Oxford Review, no. 5)Google Scholar
Gigerenzer, G., Todd, P. M., & the ABC Research Group (1999). Simple heuristics that make us smart. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Greene, J. D. (2008). The secret joke of Kant’s soul. In Sinnott-Armstrong, W. (Ed.), Moral psychology: Vol. 3. The neuroscience of morality: Emotion, brain disorders, and development (pp. 3679). MIT Press.Google Scholar
Greene, J. D., Cushman, F. A., Stewart, L. E., Lowenberg, K., Nystrom, L. E., & Cohen, J. D. (2009). Pushing moral buttons: The interaction between personal force and intention in moral judgment. Cognition, 111(3), 364371.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gürçay, B., & Baron, J. (2017). Challenges for the sequential two-systems model of moral judgment. Thinking and Reasoning, 23(1), 4980.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haidt, J., Koller, S. H., & Dias, M. G. (1993). Affect, culture, and morality, or, is it wrong to eat your dog? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(4), 613628.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hallpike, C. R. (2004). The evolution of moral understanding. Prometheus Research Group.Google Scholar
Hanselmann, M., & Tanner, C. (2008). Taboos and conflicts in decision making: Sacred values, decision difficulty, and emotions. Judgment and Decision Making, 3(1), 5163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hare, R. M. (1981). Moral thinking: Its levels, method, and point. Oxford University Press (Clarendon Press).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henle, M. (1962). On the relation between logic and thinking. Psychological Review, 69(4), 366378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harris, R. J., & Joyce, M. A. (1980). What’s fair? It depends on how you phrase the question. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 38(1), 165179.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huang, K., Bernhard, R. M., Barak-Corren, N., Bazerman, M. H., & Greene, J. D. (2021). Veil-of-ignorance reasoning mitigates self-serving bias in resource allocation during the COVID-19 crisis. Judgment and Decision Making, 16(1), 119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Idson, L. C., Chugh, D., Bereby-Meyer, Y., Moran, S., Grosskopf, B., & Bazerman, M. (2004). Overcoming focusing failures in competitive environments. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 17(3), 159172.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Johnson, E. J., Hershey, J. C., Meszaros, J., & Kunreuther, H. (1993). Framing, probability distortions, and insurance decisions. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 7, 3551.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, S. K., Frisch, D., Yurok, T. J., & Kim, E. (1998). Choices and opportunities: Another effect of framing on decisions. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 11(3), 211226.3.0.CO;2-O>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. Farrar, Strauss and Giroux.Google Scholar
Kahneman, D., & Frederick, S. (2002). Representativeness revisited: Attribute substitution in intuitive judgment. In Gilovich, T., Griffin, D., & Kahneman, D. (Eds.), Heuristics and biases: The psychology of intuitive judgment (pp. 4981). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D., & Lovallo, D. (1993). Timid choices and bold forecasts: A cognitive perspective on risk taking. Management Science, 39(1), 1731.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kahneman, D., & Ritov, I. (1994). Determinants of stated willingness to pay for public goods: A study of the headline method. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 9, 538.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decision under risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kohlberg, L. (1963). The development of children’s orientations toward a moral order. I. Sequence in the development of human thought. Vita Humana, 6(1–2), 1133.Google Scholar
Koop, G. J. (2013). An assessment of the temporal dynamics of moral decisions. Judgment and Decision Making, 8(5), 527539.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kunreuther, H., & Slovic, P. (1978). Economics, psychology, and protective behavior. American Economic Review, 68(2), 6469.Google Scholar
Legrenzi, P., Girotto, V., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1993). Focussing in reasoning and decision making. Cognition, 49(1–2), 3766.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malle, B. F. (2021). Moral judgments. Annual Review of Psychology, 72, 293318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McCaffery, E. J. (1997). Taxing women. University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCaffery, E. J., & Baron, J. (2004). Framing and taxation: Evaluation of tax policies involving household composition. Journal of Economic Psychology, 25(6), 679705.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McCaffery, E. J., & Baron, J. (2006). Isolation effects and the neglect of indirect effects of fiscal policies. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 19(4), 114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meehl, P. E. (1954). Clinical versus statistical prediction: A theoretical analysis and a look at the evidence. University of Minnesota Press.Google Scholar
Metz, S. E., Baelen, R. N., & Yu, A. (2020). Actively open-minded thinking in American adolescents. Review of Education, 8(3), 768799.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mill, J. S. (1859). On liberty. J. W. Parker & Son.Google Scholar
Patil, I., Zucchelli, M. M., Kool, W., Campbell, S., Fornasier, F., Calò, M., … Cushman, F. (2021). Reasoning supports utilitarian resolutions to moral dilemmas across diverse measures. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 120(2), 443460.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piaget, J. (1932). The moral judgment of the child. The Free Press.Google Scholar
Polya, G. (1945). How to solve it: A new aspect of mathematical method. Princeton University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rawls, J. (1971). A theory of justice. Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Read, D., Loewenstein, G., & Rabin, M. (1999). Choice bracketing. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 19, 171197.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ritov, I., & Baron, J. (1990). Reluctance to vaccinate: Omission bias and ambiguity. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 3(4), 263277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosas, A., & Aguilar-Pardo, D. (2020). Extreme time-pressure reveals utilitarian intuitions in sacrificial dilemmas. Thinking and Reasoning, 26(4), 534551.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosas, A., Bermúdez, J. P., & Aguilar-Pardo, D. (2019). Decision conflict drives reaction times and utilitarian responses in sacrificial dilemmas. Judgment and Decision Making, 14(5), 555564.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ross, W. D. (1930). The right and the good. (Reprinted 2002 by Oxford University Press)Google Scholar
Roth, A. E. (2007). Repugnance as a constraint on markets. Journal of Economic Perspectives, 21(3), 3758.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Royzman, E. B., & Baron, J. (2002). The preference for indirect harm. Social Justice Research, 15(2), 165184.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Savage, L. J. (1954). The foundations of statistics. Wiley.Google Scholar
Sherman, G. D., Vallen, B., Finkelstein, S. R., Connell, P. M., Boland, W. A., & Feemster, K. (2021). When taking action means accepting responsibility: Omission bias predicts parents’ reluctance to vaccinate due to greater anticipated culpability for negative side effects. Journal of Consumer Affairs, 55(4), 16601681.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singer, P. (1982). The expanding circle: Ethics and sociobiology. Farrar, Strauss & Giroux.Google Scholar
Singer, P. (2002). R. M. Hare’s achievements in moral philosophy. Utilitas, 14(3), 309317.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sloman, S. A. (1996). The empirical case for two systems of reasoning. Psychological Bulletin, 119(1), 322.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slovic, P. (2007). “If I look at the mass I will never act”: Psychic numbing and genocide. Judgment and Decision Making, 2(2), 7995.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Snarey, J., & Samuelson, P. (2008). Moral education in the cognitive developmental tradition: Lawrence Kohlberg’s revolutionary ideas. In Nucci, L. P. & Narvaez, D. (Eds.), Handbook of moral and character education (pp. 5379). Routledge.Google Scholar
Spranca, M., Minsk, E., & Baron, J. (1991). Omission and commission in judgment and choice. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 27(1), 76105.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suedfeld, P. (1985). APA presidential addresses: The relation of integrative complexity to historical, professional, and personal factors. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 49(6), 16431651.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suedfeld, P., Guttieri, K., & Tetlock, P. E. (2003). Assessing integrative complexity at a distance: Archival analyses of thinking and decision making. In Post, J. M. (Ed.), The psychological assessment of political leaders: With profiles of Saddam Hussein and Bill Clinton (pp. 246270). University of Michigan Press.Google Scholar
Sunstein, C. R. (2002). Risk and reason: Safety, law, and the environment. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sunstein, C. R. (2005). Moral heuristics (with commentary). Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(4), 531573.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tetlock, P. E. (2003). Thinking the unthinkable: Sacred values and taboo cognitions Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(7), 320324.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tetlock, P. E., Mellers, B. A., & Scoblic, J. P. (2017). Sacred versus pseudo-sacred values: How people cope with taboo trade-offs. American Economic Review, 107(5), 9699.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thaler, R. H., & Sunstein, C. R. (2008). Nudge: Improving decisions about health, wealth, and happiness. Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Tversky, A. (1967). Additivity, utility, and subjective probability. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 4(2), 175202.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: Heuristics and biases. Science, 185(4157), 11241131.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1981). The framing of decisions and the psychology of choice. Science, 211(4481), 453458.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ubel, P. A., & Loewenstein, G. (1996). Distributing scarce livers: The moral reasoning of the general public. Social Science and Medicine, 42(7), 10491055.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

References

Abele, A. E., & Wojciszke, B. (2007). Agency and communion from the perspective of self versus others. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(5), 751763.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Akechi, H., & Hietanen, J. K. (2021). Considering victims’ minds in the evaluation of harmful agents’ moral standing. Social Cognition, 39(4), 489503.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alicke, M. D. (2000). Culpable control and the psychology of blame. Psychological Bulletin, 126(4), 556574.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Anderson, R. A., Crockett, M. J., & Pizarro, D. A. (2020). A theory of moral praise. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 24(9), 694703.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Arico, A. (2010). Folk psychology, consciousness, and context effects. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 1(3), 371393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arico, A. (2012). Breaking out of moral typecasting. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 3(3), 425438.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arico, A., Fiala, B., Goldberg, R. F., & Nichols, S. (2011). The folk psychology of consciousness. Mind & Language, 26(3), 327352.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arpaly, N. (2003). Unprincipled virtue. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Barrett, J. L., & Keil, F. C. (1996). Conceptualizing a nonnatural entity: Anthropomorphism in God concepts. Cognitive Psychology, 31(3), 219247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bastian, B., Costello, K., Loughnan, S., & Hodson, G. (2012). When closing the human–animal divide expands moral concern: The importance of framing. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3(4), 421429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bastian, B., Crimston, C. R., Klebl, C., & van Lange, P. A. M. (2023). The moral significance of protecting environmental and cultural objects. PLoS ONE, 18(2), Article e0280393.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bastian, B., Denson, T. F., & Haslam, N. (2013). The roles of dehumanization and moral outrage in distributive justice. PLoS ONE, 8(4), Article e61842.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bastian, B., Laham, S. M., Wilson, S., Haslam, N., & Koval, P. (2011). Blaming, praising, and protecting our humanity: The implications of everyday dehumanization for judgments of moral status. British Journal of Social Psychology, 50(3), 469483.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bastian, B., Loughnan, S., Haslam, N., & Radke, H. R. M. (2012). Don’t mind meat? The denial of mind to animals used for human consumption. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 38(2), 247256.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bentham, J. (1970). Introduction to the principles of morals and legislation. Clarendon Press. (Original work published 1789)Google Scholar
Bernstein, M. H. (1998). On moral considerability: An essay on who morally matters. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bigman, Y. E., Waytz, A., Alterovitz, R., & Gray, K. (2019). Holding robots responsible: The elements of machine morality. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(5), 365368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Block, N. (1995). On a confusion about a function of consciousness. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 18(2), 227247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bonnefon, J.-F., Rahwan, I., & Shariff, A. (2024). The moral psychology of artificial intelligence. Annual Review of Psychology, 75, 653675.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brambilla, M, & Leach, C. W. (2014). On the importance of being moral: The distinctive role of morality in social judgment. Social Cognition, 32(4), 397408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bratanova, B., Loughnan, S., & Bastian, B. (2011). The effect of categorization as food on the perceived moral standing of animals. Appetite, 57(1), 193196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buckwalter, W., & Phelan, M. (2013). Function and feeling machines: A defense of the philosophical conception of subjective experience. Philosophical Studies, 166(2), 349361.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Callicott, J. B. (1980). Animal liberation. Environmental Ethics, 2(4), 311338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Capezza, N. M., & Arriaga, X. B. (2008). Why do people blame victims of abuse? The role of stereotypes of women on perceptions of blame. Sex Roles, 59(11), 839850.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carruthers, P. (1992). The animals issue: Moral theory in practice. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chalmers, D. (1995). Facing up to the problem of consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 2(3), 200219.Google Scholar
Cushman, F. (2008). Crime and punishment: Distinguishing the roles of causal and intentional analyses in moral judgment. Cognition, 108(2), 353380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
DeGrazia, D. (2008). Moral status as a matter of degree? Southern Journal of Philosophy, 46(2), 181198.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
de Vel-Palumbo, M., Schein, C., Ferguson, R., Chang, M. X-L., & Bastian, B. (2021). Morally excused but socially excluded: Denying agency through the defense of mental impairment. PLoS ONE, 16(6), Article e0252586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epley, N., & Waytz, A. (2010). Mind perception. In Fiske, S. T., Gilbert, D. T., & Lindzey, G. (Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (pp. 498541). Wiley.Google Scholar
Fischer, J. M., & Ravizza, M. (1998). Responsibility and control: A theory of moral responsibility. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., & Glick, P. (2006). Universal dimensions of social cognition: Warmth and competence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(2), 7783.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiske, S. T., Cuddy, A. J. C., Glick, P., & Xu, J. (2002). A model of (often mixed) stereotype content: Competence and warmth respectively follow from perceived status and competition. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 82(6), 878902.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fragale, A. R., Rosen, B., Xu, C., & Merideth, I. (2009). The higher they are, the harder they fall: The effects of wrongdoer status on observer punishment recommendations and intentionality attributions. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 108(1), 5365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Friedman, M. (2013). How to blame people responsibly. Journal of Value Inquiry, 47(3), 271284.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodpaster, K. E. (1978). On being morally considerable. Journal of Philosophy, 75(6), 308325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, G. P. (2015). Experimental approaches to moral standing. Philosophy Compass, 10(12), 914926.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, G. P., Piazza, J., & Rozin, P. (2014). Moral character predominates in person perception and evaluation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 106(1), 148168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, H. M., Gray, K., & Wegner, D. (2007). Dimensions of mind perception. Science, 315(5812), 619.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, K., Young, L., & Waytz, A. (2012). Mind perception is the essence of morality. Psychological Inquiry, 23(2), 101124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gray, K., & Wegner, D. (2009). Moral typecasting: Divergent perceptions of moral agents and moral patients. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 96(3), 503520.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, K., & Wegner, D. (2011). To escape blame, don’t be a hero – Be a victim. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 47(2), 516519.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Greene, J. (2008). The secret joke of Kant’s soul. In Sinnott-Armstrong, W. (Ed.), Moral psychology: Vol. 3. The neuroscience of morality: Emotion, brain disorders, and development (pp. 3580). MIT Press.Google Scholar
Greene, J., & Cohen, J. (2004). For the law, neuroscience changes nothing and everything. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B, 359(1451), 17751785.Google ScholarPubMed
Guglielmo, S., Monroe, A. E., and Malle, B. F. (2009). At the heart of morality lies folk psychology. Inquiry, 52(5), 449466.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gunnthorsdottir, A. (2001). Physical attractiveness of an animal species as a decision factor for its preservation. Anthrozoös, 14(4), 204215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haslam, N. (2006). Dehumanization: An integrative review. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 10(3), 252264.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haslam, N., & Loughnan, S. (2014). Dehumanization and infrahumanization. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 399423.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Huebner, B. (2010). Commonsense concepts of phenomenal consciousness: Does anyone care about functional zombies? Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 9(1), 133155.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jack, A. I., & Robbins, P. (2012). The phenomenal stance revisited. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 3(3), 383403.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kant, I. (1998). Groundwork of the metaphysics of morals (M. Gregor, Ed. and Trans.). Cambridge University Press. (Original work published 1785)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kant, I. (1996). The metaphysics of morals (M. Gregor, Ed. and Trans.). Cambridge University Press. (Original work published 1797)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Khamitov, M., Rotman, J. D., & Piazza, J. (2016). Perceiving the agency of harmful agents: A test of the dehumanization versus moral typecasting accounts. Cognition, 146(1), 3347.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klebl, C., Luo, Y., & Bastian, B. (2022). Beyond aesthetic judgment: Beauty increases moral standing through perceptions of purity. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 48(6), 954967.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klebl, C., Luo, Y., Tan, N. P, Ping Ern, J. T., & Bastian, B. (2021). Beauty of the Beast: Beauty as an important dimension in the moral standing of animals. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 75, Article 101624.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Knobe, J., & Prinz, J. (2008). Intuitions about consciousness: Experimental studies. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 7(1), 6783.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kozachenko, H. H., & Piazza, J. (2021). How children and adults value different animal lives. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 210, Article 105204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ladak, A., Loughnan, S., & Wilks, M. (2023). The moral psychology of artificial intelligence. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 33(1), 2734.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loughnan, S., Haslam, N., & Bastian, B. (2010). The role of meat consumption in the denial of moral status and mind to meat animals. Appetite, 55(1), 156159.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malle, B. F. (2019). How many dimensions of mind perception really are there? In Goel, A. K., Seifert, C. M., & Freska, C. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 41st Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 22682274). Cognitive Science Society.Google Scholar
Monroe, A. E., Dillon, K. D., & Malle, B. F. (2014). Bringing free will down to earth: People’s psychological concept of free will and its role in moral judgment. Consciousness and Cognition, 27, 100108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nadler, J. (2012). Blaming as a social process: The influence of character and moral emotion on blame. Law and Contemporary Problems, 75(2), 131.Google Scholar
Nadler, J., & McDonnell, M.-H. (2012). Moral character, motive, and the psychology of blame. Cornell Law Review, 97(2), 255304.Google Scholar
Nagel, T. (1974). What is it like to be a bat? Philosophical Review, 83(4), 435450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nijssen, S. R. R., Müller, B. C. N., Bosse, T., & Paulus, M. (2023). Can you count on a calculator? The role of agency and affect in judgments of robots as moral agents. Human-Computer Interaction, 38(5–6), 400416.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Opotow, S. (1993). Animals and the scope of justice. Journal of Social Issues, 49(1), 7185.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Phelan, M., Arico, A., & Nichols, S. (2013). Thinking things and feeling things: On an alleged discontinuity in folk metaphysics of mind. Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences, 12(4), 703725.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piazza, J., Landy, J. F., & Goodwin, G. P. (2014). Cruel nature: Harmfulness as an important, overlooked dimension in judgments of moral standing. Cognition, 131(1), 108124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piazza, J., & Loughnan, S. (2016). When meat gets personal, animals’ minds matter less: Motivated use of intelligence information in judgments of moral standing. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 7(8), 867874.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pizarro, D., & Tannenbaum, D. (2012). Bringing character back: How the motivation to evaluate character influences judgments of moral blame. In Mikulincer, M. & Shaver, P. R. (Eds.), The social psychology of morality: Exploring the causes of good and evil (pp. 91108). American Psychological Association.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Raz, J. (1984). On the nature of rights. Mind, 93(370), 194214.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robbins, P., & Alvear, F. (2023). Deformative experience: Explaining the effects of adversity on moral evaluation. Social Cognition, 41(5), 415446.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robbins, P., & Jack, A. I. (2006). The phenomenal stance. Philosophical Studies, 127(1), 5985.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robbins, P., & Litton, P. (2018). Crime, punishment, and causation: The effect of etiological information on the perception of moral agency. Psychology, Public Policy, and Law, 24(1), 118127.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schein, C., & Gray, K. (2018). The Theory of Dyadic Morality: Reinventing moral judgments by redefining harm. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 22(1), 3270.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schtulman, A., & Lindeman, M. (2016). Attributes of God: Conceptual foundations of a foundational belief. Cognitive Science, 40(3), 635670.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schwartz, F., Djeriouat, H., & Trémolière, B. (2022). Agents’ moral character shapes people’s moral evaluations of accidental harm transgressions. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 102, Article 104378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singer, P. (1989). All animals are equal. In Regan, T. and Singer, P. (Eds.), Animal rights and human obligations (pp. 215226). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Strawson, P. F. (1962). Freedom and resentment. Proceedings of the British Academy, 48, 187211.Google Scholar
Sytsma, J., & Machery, E. (2009). How to study folk intuitions about phenomenal consciousness. Philosophical Psychology, 22(1), 2135.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sytsma, J., & Machery, E. (2010). Two conceptions of subjective experience. Philosophical Studies, 151(2), 299327.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sytsma, J., & Machery, E. (2012). The two sources of moral standing. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 3(3), 303324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Todd, P. (2019). A unified account of the moral standing to blame. Noûs, 53(2), 347374.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vetter, B. (2013). ‘Can’ without possible worlds: Semantics for anti-Humeans. Philosophers’ Imprint, 13(16), 127.Google Scholar
Vollmer, F. (1993). A theory of traits. Philosophical Psychology, 6(1), 6779.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ward, A. F., Olsen, A. S., & Wegner, D. M. (2013). The harm-made mind: Observing victimization augments attributions of minds to vegetative patients, robots, and the dead. Psychological Science, 24(8), 14371445.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Waytz, A., Gray, K., Epley, N., & Wegner, D. M. (2010). Causes and consequences of mind perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(8), 383388.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weisman, K., Dweck, C. S., & Markman, E. M. (2017). Rethinking people’s conceptions of mental life. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(43), 1137411379.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weisman, K., Legare, C. H., Smith, R. E., Dzokoto, V. A., Aulino, F., Ng, E., Dulin, J. C., Ross-Zehnder, N., Brahinsky, J. D., & Luhrmann, T. M. (2021). Similarities and differences in concepts of mental life among adults and children in five cultures. Nature Human Behaviour, 5(10), 13581368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

References

Algoe, S. B., & Haidt, J. (2009). Witnessing excellence in action: The ‘other-praising’ emotions of elevation, gratitude, and admiration. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 4(2), 105127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alicke, M. D. (2000). Culpable control and the psychology of blame. Psychological Bulletin, 126(4), 556574.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Allpress, J. A., Brown, R., Giner-Sorolla, R., Deonna, J. A., & Teroni, F. (2014). Two faces of group-based shame: Moral shame and image shame differentially predict positive and negative orientations to ingroup wrongdoing. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 40(10), 12701284.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aquino, K., Freeman, D., Reed, A. II, Lim, V. K. G., & Felps, W. (2009). Testing a social-cognitive model of moral behavior: The interactive influence of situations and moral identity centrality. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(1), 123141.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Averill, J. (1983). Studies on anger and aggression: Implications for theories of emotion. American Psychologist, 38(11), 11451160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Avramova, Y. R., & Inbar, Y. (2013). Emotion and moral judgment. WIREs Cognitive Science, 4(2), 169178.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aznar, A., Tenenbaum, H., & Russell, P. S. (2021). Is moral disgust socially learned? Emotion, 23(1), 289301.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, L., & Russell, J. (2015). The psychological construction of emotion. Guilford Publications.Google Scholar
Bartoș, S. E., Russell, P. S., & Hegarty, P. (2020). Heroes against homophobia: Does elevation uniquely block homophobia by inhibiting disgust? Cognition and Emotion, 34(6), 11231142.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Batson, C. D., & Ahmad, N. Y. (2009). Using empathy to improve intergroup attitudes and relations. Social Issues and Policy Review, 3(1), 141177.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Becker, J. C., Tausch, N., & Wagner, U. (2011). Emotional consequences of collective action participation: Differentiating self-directed and outgroup-directed emotions. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 37(12), 15871598.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloom, P. (2004). Descartes’ baby. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Bloom, P. (2017). Empathy and its discontents. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 21(1), 2431.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Branscombe, N. R., & Doosje, B. (2004). Collective guilt: International perspectives. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cameron, C. D., Hutcherson, C. A., Ferguson, A. M., Scheffer, J. A., Hadjiandreou, E., & Inzlicht, M. (2019). Empathy is hard work: People choose to avoid empathy because of its cognitive costs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 148(6), 962976.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carver, C. S., & Harmon-Jones, E. (2009). Anger is an approach-related affect: Evidence and implications. Psychological Bulletin, 135(2), 183204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chaudhury, S. H., Garg, N., & Jiang, Z. (2021). The curious case of threat-awe: A theoretical and empirical reconceptualization. Emotion, 22(7), 16531669.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cohen, T. R., Wolf, S. T., Panter, A. T., & Insko, C. A. (2011). Introducing the GASP scale: A new measure of guilt and shame proneness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(5), 947966.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cohen-Chen, S., Pliskin, R., & Goldenberg, A. (2020). Feel good or do good? A valence–function framework for understanding emotions. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 29(4), 388393.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cottrell, C. A., & Neuberg, S. L. (2005). Different emotional reactions to different groups: A sociofunctional threat-based approach to “prejudice.” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 88(5), 770789.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cova, F., Deonna, J., & Sander, D. (2013). The emotional shape of our moral life: Anger-related emotions and mutualistic anthropology. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 36(1), 8687.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowen, A. S., & Keltner, D. (2017). Self-report captures 27 distinct categories of emotion bridged by continuous gradients. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(38), E7900E7909.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crimston, C. R., Blessing, S., Gilbert, P., & Kirby, J. N. (2022). Fear leads to suffering: Fears of compassion predict restriction of the moral boundary. British Journal of Social Psychology, 61(1), 345365.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cuff, B. M. P., Brown, S. J., Taylor, L., & Howat, D. J. (2016). Empathy: A review of the concept. Emotion Review, 8(2), 144153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Danovitch, J., & Bloom, P. (2009). Children’s extension of disgust to physical and moral events. Emotion, 9(1), 107112.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Darley, J. M., Klosson, E. C., & Zanna, M. P. (1978). Intentions and their contexts in the moral judgments of children and adults. Child Development, 49(1), 6674.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Decety, J., & Jackson, P. L. (2004). The functional architecture of human empathy. Behavior Cognitive Neuroscience Review, 3(2), 71100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diessner, R., Iyer, R., Smith, M. M., & Haidt, J. (2013). Who engages with moral beauty? Journal of Moral Education, 42(2), 139163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Doosje, B., Branscombe, N. R., Spears, R., & Manstead, A. S. R. (1998). Guilty by association: When one’s group has a negative history. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75(4), 872886.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenberg, N. (1988). Empathy and sympathy: A brief review of the concepts and empirical literature. Anthrozoös, 2(1), 1517.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., & Spinrad, T. L. (2006). Prosocial development. In Eisenberg, N., Damon, W., & Lerner, R. M. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Vol. 3. Social, emotional and personality development (pp. 646718). Wiley.Google Scholar
Ekman, P. (1999). Basic emotions. John Wiley & Sons Ltd.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ekman, P., O’Sullivan, M., & Matsumoto, D. (1991). Contradictions in the study of contempt: What’s it all about? Reply to Russell. Motivation and Emotion, 15(4), 293296.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellemers, N., van der Toorn, J., Paunov, Y., & van Leeuwen, T. (2019). The psychology of morality: A review and analysis of empirical studies published from 1940 through 2017. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 23(4), 332366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eskine, K. J., Novreske, A., & Richards, M. (2013). Moral contagion effects in everyday interpersonal encounters. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(5), 947950.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fehr, B., & Russell, J. A. (1984). Concept of emotion viewed from a prototype perspective. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113(3), 464486.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, A., & Giner-Sorolla, R. (2016). Contempt: Derogating others while keeping calm. Emotion Review, 8(4), 346357.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fischer, A. H., & Roseman, I. J. (2007). Beat them or ban them: The characteristics and social functions of anger and contempt. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93(1), 103115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiske, A. P. (2020). The lexical fallacy in emotion research: Mistaking vernacular words for psychological entities. Psychological Review, 127(1), 95113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gausel, N., & Leach, C. W. (2011). Concern for self‐image and social image in the management of moral failure: Rethinking shame. European Journal of Social Psychology, 41(4), 468478.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ghelfi, E., Christopherson, C. D., Urry, H. L., Lenne, R. L., Legate, N., Fischer, M. A., Wagemans, F. M. A., Wiggins, B., Barrett, T., Bornstein, M., de Haan, B., Guberman, J., Issa, N., Kim, J., Na, E., O’Brien, J., Paulk, A., Peck, T., Sashihara, M., & Sullivan, D. (2020). Reexamining the effect of gustatory disgust on moral judgment: A multilab direct replication of Eskine, Kacinik, and Prinz (2011). Advances in Methods and Practices in Psychological Science, 3(1), 323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giner-Sorolla, R., & Chapman, H. A. (2017). Beyond purity: Moral disgust toward bad character. Psychological Science, 28(1), 8091.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giner-Sorolla, R., & Espinosa, P. (2011). Social cuing of guilt by anger and of shame by disgust. Psychological Science, 22(1), 4953.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giner-Sorolla, R., Piazza, J., & Espinosa, P. (2011). What do the TOSCA guilt and shame scales really measure: Affect or action? Personality and Individual Differences, 51(4), 445450.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giner-Sorolla, R., & Russell, P. S. (2019). Not just disgust: Fear and anger also relate to intergroup dehumanization. Collabra: Psychology, 5(1), Article 58.Google Scholar
Gocłowska, M. A., Elliot, A. J., van Elk, M., Bulska, D., Thorstenson, C. A., & Baas, M. (2023). Awe arises in reaction to exceeded rather than disconfirmed expectancies. Emotion, 23(1), 1529.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goetz, J. L., Keltner, D., & Simon-Thomas, E. (2010). Compassion: An evolutionary analysis and empirical review. Psychological Bulletin, 136(3), 351374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldberg, J. H., Lerner, J. S., & Tetlock, P. E. (1999). Rage and reason: The psychology of the intuitive prosecutor. European Journal of Social Psychology, 29(5–6), 781795.3.0.CO;2-3>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gottlieb, S., Keltner, D., & Lombrozo, T. (2018). Awe as a scientific emotion. Cognitive Science, 42(6), 20812094.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Guan, F., Chen, J., Chen, O., Liu, L., & Zha, Y. (2019). Awe and prosocial tendency. Current Psychology, 38(4), 10331041.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haidt, J. (2001). The emotional dog and its rational tail: A social intuitionist approach to moral judgment. Psychological Review, 108(4), 814834.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haidt, J. (2003). The moral emotions. In Davidson, R. J., Scherer, K. R., & Goldsmith, H. H. (Eds.), Handbook of affective sciences (pp. 852870). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Haidt, J. (2012). The righteous mind: Why good people are divided by politics and religion. Vintage Books.Google Scholar
Hodges, S. D., & Klein, K. J. K. (2001). Regulating the costs of empathy: The price of being human. The Journal of Socio-Economics, 30(5), 437452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Horberg, E. J., Oveis, C., Keltner, D., & Cohen, A. B. (2009). Disgust and the moralization of purity. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 97(6), 963976.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hutcherson, C. A., & Gross, J. J. (2011). The moral emotions: A social-functionalist account of anger, disgust, and contempt. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 100(4), 719737.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Inbar, Y., Pizarro, D. A., Gilovich, T., & Ariely, D. (2013). Moral masochism: On the connection between guilt and self-punishment. Emotion, 13(1), 1418.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iyer, A., Schmader, T., & Lickel, B. (2007). Why individuals protest the perceived transgressions of their country: The role of anger, shame and guilt. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 33(4), 572587.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Izard, C. E. (1971). The face of emotion. Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Izard, C. E. (1977). Differential emotions theory. In Izard, C. E. (Ed.), Human emotions (pp. 4366). Springer US.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Izard, C. E. (1991). The psychology of emotions. Springer Science & Business Media.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, A., & Fitness, J. (2008). Moral hypervigilance: The influence of disgust sensitivity in the moral domain. Emotion, 8(5), 613627.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Keltner, D., & Haidt, J. (2003). Approaching awe, a moral, spiritual, and aesthetic emotion. Cognition and Emotion, 17(2), 297314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klimecki, O. M. (2019). The role of empathy and compassion in conflict resolution. Emotion Review, 11(4), 310325.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kupfer, T. R., & Giner-Sorolla, R. (2021). Reputation management as an alternative explanation for the “contagiousness” of immorality. Evolution and Human Behavior, 42(2), 130139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuppens, P., Van Mechelen, I., & Meulders, M. (2004). Every cloud has a silver lining: Interpersonal and individual differences determinants of anger-related behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30(12), 15501564.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kuppens, P., Van-Mechelen, I., Smits, D. J. M., De Boek, P., & Ceulemans, E. (2007). Individual differences in patterns of appraisal and anger experience. Cognition and Emotion, 21(4), 689713.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lai, C. K., Haidt, J., & Nosek, B. A. (2014). Moral elevation reduces prejudice against gay men. Cognition & Emotion, 28(5), 781794.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Landy, J. F., & Goodwin, G. P. (2015). Does incidental disgust amplify moral judgment? A meta-analytic review of experimental evidence. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 10(4), 518536.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lazarus, R. S. (1991). Emotion and adaptation. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leach, C. W. (2017). Understanding shame and guilt. In Woodyat, L., Worthington, E. L., Jr., Wenzel, M., & Griffin, B. J. (Eds.), Handbook of the psychology of self-forgiveness (pp. 1728). Springer International Publishing AG.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Leach, C. W., & Cidam, A. (2015). When is shame linked to constructive approach orientation? A meta-analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(6), 9831002.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lerner, J. S., Goldberg, J. H., & Tetlock, P. E. (1998). Sober second thought: The effects of accountability, anger, and authoritarianism on attributions of responsibility. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 24(6), 563574.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lickel, B., Schmader, T., Curtis, M., Scarnier, M., & Ames, D. R. (2005). Vicarious shame and guilt. Group Processes and Intergroup Relations, 8(2), 145157.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lickel, B., Steele, R. R., & Schmader, T. (2011). Group-based shame and guilt: Emerging directions in research. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 5(3), 153163.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lomas, T. (2019). Anger as a moral emotion: A “bird’s eye” systematic review. Counselling Psychology Quarterly, 32(3–4), 341395.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Malle, B. F. (2021). Moral judgments. Annual Review of Psychology, 72, 293318.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malle, B. F., Voiklis, J., & Kim, B. (2018). Understanding contempt against the background of blame. In Mason, M. (Ed.), The moral psychology of contempt (pp. 79105). Rowman & Littlefield.Google Scholar
Nabi, R. L. (2002). The theoretical versus the lay meaning of disgust: Implications for emotion research. Cognition and Emotion, 16(5), 695703.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Noon, D. (2019). The antecedent and functions of group-based moral emotions. [Doctoral thesis, University of Surrey].Google Scholar
Nussbaum, M. C. (2004). Hiding from humanity: Disgust, shame, and the law. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Oatley, K., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (1996). The communicative theory of emotions: Empirical tests, mental models, and implications for social interaction. In Martin, L. L. & Tesser, A. (Eds.), Striving and feeling: Interactions among goals, affect, and self-regulation (pp. 363393). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.Google Scholar
Onu, D., Kessler, T., & Smith, J. R. (2016). Admiration: A conceptual review. Emotion Review, 8(3), 218230.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ortony, A., Clore, G. L., & Collins, A. (1988). The cognitive structure of emotions. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parrott, W. G. (Ed.). (2001). Emotions in social psychology. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Plutchik, R. (1980). A general psychoevolutionary theory of emotion. In Plutchik, R. & Kellerman, H. (Eds.), Theories of emotion (pp. 333). Academic Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pohling, R., & Diessner, R. (2016). Moral elevation and moral beauty: A review of the empirical literature. Review of General Psychology, 20(4), 412425.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rees, J. H., Allpress, J. A., & Brown, R. (2013). Nie wieder: Group-based emotions for in-group wrongdoing affect attitudes toward unrelated minorities. Political Psychology, 34(3), 387407.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rivis, A., Sheeran, P., & Armitage, C. J. (2009). Expanding the affective and normative components of the theory of planned behavior: A meta-analysis of anticipated affect and moral norms. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 39(12), 29853019.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Roseman, I. J., Antoniou, A. A., & Jose, P. E. (1996). Appraisal determinants of emotions: Constructing a more accurate and comprehensive theory. Cognition and Emotion, 10(3), 241277.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rottman, J., DeJesus, J., & Greenebaum, H. (2019). Developing disgust: Theory, measurement, and application. In LoBue, V., Pérez-Edgar, K., & Buss, K. A. (Eds.), Handbook of emotional development (pp. 283309). Springer Nature Switzerland AG.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Royzman, E., Atanasov, P., Landy, J. F., Parks, A., & Gepty, A. (2014). CAD or MAD? Anger (not disgust) as the predominant response to pathogen-free violations of the divinity code. Emotion, 14(5), 892907.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rozin, P., Haidt, J., & McCauley, C. R. (1993). Disgust. In Lewis, M., Haviland-Jones, J. M., & Barrett, L.F. (Eds), Handbook of emotions (pp. 575594). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Rozin, P., Lowery, L., Imada, S., & Haidt, J. (1999). The CAD triad hypothesis: A mapping between three moral emotions (contempt, anger, disgust) and three moral codes (community, autonomy, divinity). Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76(4), 574586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rozin, P., Markwith, M., & Nemeroff, C. (1992). Magical contagion beliefs and fear of AIDS 1. Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 22(14), 10811092.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozin, P., Markwith, M., & Ross, B. (1990). The sympathetic magical law of similarity: Nominal realism and neglect of negatives in response to negative labels. Psychological Science, 1(6), 383384.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozin, P., Millman, L., & Nemeroff, C. (1986). Operation of the laws of sympathetic magic in disgust and other domains. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50(4), 703712.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rozin, P., & Nemeroff, C. (2002). Sympathetic magical thinking: The contagion and similarity ‘heuristics.’ Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rudd, M., Vohs, K. D., & Aaker, J. (2012) Awe expands people’s perception of time, alters decision making, and enhances well-being. Psychological Science, 23(10), 11301136.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russell, J. A. (1991). Negative results on a reported facial expression of contempt. Motivation and Emotion, 15(4), 281291.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, P. S., & Giner-Sorolla, R. (2011). Moral anger, but not moral disgust, responds to intentionality. Emotion, 11(2), 233240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Russell, P. S., & Giner-Sorolla, R. (2013). Bodily moral disgust: What it is, how it is different from anger, and why it is an unreasoned emotion. Psychological Bulletin, 139(2), 328351.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, P. S., Piazza, J., & Giner-Sorolla, R. (2013). CAD revisited: Effects of the word moral on the moral relevance of disgust (and other emotions). Social Psychological and Personality Science, 4(1), 6268.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Russell, P. S., Smith, D. M., Birtel, M. D., Hart, K. H., & Golding, S. E. (2021). The role of emotions and injunctive norms in breastfeeding: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Health Psychology Review, 16(2), 257279.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sasse, J., Halmburger, A., & Baumert, A. (2020). The functions of anger in moral courage: Insights from a behavioral study. Emotion, 22(6), 13211335.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schnall, S., Haidt, J., Clore, G. L., & Jordan, A. H. (2008). Disgust as embodied moral judgment. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(8), 10961109.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schnall, S., & Roper, J. (2012). Elevation puts moral values into action. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3(3), 373378.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Seger, C. R., Banerji, I., Park, S. H., Smith, E. R., & Mackie, D. M. (2017). Specific emotions as mediators of the effect of intergroup contact on prejudice: Findings across multiple participant and target groups. Cognition and Emotion, 31(5), 923936.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaver, P., Schwartz, J., Kirson, D., & O’Connor, C. (1987). Emotion knowledge: Further exploration of a prototype approach. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(6), 10611086.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sheikh, S., & Janoff-Bulman, R. (2010). The “shoulds” and “should nots” of moral emotions: A self-regulatory perspective on shame and guilt. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 36(2), 213224.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Smith, C. A., & Ellsworth, P. C. (1985). Patterns of cognitive appraisal in emotion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 48(4), 813838.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stancato, D., & Keltner, D. (2019). Awe, ideological conviction, and perceptions of ideological opponents. Emotion, 21(1), 6172.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sweetman, J., Spears, R., Livingstone, A., & Manstead, A. (2013). Admiration regulates social hierarchy: Antecedents, dispositions, and effects on intergroup behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 49(3), 534542.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sznycer, D. (2019). Forms and functions of the self-conscious emotions. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(2), 143157.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tangney, J. P., & Dearing, R. L. (2002). Shame and guilt. Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J., & Mashek, D. J. (2007). Moral emotions and moral behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 58(1), 345372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tausch, N., Becker, J. C., Spears, R., Christ, O., Saab, R., Singh, P., & Siddiqui, R. N. (2011). Explaining radical group behavior: Developing emotion and efficacy routes to normative and nonnormative collective action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(1), 129148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Teper, R., Zhong, C. B., & Inzlicht, M. (2015). How emotions shape moral behavior: Some answers (and questions) for the field of moral psychology. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 9(1), 114.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Teroni, F., & Deonna, J. A. (2008). Differentiating shame from guilt. Consciousness and Cognition: An International Journal, 17(3), 725740.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Terrizzi, J. A. Jr., & Shook, N. J. (2020). On the origin of shame: Does shame emerge from an evolved disease-avoidance architecture? Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience, 14, Article 19.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tetlock, P. E., Visser, P. S., Singh, R., Polifroni, M., Scott, A., Elson, S. B., Mazzocco, P., & Rescober, P. (2007). People as intuitive prosecutors: The impact of social control goals on attributions of responsibility. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 43(2), 195209.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Thomson, A. L., & Siegel, J. T. (2017). Elevation: A review of scholarship on a moral and other-praising emotion. The Journal of Positive Psychology, 12(6), 628638.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tompkins, S. S. (1963). Affect, imagery, consciousness: II. The Negative Affects. Springer.Google Scholar
Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2006). Appraisal antecedents of shame and guilt: Support for a theoretical model. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32(10), 13391351.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2007). The psychological structure of pride: A tale of two facets. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 92(3), 506525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tybur, J. M., Molho, C., Cakmak, B., Cruz, T. D., Singh, G. D., & Zwicker, M. (2020). Disgust, anger, and aggression: Further tests of the equivalence of moral emotions. Collabra: Psychology, 6(1), Article 34.Google Scholar
Van de Vyver, J., & Abrams, D. (2015). Testing the prosocial effectiveness of the prototypical moral emotions: Elevation increases benevolent behaviors and outrage increases justice behaviors. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 58, 2333.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Västfjäll, D., Slovic, P., Mayorga, M., & Peters, E. (2014). Compassion fade: Affect and charity are greatest for a single child in need. PLoS ONE, 9, Article e100115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weber, H. (2004). Explorations in the social construction of anger. Motivation and Emotion, 28, 197219.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wheatley, T., & Haidt, J. (2005). Hypnotic disgust makes moral judgments more severe. Psychological Science, 16(10), 780784.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wranik, T., & Scherer, K. R. (2010). Why do I get angry? A componential appraisal approach. In Potegal, M., Stemmler, G., & Spielberger, C. (Eds.), International handbook of anger (pp. 243266). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yang, Y., Hu, J., Jing, F., & Nguyen, B. (2018). From awe to ecological behavior: The mediating role of connectedness to nature. Sustainability, 10(7), Article 2477.Google Scholar
Zaki, J., & Cikara, M. (2015). Addressing empathic failures. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 24(6), 471476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zickfeld, J. H., Schubert, T. W., Seibt, B., & Fiske, A. P. (2017). Empathic concern is part of a more general communal emotion. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, Article 723.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

References

Allison, P. (1990). The altruistic personality: Rescuers of Jews in Nazi Europe. Public Opinion Quarterly, 54(3), 442444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ames, D. R. (2004). Inside the mind reader’s tool kit: Projection and stereotyping in mental state inference. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(3), 340353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ashar, Y. K., Andrews-Hanna, J. R., Dimidjian, S., & Wager, T. D. (2017). Empathic care and distress: Predictive brain markers and dissociable brain systems. Neuron, 94(6), 12631273.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bardi, L., Six, P., & Brass, M. (2017). Repetitive TMS of the temporo-parietal junction disrupts participant’s expectations in a spontaneous theory of mind task. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 12(11), 17751782.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bartal, I. B. A., Decety, J., & Mason, P. (2011). Empathy and pro-social behavior in rats. Science, 334(6061), 14271430.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Batson, C. D. (2009). These things called empathy: Eight related but distinct phenomena. In Decety, J. & Ickes, W. (Eds.), The social neuroscience of empathy (pp. 315). MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Batson, C. D. (2014). The empathy-altruism hypothesis: Issues and implications. In Decety, J. (Ed.), Empathy: From bench to bedside (pp. 4154). MIT Press.Google Scholar
Batson, C. D., Duncan, B. D., Ackerman, P., Buckley, T., & Birch, K. (1981). Is empathic emotion a source of altruistic motivation? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 40(2), 290302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Batson, C. D., Klein, T. R., Highberger, L., & Shaw, L. L. (1995). Immorality from empathy-induced altruism: When compassion and justice conflict. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(6), 10421054.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Batson, C. D., Lishner, D. A., Carpenter, A., Dulin, L., Harjusola-Webb, S., Stocks, E. L., Gale, S., Hassan, O., & Sampat, B. (2003). “… As you would have them do unto you”: Does imagining yourself in the other’s place stimulate moral action? Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29(9), 11901201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Batson, C. D., Lishner, D. A., Cook, J., & Sawyer, S. (2005). Similarity and nurturance: Two possible sources of empathy for strangers. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 27, 1525.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ben-Ami Bartal, I., Shan, H., Molasky, N. M., Murray, T. M., Williams, J. Z., Decety, J., & Mason, P. (2016). Anxiolytic treatment impairs helping behavior in rats. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, Article 850.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blader, S. L., & Tyler, T. R. (2002). Justice and empathy: What motivates people to help others? In Ross, M. & Miller, D. T. (Eds.), The justice motive in everyday life (pp. 226250). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Briefer, E. F. (2018). Vocal contagion of emotions in non-human animals. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 285(1873), Article 20172783.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bruneau, E. G., Cikara, M., & Saxe, R. (2017). Parochial empathy predicts reduced altruism and the endorsement of passive harm. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 8(8), 934942.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buffone, A. E., Poulin, M., DeLury, S., Ministero, L., Morrisson, C., & Scalco, M. (2017). Don’t walk in her shoes! Different forms of perspective taking affect stress physiology. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 72, 161168.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Burkett, J. P., Andari, E., Johnson, Z. V., Curry, D. C., de Waal, F. B., & Young, L. J. (2016). Oxytocin-dependent consolation behavior in rodents. Science, 351(6271), 375378.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cacioppo, J. T., & Decety, J. (2011). Social neuroscience: Challenges and opportunities in the study of complex behavior. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1224(1), 162173.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cameron, C. D. (2018). Motivating empathy: Three methodological recommendations for mapping empathy. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 12(11), Article e12418.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cao, Y., Contreras-Huerta, L. S., McFadyen, J., & Cunnington, R. (2015). Racial bias in neural response to others’ pain is reduced with other-race contact. Cortex, 70, 6878.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carrillo, M., Han, Y., Migliorati, F., Liu, M., Gazzola, V., & Keysers, C. (2019). Emotional mirror neurons in the rat’s anterior cingulate cortex. Current Biology, 29(8), 13011312.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carter, R. M., & Huettel, S. A. (2013). A nexus model of the temporal–parietal junction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 17(7), 328336.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cheng, Y., Chen, C., Lin, C. P., Chou, K. H., & Decety, J. (2010). Love hurts: An fMRI study. Neuroimage, 51(2), 923929.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cialdini, R. B., Brown, S. L., Lewis, B. P., Luce, C., & Neuberg, S. L. (1997). Reinterpreting the empathy altruism relationship: When one into one equals oneness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 481494.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cikara, M., Botvinick, M. M., & Fiske, S. T. (2011). Us versus them: Social identity shapes responses to intergroup competition and harm. Psychological Science, 22, 306313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cikara, M., & Fiske, S. T. (2013). Their pain, our pleasure: Stereotype content and schadenfreude. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 1299(1), 5259.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Contreras-Huerta, L. S., Baker, K. S., Reynolds, K. J., Batalha, L., & Cunnington, R. (2013). Racial bias in neural empathic responses to pain. PLoS ONE, 8(12), Article e84001.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curry, O. S. (2016). Morality as cooperation: A problem-centred approach. In Shackelford, T. K. & Hansen, R. D. (Eds.), The evolution of morality (pp. 2751). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cushman, F., Gray, K., Gaffey, A., & Mendes, W. B. (2012). Simulating murder: The aversion to harmful action. Emotion, 12(1), 27.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Decety, J. (2020). Empathy in medicine: What it is, and how much we really need it. American Journal of Medicine, 133, 561566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Decety, J., Chen, C., Harenski, C., & Kiehl, K. A. (2013). An fMRI study of affective perspective taking in individuals with psychopathy: Imagining another in pain does not evoke empathy. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 7, Article 489.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Decety, J., & Cowell, J. M. (2014). The complex relation between morality and empathy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(7), 337339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Decety, J., & Cowell, J. M. (2015). Empathy, justice, and moral behavior. American Journal of Bioethics – Neuroscience, 6(3), 314.Google ScholarPubMed
Decety, J., & Cowell, J. M. (2018). Interpersonal harm aversion as a necessary foundation for morality: A developmental neuroscience perspective. Development and Psychopathology, 30(1), 153164.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Decety, J., Echols, S. C., & Correll, J. (2010). The blame game: The effect of responsibility and social stigma on empathy for pain. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 22(5), 985997.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Decety, J., & Jackson, P. L. (2004). The functional architecture of human empathy. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience Reviews, 3(2), 71100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Decety, J., & Lamm, C. (2007). The role of the right temporoparietal junction in social interaction: How low-level computational processes contribute to meta-cognition. The Neuroscientist, 13(6), 580593.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Decety, J., & Sommerville, J. A. (2003). Shared representations between self and other: A social cognitive neuroscience view. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(12), 527533.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
DeSteno, D. (2015). Compassion and altruism: How our minds determine who is worthy of help. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 3, 8083.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dickert, S., & Slovic, P. (2009). Attentional mechanisms in the generation of sympathy. Judgment and Decision Making, 4, 297306.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dovidio, J. F., & Banfield, J. C. (2015). Prosocial behavior and empathy. In Wright, J. D. (Ed.), International encyclopedia of the social and behavioral science, (2nd ed., Vol. 19, pp. 216220). Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engert, V., Linz, R., & Grant, J. A. (2019). Embodied stress: The physiological resonance of psychosocial stress. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 105, 138146.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Epley, N., Keysar, B., Van Boven, L., & Gilovich, T. (2004). Perspective taking as egocentric anchoring and adjustment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87(3), 327339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
FeldmanHall, O., Dalgleish, T., Evans, D., & Mobbs, D. (2015). Empathic concern drives costly altruism. NeuroImage, 105, 347356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferris, C. F. (2014). Using awake animal imaging to understand neural circuits of emotion: Studies ranging from maternal care to aggression. In Decety, J. & Christen, Y. (Eds.), New frontiers in social neuroscience (pp. 111126). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fetherstonhaugh, D., Slovic, P., Johnson, S. M., & Friedrich, J. (1997). Insensitivity to the value of human life: A study of psychophysical numbing. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 14, 283300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Filippi, M., Riccitelli, G., Falini, A., Di Salle, F., Vuilleumier, P., Comi, G., & Rocca, M. A. (2010). The brain functional networks associated to human and animal suffering differ among omnivores, vegetarians and vegans. PLOS ONE, 5(5), Article e10847.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Francois, P., Fujiwara, T., & Van Ypersele, T. (2018). The origins of human prosociality: Cultural group selection in the workplace and the laboratory. Science Advances, 4(9), Article e2201.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Galinsky, A. D., & Moskowitz, G. B. (2000). Perspective-taking: Decreasing stereotype expression, stereotype accessibility, and in-group favoritism. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 78(4), 708724.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Genevsky, A., Västfjäll, D., Slovic, P., & Knutson, B. (2013). Neural underpinnings of the identifiable victim effect: Affect shifts preferences for giving. The Journal of Neuroscience, 33(43), 1718817196.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gleichgerrcht, E., & Young, L. (2013). Low levels of empathic concern predict utilitarian moral judgment. PLoS ONE, 8(4), Article e60418.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goetz, J. L., Keltner, D., & Simon-Thomas, E. (2010). Compassion: An evolutionary analysis and empirical review. Psychological Bulletin, 136, 351375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gordon, I., Zagoory-Sharon, O., Leckman, J. F., & Feldman, R. (2010). Oxytocin and the development of parenting in humans. Biological Psychiatry, 68(4), 377382.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gordon, R. M. (2021). Simulation, predictive coding and the shared world. In Gilead, M. & Ochsner, K. (Eds.), The neural basis of mentalizing (pp. 237255). Springer Nature.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haidt, J., & Joseph, C. (2004). Intuitive ethics: How innately prepared intuitions generate culturally variable virtues. Daedalus, 133, 5566.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behaviour. Journal of Theoretical Biology, 7(1), 1752.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harbaugh, W. T., Mayr, U., & Burghart, D. R. (2007). Neural responses to taxation and voluntary giving reveal motives for charitable donations. Science, 316(5831), 16221625.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Higgins, E. T. (2015). What is value? Where does it come from? A psychological perspective. In Brosch, T. & Sander, D. (Eds.), Handbook of value: Perspectives from economics, neuroscience, philosophy, psychology, and sociology (pp. 4363). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hodges, S. D., & Klein, K. J. K. (2001). Regulating the costs of empathy: The price of being human. The Journal of Socio-Economics, 30(5), 437452.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hrdy, S. B. (2014). Development + social selection in the emergence of “emotionally” modern humans. In Decety, J. & Christen, Y. (Eds.), New frontiers in social neuroscience (pp. 5791). Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huang, S., & Han, S. (2014). Shared beliefs enhance shared feelings: Religious/irreligious identifications modulate empathic neural responses. Social Neuroscience, 9, 639649.Google ScholarPubMed
Jackson, P. L., Brunet, E., Meltzoff, A. N., & Decety, J. (2006). Empathy examined through the neural mechanisms involved in imagining how I feel versus how you feel pain. Neuropsychologia, 44(5), 752761.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacob, F. (1977). Evolution and tinkering. Science, 196(4295), 11611166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jin, K. S., & Baillargeon, R. (2017). Infants possess an abstract expectation of ingroup support. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(31), 81998204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, B. D., & King, R. D. (2017). Facial profiling: Race, physical appearance, and punishment. Criminology, 55(3), 520547.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kenkel, W. M., Perkeybile, A. M., & Carter, C. S. (2017). The neurobiological causes and effects of alloparenting. Developmental Neurobiology, 77(2), 214232.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kenrick, D. T., & Griskevicius, V. (2013). The rational animal: How evolution made us smarter than we think. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Kogut, T., & Ritov, I. (2005). The identified victim effect: An identified group, or just a single individual? Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 18, 157167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krishnan, A., Woo, C. W., Chang, L. J., Ruzic, L., Gu, X., López-Solà, M., Jackson, P. L., Pujol, J., Fan, J., & Wager, T. D. (2016). Somatic and vicarious pain are represented by dissociable multivariate brain patterns. eLife, 5, Article e15166.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lamm, C., Batson, C. D., & Decety, J. (2007). The neural basis of human empathy: Effects of perspective-taking and cognitive appraisal. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 4258.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lamm, C., Decety, J., & Singer, T. (2011). Meta-analytic evidence for common and distinct neural networks associated with directly experienced pain and empathy for pain. Neuroimage, 54(3), 24922502.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Langford, D. J., Tuttle, A. H., Brown, K., Deschenes, S., Fischer, D. B., Mutso, A., Root, K. C., Sotocinal, S. G., Stern, M. A., Mogil, J. S., & Sternberg, W. F. (2010). Social approach to pain in laboratory mice. Social Neuroscience, 5(2), 163170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lanzetta, J. T., & Englis, B. G. (1989). Expectations of cooperation and competition and their effects on observers’ vicarious emotional responses. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 56, 543554.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lockwood, P. L. (2016). The anatomy of empathy: Vicarious experience and disorders of social cognition. Behavioural Brain Research, 311, 255266.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Loewenstein, G., & Small, D. (2007). The scarecrow and the tin man: The vicissitudes of human sympathy and caring. Review of General Psychology, 11, 112126.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mar, R. A. (2018). Stories and the promotion of social cognition. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 27(4), 257262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marean, C. W. (2015). The most invasive species of all. Scientific American, 313(2), 3239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McAuliffe, W. H., Carter, E. C., Berhane, J., Snihur, A. C., & McCullough, M. E. (2020). Is empathy the default response to suffering? A meta-analytic evaluation of perspective taking’s effect on empathic concern. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 24(2), 141162.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mendl, M., Burman, O. H., & Paul, E. S. (2010). An integrative and functional framework for the study of animal emotion and mood. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 277(1696), 28952904.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Molenberghs, P., Gapp, J., Wang, B., Louis, W. R., & Decety, J. (2016). Increased moral sensitivity for outgroup perpetrators harming ingroup members. Cerebral Cortex, 26(1), 225233.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Morelli, S. A., Sacchet, M. D., & Zaki, J. (2015). Common and distinct neural correlates of personal and vicarious reward: A quantitative meta-analysis. NeuroImage, 112, 244253.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mumper, M. L., & Gerrig, R. J. (2017). Leisure reading and social cognition: A meta-analysis. Psychology of Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 11(1), 109120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Neyer, F. J., & Lang, F. R. (2003). Blood is thicker than water: Kinship orientation across adulthood. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84(2), 310321.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Oliner, S., & Oliner, P. (1988). The altruistic personality. Free Press.Google Scholar
Park, J. H., Schaller, M., & Van Vugt, M. (2008). Psychology of human kin recognition: Heuristic cues, erroneous inferences, and their implications. Review of General Psychology, 12(3), 215235.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patil, I., Calò, M., Fornasier, F., Cushman, F., & Silani, G. (2017). The behavioral and neural basis of empathic blame. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richerson, P. J., Boyd, R., & Henrich, J. (2010). Gene-culture coevolution in the age of genomics. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107, 89858992.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rosin, H. (2013, April 29). Why all this maternal sympathy for Dzhokhar? Slate. https://slate.com/human-interest/2013/04/maternal-sympathy-for-dzhokhar-tsarnaev-what-s-it-about.htmlGoogle Scholar
Ruby, P., & Decety, J. (2001). Effect of subjective perspective taking during simulation of action: A PET investigation of agency. Nature Neuroscience, 4(5), 546550.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruby, P., & Decety, J. (2004). How would you feel versus how do you think she would feel? A neuroimaging study of perspective taking with social emotions. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 16, 988999.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sato, N., Tan, L., Tate, K., & Okada, M. (2015). Rats demonstrate helping behavior toward a soaked conspecific. Animal Cognition, 18(5), 10391047.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Scott-Phillips, T. C., Dickins, T. E., & West, S. A. (2011). Evolutionary theory and the ultimate–proximate distinction in the human behavioral sciences. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 6(1), 3847.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shdo, S. M., Ranasinghe, K. G., Gola, K. A., Mielke, C. J., Sukhanov, P. V., Miller, B. L., & Rankin, K. P. (2018). Deconstructing empathy: Neuroanatomical dissociations between affect sharing and prosocial motivation using a patient lesion model. Neuropsychologia, 116, 126135.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Simas, E. N., Clifford, S., & Kirkland, J. H. (2020). How empathic concern fuels political polarization. American Political Science Review, 114(1), 258269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Singer, T., Seymour, B., O’Doherty, J. P., Stephan, K. E., Dolan, R. J., & Frith, C. D. (2006). Empathic neural responses are modulated by the perceived fairness of others. Nature, 439, 466469.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Slovic, P. (2007). If I look at the mass I will never act: Psychic numbing and genocide. Judgment and Decision Making, 2, 7995.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Slovic, P., Västfjäll, D., Erlandsson, A., & Gregory, R. (2017). Iconic photographs and the ebb and flow of empathic response to humanitarian disasters. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(4), 640644.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Small, D. A., Loewenstein, G., & Slovic, P. (2007). Sympathy and callousness: The impact of deliberative thought on donations to identifiable and statistical victims. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 102(2), 143153.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steinbeis, N. (2016). The role of self–other distinction in understanding others’ mental and emotional states: Neurocognitive mechanisms in children and adults. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 371(1686), Article 20150074.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strathearn, L., Fonagy, P., Amico, J., & Montague, P. R. (2009). Adult attachment predicts maternal brain and oxytocin response to infant cues. Neuropsychopharmacology, 34(13), 26552666.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tice, D. M., Bratslavsky, E., & Baumeister, R. F. (2001). Emotional distress regulation takes precedence over impulse control: If you feel bad, do it. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 80(1), 5367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Västfjäll, D., Slovic, P., Mayorga, M., & Peters, E. (2014). Compassion fade: Affect and charity are greatest for a single child in need. PLOS ONE, 9(6), Article e100115.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vaughn, D. A., Savjani, R. R., Cohen, M. S., & Eagleman, D. M. (2018). Empathic neural responses predict group allegiance. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 12, Article 302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vekaria, K. M., O’Connell, K., Rhoads, S. A., Brethel-Haurwitz, K. M., Cardinale, E. M., Robertson, E. L., Walitt, B., VanMeter, J. W., & Marsh, A. A. (2020). Activation in bed nucleus of the stria terminalis (BNST) corresponds to everyday helping. Cortex, 127, 6777.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Xu, X., Zuo, X., Wang, X., & Han, S. (2009). Do you feel my pain? Racial group membership modulates empathic neural responses. Journal of Neuroscience, 29(26), 85258529.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yamada, M., Lamm, C., & Decety, J. (2011). Pleasing frowns, disappointing smiles: An ERP investigation of counterempathy. Emotion, 11(6), 13361345.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Zahn-Waxler, C., Schoen, A., & Decety, J. (2018). An interdisciplinary perspective on the origins of concern for others: Contributions from psychology, neuroscience philosophy and sociobiology. In Roughley, N. & Schramme, T. (Eds.), Forms of fellow feeling: Empathy, sympathy, concern and moral agency (pp. 184215). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zaki, J. (2014). Empathy: A motivated account. Psychological Bulletin, 140(6), 16081647.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Thinking and Feeling
  • Edited by Bertram Malle, Brown University, Rhode Island, Philip Robbins, University of Missouri
  • Book: The Cambridge Handbook of Moral Psychology
  • Online publication: 20 February 2025
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108894357.009
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • Thinking and Feeling
  • Edited by Bertram Malle, Brown University, Rhode Island, Philip Robbins, University of Missouri
  • Book: The Cambridge Handbook of Moral Psychology
  • Online publication: 20 February 2025
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108894357.009
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • Thinking and Feeling
  • Edited by Bertram Malle, Brown University, Rhode Island, Philip Robbins, University of Missouri
  • Book: The Cambridge Handbook of Moral Psychology
  • Online publication: 20 February 2025
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781108894357.009
Available formats
×