Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-6bf8c574d5-h6jzd Total loading time: 0.001 Render date: 2025-02-22T19:22:02.300Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Part IV - Origins, Development, and Variation

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 February 2025

Bertram Malle
Affiliation:
Brown University, Rhode Island
Philip Robbins
Affiliation:
University of Missouri
Get access
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2025

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Abdolmaleky, H. M., Thiagalingam, S., & Wilcox, M. (2005). Genetics and epigenetics in major psychiatric disorders: Dilemmas, achievements, applications, and future scope. American Journal of Pharmacogenomics, 5(3), 149160.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Abram, D. (1996). Spell of the sensuous. Vintage Press.Google Scholar
Adams, D. W. (2020). Education for extinction: American Indians and the boarding school experience, 1875–1928 (2nd ed.). University Press of Kansas.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnsten, A. F. T. (2009). Stress signaling pathways that impair prefrontal cortex structure and function. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(6), 410422.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Basso, K. (1996). Wisdom sits in places: Landscape and language among the western Apache. University of New Mexico Press.Google Scholar
Bethlehem, R. A. I., Seidlitz, J., White, S. R., Vogel, J. W., Anderson, K. M., Adamson, C., Adler, S., Alexopoulos, G. S., Anagnostou, E., Areces-Gonzalez, A., Astle, D. E., Auyeung, B., Ayub, M., Bae, J., Ball, G., Baron-Cohen, S., Beare, R., Bedford, S. A., Benegal, V., … & Alexander-Bloch, A. F. (2022). Brain charts for the human lifespan. Nature, 604, 525533.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blasi, A. (1980). Bridging moral cognition and moral action: A critical review of the literature. Psychological Bulletin, 88(1), 145.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boehm, C. (1999). Hierarchy in the forest: The evolution of egalitarian behavior. Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bram, M. (2002). The recovery of the west: An essay in symbolic history. Xlibris.Google Scholar
Bram, M. (2018). A history of humanity. Primus Books.Google Scholar
Burkart, J. M., Hrdy, S. B., & Van Schaik, C. P. (2009). Cooperative breeding and human cognitive evolution. Evolutionary Anthropology, 18(5), 175186.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Carter, C. S., & Porges, S. W. (2013). Neurobiology and the evolution of mammalian social behavior. In Narvaez, D., Panksepp, J., Schore, A., & Gleason, T. (Eds.), Evolution, early experience and human development (pp. 132151). Oxford.Google Scholar
Chua, A. (2011). Battle hymn of the tiger mother. Penguin.Google Scholar
Crittenden, P. M. (1995). Attachment and psychopathology. In Goldberg, S., Muir, R., & Kerr, J. (Eds.), Attachment theory: Social, developmental, and clinical perspectives (pp. 367406). The Analytic Press.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1962). The origin of species. Collier Books. (Original work published 1859)Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1981). The descent of man. Princeton University Press. (Original work published 1871)Google Scholar
deMause, L. (1995). The history of childhood. Psychohistory Press.Google Scholar
Doidge, N. (2007). The brain that changes itself. Viking.Google Scholar
Doris, J. (2005). Lack of character. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dunn, R. (2011). The wild life of our bodies: Predators, parasites, and partners that shape who we are today. Harper.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., & Eggum, N. D. (2009). Empathic responding: Sympathy and personal distress. In Decety, J. & Ickes, W. (Eds.), The social neuroscience of empathy (pp. 7183). Boston Review.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisler, R., & Fry, D. P. (2019). Nurturing our humanity. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erikson, E. H. (1950). Childhood and society. Norton.Google Scholar
Felitti, V. (2010). Adverse childhood experiences and their relation to adult health and well-being: Turning gold into lead [Conference session]. Human Nature and Early Experience: Addressing the Environment of Evolutionary Adaptedness. University of Notre Dame.Google Scholar
Felitti, V. J., & Anda, R. F. (2005). The Adverse Childhood Experiences (ACE) Study. Centers for Disease Control and Kaiser Permanente.Google Scholar
Fisher, M. M., & Eugster, E. A. (2014). What is in our environment that effects puberty? Reproductive Toxicology, 44, 714.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foot, P. (2001). Natural goodness. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frensch, P. A. (1998). One concept, multiple meanings: On how to define the concept of implicit learning. In Stadler, M. A. & Frensch, P. A. (Eds.), Handbook of implicit learning (pp. 47104). Sage Publications, Inc.Google Scholar
Fry, D. P. (2006). The human potential for peace: An anthropological challenge to assumptions about war and violence. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Fry, D. P. (Ed.). (2013). War, peace, and human nature: The convergence of evolutionary and cultural views. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gabel, P. (2018). The desire for mutual recognition. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garner, A., Yogman, M., & Committee on Psychosocial Aspects of Child and Family Health, Section on Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, Council on Early Childhood. (2021). Preventing childhood toxic stress: Partnering with families and communities to promote relational health. Pediatrics, 148(2), Article e2021052582.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gershoff, E. T., & Grogan-Kaylor, A. (2016). Spanking and child outcomes: Old controversies and new meta-analyses. Journal of Family Psychology, 30(4), 453469.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gielen, U. P., & Markoulis, D. C. (1994). Preference for principled moral reasoning: A developmental and cross-cultural perspective. In Adler, L. L. & Gielen, U. P. (Eds.), Cross-cultural topics in psychology (pp. 7387). Praeger.Google Scholar
Gómez-Robles, A., Hopkins, W. D., Schapiro, S. J., & Sherwood, C. C. (2015). Relaxed genetic control of cortical organization in human brains compared with chimpanzees. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(48), 1479914804.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gowdy, J. (1998). Limited wants, unlimited means: A reader on hunter-gatherer economics and the environment. Island Press.Google Scholar
Graeber, D., & Wengrow, D. (2021). The dawn of everything: A new history of humanity. MacMillan.Google Scholar
Greer, A. (Ed.). (2000). The Jesuit relations: Natives and missionaries in seventeenth-century North America. Bedford/St. Martin’s.Google Scholar
Hambrick, E. P., Brawner, T. W., Perry, B. D., Brandt, K., Hofmeister, C., & Collins, J. O. (2019). Beyond the ACE score: Examining relationships between timing of developmental adversity, relational health and developmental outcomes in children. Archives of Psychiatric Nursing, 33(3), 238247.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hare, B., & Yamamoto, S. (2017). Bonobos: Unique in mind, brain and behavior. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Harvey, G. (2017). Animism: Respecting the living world (2nd ed.). C. Hurst & Co.Google Scholar
Hawkes, K., & Coxworth, J. E. (2013). Grandmothers and the evolution of human longevity: A review of findings and future directions. Evolutionary Anthropology, 22(6), 294302.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Held, V. (1993). Feminist morality: Transforming culture, society, and politics. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Henley, T., & Rossano, M. (Eds.). (2022). Psychology and cognitive archaeology: An interdisciplinary approach to the study of the human mind. Routledge.Google Scholar
Henley, T., Rossano, M., & Kardas, E. (Eds.). (2019). Handbook of cognitive archaeology: A psychological framework. Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 6183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henry, J. P., & Wang, S. (1998). Effects of early stress on adult affiliative behavior, Psychoneuroendocrinology, 23(8), 863875.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hewlett, B. S., & Lamb, M. E. (2005). Hunter-gatherer childhoods: Evolutionary, developmental and cultural perspectives. Aldine Transaction.Google Scholar
Hrdy, S. (2009). Mothers and others: The evolutionary origins of mutual understanding. Belknap Press.Google Scholar
Ingold, T. (2005). On the social relations of the hunter-gatherer band. In Lee, R. B. & Daly, R. (Eds.), The Cambridge encyclopedia of hunters and gatherers (pp. 399410). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Ingold, T. (2011). The perception of the environment: Essays on livelihood, dwelling and skill. Routledge.Google Scholar
Ingold, T. (2013). Prospect. In Ingold, T. & Palsson, G. (Eds.), Biosocial becomings: Integrating social and biological anthropology (pp. 121). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jablonka, E., & Lamb, M. J. (2006). Evolution in four dimensions: Genetic, epigenetic, behavioral, and symbolic variation in the history of life. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Johnson, M. (2014). Morality for humans: Ethical understanding from the perspective of cognitive science. University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jost, J. T., Glaser, J., Kruglanski, A. W., & Sulloway, F. J. (2003). Political conservatism as motivated social cognition. Psychological Bulletin, 129(3), 339375.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kagan, J., & Fox, N. A. (2006). Biology, culture, and temperamental biases. In Damon, W. & Lerner, R. M. (Series Eds.) & Eisenberg, N. (Vol. Ed.), Handbook of child psychology (Vol. 3, pp. 167225). Wiley.Google Scholar
Klinenberg, E. (2012). Going solo: The extraordinary rise and surprising appeal of living alone. Penguin.Google Scholar
Kohlberg, L. (1981). The philosophy of moral development: Essays on moral development (Vol. 1). Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Kohlberg, L. (1984). The psychology of moral development: Essays on moral development (Vol. 2). Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Krebs, D. L. (2005). The evolution of morality. In Buss, D. (Ed.), Evolutionary psychology handbook (pp. 747774). John Wiley & Sons.Google Scholar
Lee, R. B., & Daly, R. (Eds.). (2005). The Cambridge encyclopedia of hunters and gatherers. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lodge, R. C. (1944). Balanced philosophy and eclecticism. Journal of Philosophy, 41(4), 8591.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lopez-Otin, C., & Kroemer, G. (2021). Hallmarks of health. Cell, 184(1), 3363.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lupien, S. J., McEwen, B. S., Gunnar, M. R., & Heim, C. (2009). Effects of stress throughout the lifespan on the brain, behaviour and cognition. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 10(6), 434445.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luria, A. R. (1976). Cognitive development: Its cultural and social foundations (M. Lopez Morillas & L. Solataroff, Trans.). Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Maté, G. (2010). In the realm of hungry ghosts. North Atlantic Books.Google Scholar
Maté, G., & Maté, D. (2022). The myth of normal: Trauma, illness, and healing in a toxic culture. Avery.Google Scholar
McPherson, D. H., & Rabb, J. D. (2011). Indian from the inside: Native American philosophy and cultural renewal (2nd ed.). MacFarland & Co.Google Scholar
Menakem, R. (2017). My grandmother’s hands: Racialized trauma and the pathway to mending our hearts and bodies. Central Recovery Press.Google Scholar
Midgely, M. (2010). The solitary self: Darwin and the selfish gene. Acumen.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2007). Attachment in adulthood: Structure, dynamics, and change. The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Milburn, M. A., & Conrad, S. D. (2016). Raised to rage: The politics of anger and the roots of authoritarianism. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Montagu, A. (1968). Brains, genes, culture, immaturity, and gestation. In Montagu, A. (Ed.), Culture: Man’s adaptive dimension (pp. 102113). Oxford.Google Scholar
Narvaez, D. (2008). Triune ethics: The neurobiological roots of our multiple moralities. New Ideas in Psychology, 26(1), 95119.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Narvaez, D. (2010). Moral complexity: The fatal attraction of truthiness and the importance of mature moral functioning. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5(2), 163181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Narvaez, D. (2013). The 99% – Development and socialization within an evolutionary context: Growing up to become “A good and useful human being.” In Fry, D. (Ed.), War, peace and human nature: The convergence of evolutionary and cultural views (pp. 643672). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Narvaez, D. (2014). Neurobiology and the development of human morality: Evolution, culture and wisdom. W.W. Norton.Google Scholar
Narvaez, D. (2016). Baselines for virtue. In Annas, J., Narvaez, D., & Snow, N. (Eds.), Developing the virtues: Integrating perspectives (pp. 1433). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Narvaez, D. (2017). Are we losing it? Darwin’s moral sense and the importance of early experience. In Joyce, R. (Ed.), Routledge handbook of evolution and philosophy (pp. 322332). Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Narvaez, D. (Ed.). (2018). Basic needs, wellbeing and morality: Fulfilling human potential. Palgrave Macmillan.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Narvaez, D. (2019). In search of baselines: Why psychology needs cognitive archaeology. In Henley, T., Rossano, M. & Kardas, E. (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive archaeology: A psychological framework (pp. 104119). Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Narvaez, D. (2020a). Ecocentrism: Resetting baselines for virtue development. Ethical Theory and Moral Practice, 23(3), 391406.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Narvaez, D. (2020b). Moral education in a time of human ecological devastation. Journal of Moral Education, 50(1), 5567.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Narvaez, D. (2021). Species-typical phronesis for a living planet. In De Caro, M. & Vaccarezza, M. S. (Eds.), Practical wisdom: Philosophical and psychological perspectives (pp. 160180). Routledge.Google Scholar
Narvaez, D., Arrows, Four, Halton, E., Collier, B., & Enderle, G. (Eds.). (2019). Indigenous sustainable wisdom: First Nation know-how for global flourishing. Peter Lang.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Narvaez, D., Gettler, L., Braungart-Rieker, J., Miller Graff, L., & Hastings, P. (2016). The flourishing of young children: Evolutionary baselines. In Narvaez, D., Braungart-Rieker, J., Miller, L., Gettler, L., & Hastings, P. (Eds.), Contexts for young child flourishing: Evolution, family and society (pp. 327). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Narvaez, D., Gleason, T., Tarsha, M., Woodbury, R., Cheng, A., & Wang, L. (2021). Sociomoral temperament: A mediator between wellbeing and social outcomes in young children. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, Article 5111.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Narvaez, D., Moore, D. S., Witherington, D. C., Vandiver, T. I., & Lickliter, R. (2022). Evolving evolutionary psychology. American Psychologist, 77(3), 424438.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Narvaez, D., Panksepp, J., Schore, A., & Gleason, T. (Eds.). (2013). Evolution, early experience, and human development: From research to practice and policy. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Narvaez, D., Wang, L, & Cheng, A. (2016). Evolved developmental niche history: Relation to adult psychopathology and morality. Applied Developmental Science, 20(4), 294309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Narvaez, D., & Witherington, D. (2018). Getting to baselines for human nature, development and wellbeing. Archives of Scientific Psychology, 6(1), 205213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Narvaez, D., Woodbury, R., Gleason, T., Kurth, A., Cheng, A., Wang, L., Deng, L., Gutzwiller-Helfenfinger, E., Christen, M., & Näpflin, C. (2019). Evolved development niche provision: Moral socialization, social maladaptation and social thriving in three countries. Sage Open, 9(2).CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Newen, A., De Bruin, L., & Gallagher, S. (Eds.). (2018). The Oxford handbook of 4e cognition. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Niehoff, D. (1999). The biology of violence: How understanding the brain, behavior, and environment can break the vicious circle of aggression. Free Press.Google Scholar
Ong, W. (2002). Orality and literacy. Routledge.Google Scholar
Oster, E. (2019). Cribsheet: A data-driven guide to better, more relaxed parenting, from birth to preschool. Penguin Press.Google Scholar
Panksepp, J. (1998). Affective neuroscience: The foundations of human and animal emotions. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Piaget, J. (1965). The moral judgment of the child (M. Gabain, Trans.). Free Press. (Originally published in 1932)Google Scholar
Porges, S. (2011). Polyvagal theory. Norton.Google Scholar
Porter, B. (2021). Unprotected: A memoir. Abrams.Google Scholar
Power, C., Finnegan, M., & Callan, H. (2017). Introduction. In Power, C., Finnegan, M., & Callan, H. (Eds.), Human origins: Contributions from social anthropology (pp. 134). Berghahn.Google Scholar
Pulcini, E. (2019). Is care a gift? In Vaughan, G. (Ed.), The maternal roots of the gift economy (pp. 7893). Ianna Publications.Google Scholar
Redfield, R. (1953). The primitive world and its transformations. Cornell University Press.Google Scholar
Redfield, R. (1956). Peasant society and culture: An anthropological approach to civilization. University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Rest, J. (1983). Morality. In Flavell, J. & Markham, E. (Eds.), Cognitive development, from P. Mussen (Ed.), Manual of child psychology, Vol. 3 (pp. 556629). Wiley.Google Scholar
Sahlins, M. (1968). Notes on the original affluent society. In Lee, R. B. & DeVore, I. (Eds.), Man the hunter (pp. 8589). Aldine Publishing Company.Google Scholar
Sahlins, M. (2008). The Western illusion of human nature. Prickly Paradigm Press.Google Scholar
Schore, A. N. (2003a). Affect dysregulation and disorders of the self. Norton.Google Scholar
Schore, A. N. (2003b). Affect regulation and the repair of the self. Norton.Google Scholar
Sheldrake, M. (2021). Entangled life: How fungi make our worlds, change our minds & shape our futures. Random House.Google Scholar
Shepard, P. (1998). Coming home to the Pleistocene (Shepard, F. R., Ed.). Island Press/Shearwater Books.Google Scholar
Shonkoff, J. P., & Phillips, D. A. (2000). From neurons to neighborhoods: The science of early childhood development. National Academy Press.Google Scholar
Siepel, K. H. (2015). Conquistador voices: The Spanish conquest of the Americas as recounted largely by the participants: Christopher Columbus, Hernán Cortés. Spruce Tree.Google Scholar
Simard, S. (2021). Finding the Mother Tree: Discovering how the forest is wired for intelligence and healing. Knopf.Google Scholar
Slingerland, E. G. (2014). Trying not to try: Ancient China, modern science, and the power of spontaneity. Broadway Books.Google Scholar
Sorenson, E. R. (1998). Preconquest consciousness. In Wautischer, H. (Ed.), Tribal epistemologies (pp. 79115). Ashgate.Google Scholar
Tarsha, M. S., & Narvaez, D. (2021). Effects of adverse childhood experience on physiological regulation are moderated by evolved developmental niche history. Anxiety, Stress & Coping, 35(4), 488500.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tarsha, M. S., & Narvaez, D. (2023). The developmental neurobiology of moral mindsets: Basic needs and childhood experience. In Berg, M. & Chang, E. (Eds.), Motivation & morality: A biopsychosocial approach (pp. 187204). APA Books.Google Scholar
Thompson, M. (1995). The representation of life. In Hursthouse, R., Lawrence, G., & Quinn, W. (Eds.), Virtues and reasons (pp. 247296). Clarendon Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tomasello, M. (2019). Becoming human: A theory of ontogeny. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Tomkins, S. (1965). Affect and the psychology of knowledge. In Tomkins, S. S. & Izard, C. E. (Eds.), Affect, cognition, and personality (pp. 7297). Springer.Google Scholar
Topa, W., & Narvaez, D. (2022). Restoring the kinship worldview: Indigenous voices introduce 28 precepts for rebalancing life on planet earth. North Atlantic Books.Google Scholar
Trevathan, W. R. (2011). Human birth: An evolutionary perspective (2nd ed.). Aldine de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Turiel, E. (1983). The development of social knowledge: Morality and convention. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Turnbull, C. M. (1984). The human cycle. Simon & Schuster.Google Scholar
Varela, F. (1999). Ethical know-how: Action, wisdom, and cognition. Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Vaughan, G. (2007). Introduction: A radically different worldview is possible. In Vaughan, G. (Ed.), Women and the gift economy (pp. 140). Ianna Publications.Google Scholar
Vaughan, G. (Ed.). (2019). The maternal roots of the gift economy. Ianna Publications.Google Scholar
Watson, J. (1988). The relation of philosophy to science. In Rabb, J. D. (Ed.), Religion and science in early Canada (pp. 1839). Ronald P. Frye.Google Scholar
Weaver, L. N., Varricchio, D. J., Sargis, E. J., Chen, M., Freimuth, W. J., & Wilson Mantilla, G. P. (2021). Early mammalian social behaviour revealed by multituberculates from a dinosaur nesting site. Nature Ecology & Evolution, 5(1), 3237.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Weiss, G. (2012). Ayn Rand nation: The hidden struggle for America’s soul. Macmillan.Google Scholar
West-Eberhard, M. J. (2003). Developmental plasticity and evolution. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Widlok, T. (2017). Anthropology and the economy of sharing. Routledge.Google Scholar
Wolynn, M. (2016). It didn’t start with you: How inherited family trauma shapes who we are and how to end the cycle. Penguin.Google Scholar
Worster, D. (1994). Nature’s economy: A history of ecological ideas (2nd ed.). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Wrangham, R. W., & Peterson, D. (1996). Demonic males: Apes and the origins of human violence. Houghton, Mifflin and Company.Google Scholar
Young, J. (2019). Connection modeling: Metrics for deep nature-connection, mentoring, and culture repair. In Narvaez, D., Four Arrows, E. Halton, B. Collier, , & Enderle, G. (Eds.), Indigenous sustainable wisdom: First Nation knowhow for global flourishing (pp. 219243). Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Young, J., Haas, E., & McGown, E. (2010). Coyote’s guide to connecting with nature (2nd ed.). Owlink Media.Google Scholar

References

Amir, D., & McAuliffe, K. (2020). Cross-cultural, developmental psychology: Integrating approaches and key insights. Evolution and Human Behavior, 41(5), 430444.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnett, J. J. (2008). The neglected 95%: Why American psychology needs to become less American. The American Psychologist, 63(7), 602614.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Aslin, R. N. (2007). What’s in a look? Developmental Science, 10(1), 4853.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Austin, W. (1980). Friendship and fairness: Effects of type of relationship and task performance on choice of distribution rules. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 6(3), 402408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baillargeon, R., Setoh, P., Sloane, S., Jin, K., & Bian, L. (2014). Infant social cognition: Psychological and sociomoral reasoning. In Gazzaniga, M. S. & Mangun, G. R. (Eds.), The cognitive neurosciences (5th ed., pp. 714). Boston Review.Google Scholar
Bandura, A. (1977). Self-efficacy: Toward a unifying theory of behavioral change. Psychological Review, 84(2), 191215.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baumeister, R. F., Bratslavsky, E., Finkenauer, C., & Vohs, K. D. (2001). Bad is stronger than good. Review of General Psychology, 5(4), 323370.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bian, L., Sloane, S., & Baillargeon, R. (2018). Infants expect ingroup support to override fairness when resources are limited. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 115(11), 27052710.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bloom, P. (2013). Just babies: The origins of good and evil. Bodley Head.Google Scholar
Boyd, R., & Richerson, P. J. (1992). Punishment allows the evolution of cooperation (or anything else) in sizable groups. Ethology and Sociobiology, 13(3), 171195.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, D. E. (1991). Human universals. Temple University Press.Google Scholar
Buon, M., Jacob, P., Margules, S., Brunet, I., Dutat, M., Cabrol, D., & Dupoux, E. (2014). Friend or foe? Early social evaluation of human interactions. PLoS ONE, 9(2), Article e88612.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Burns, M., & Sommerville, J. (2014). “I pick you”: The impact of fairness and race on infants’ selection of social partners. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article 93.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Buyukozer Dawkins, M., Sloane, S., & Baillargeon, R. (2019). Do infants in the first year of life expect equal resource allocations? Frontiers in Psychology, 10, Article 116.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casler, K., Terziyan, T., & Greene, K. (2009). Toddlers view artifact function normatively. Cognitive Development, 24(3), 240247.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Chae, J. J. K., & Song, H. (2018). Negativity bias in infants’ expectations about agents’ dispositions. British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 36(4), 620633.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Choi, Y., & Luo, Y. (2015). 13-month-olds’ understanding of social interactions. Psychological Science, 26(3), 274283.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cowell, J. M., & Decety, J. (2015). Precursors to morality in development as a complex interplay between neural, socioenvironmental, and behavioral facets. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 112(41), 1265712662.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Curry, O. S., Jones Chesters, M., & Van Lissa, C. J. (2019). Mapping morality with a compass: Testing the theory of ‘morality-as-cooperation’ with a new questionnaire. Journal of Research in Personality, 78, 106124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Curtin, C. M., Barrett, H. C., Bolyanatz, A., Crittenden, A. N., Fessler, D. M. T., Fitzpatrick, S., Gurven, M., Kanovsky, M., Kushnick, G., Laurence, S., Pisor, A., Scelza, B., Stich, S., von Rueden, C., & Henrich, J. (2020). Kinship intensity and the use of mental states in moral judgment across societies. Evolution and Human Behavior, 41(5), 415429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cushman, F. (2008). Crime and punishment: Distinguishing the roles of causal and intentional analyses in moral judgment. Cognition, 108(2), 353380.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Damon, W. (1977). The social world of the child. Jossey-Bass.Google Scholar
DesChamps, T. D., Eason, A. E., & Sommerville, J. A. (2016). Infants associate praise and admonishment with fair and unfair individuals. Infancy: The Official Journal of the International Society on Infant Studies, 21(4), 478504.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Deutsch, M. (1975). Equity, equality, and need: What determines which value will be used as the basis of distributive justice? Journal of Social Issues, 31(3), 137149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Eisenberg, N. (1986). Altruistic emotion, cognition, and behavior. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Fantz, R. L. (1964). Visual experience in infants: Decreased attention to familiar patterns relative to novel ones. Science, 146(3644), 668670.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fawcett, C., & Liszkowski, U. (2012). Infants anticipate others’ social preferences. Infant and Child Development, 21(3), 239249.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Feigenson, L., Dehaene, S., & Spelke, E. (2004). Core systems of number. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(7), 307314.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fidler, F., & Wilcox, J. (2021). Reproducibility of scientific results. In Zalta, E. N. (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy (Summer 2021 ed.). https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/sum2021/entries/scientific-reproducibility/Google Scholar
Franchin, L., Savazzi, F., Neira-Gutierrez, I. C., & Surian, L. (2019). Toddlers map the word ‘good’ to helping agents, but not to fair distributors. Journal of Child Language, 46(1), 98110.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frank, M. C., Bergelson, E., Bergmann, C., Cristia, A., Floccia, C., Gervain, J., Hamlin, J. K., Hannon, E. E., Kline, M., Levelt, C., Lew-Williams, C., Nazzi, T., Panneton, R., Rabagliati, H., Soderstrom, M., Sullivan, J., Waxman, S., & Yurovsky, D. (2017). A collaborative approach to infant research: Promoting reproducibility, best practices, and theory-building. Infancy, 22(4), 421435.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Geraci, A., & Surian, L. (2011). The developmental roots of fairness: Infants’ reactions to equal and unequal distributions of resources. Developmental Science, 14(5), 10121020.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gray, K., Young, L., & Waytz, A. (2012). Mind perception is the essence of morality. Psychological Inquiry, 23(2), 101124.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grusec, J. E., Chaparro, M. P., Johnston, M., & Sherman, A. (2014). The development of moral behavior from a socialization perspective. In Killen, M. & Smetana, J. G. (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (2nd ed., pp. 113134). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Haidt, J., & Graham, J. (2007). When morality opposes justice: Conservatives have moral intuitions that liberals may not recognize. Social Justice Research, 20(1), 98116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haidt, J., Koller, S. H., & Dias, M. G. (1993). Affect, culture, and morality, or is it wrong to eat your dog? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 65(4), 613628.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamlin, J. K. (2013a). Moral judgment and action in preverbal infants and toddlers: Evidence for an innate moral core. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(3), 186193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamlin, J. K. (2013b). Failed attempts to help and harm: Intention versus outcome in preverbal infants’ social evaluations. Cognition, 128(3), 451474.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamlin, J. K. (2014). Context-dependent social evaluation in 4.5-month-old human infants: The role of domain-general versus domain-specific processes in the development of social evaluation. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article 614.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamlin, J. K. (2015). The case for social evaluation in preverbal infants: Gazing toward one’s goal drives infants’ preferences for Helpers over Hinderers in the hill paradigm. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article 1563.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamlin, J. K., Mahajan, N., Liberman, Z., & Wynn, K. (2013). Not like me = bad: Infants prefer those who harm dissimilar others. Psychological Science, 24(4), 589594.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamlin, J. K., Ullman, T., Tenenbaum, J., Goodman, N., & Baker, C. (2013). The mentalistic basis of core social cognition: Experiments in preverbal infants and a computational model. Developmental Science, 16(2), 209226.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamlin, J. K., & Wynn, K. (2011). Young infants prefer prosocial to antisocial others. Cognitive Development, 26(1), 3039.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2007). Social evaluation by preverbal infants. Nature, 450(7169), 557559.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2010). 3-month-olds show a negativity bias in their social evaluations. Developmental Science, 13(6), 923929.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2012). Reply to Scarf et al.: Nuanced social evaluation: Association doesn’t compute. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(22), E1427E1427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hamlin, J. K., Wynn, K., Bloom, P., & Mahajan, N. (2011). How infants and toddlers react to antisocial others. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 108(50), 1993119936.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J., Heine, S. J., & Norenzayan, A. (2010). The weirdest people in the world? Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33(2–3), 6183.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Henrich, J., & Henrich, N. (2007). Why humans cooperate: A cultural and evolutionary explanation. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heyes, C. (2014). False belief in infancy: A fresh look. Developmental Science, 17(5), 647659.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hrdy, S. B. (2011). Mothers and others: The evolutionary origins of mutual understanding. Harvard University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ioannidis, J. P. A. (2005). Why most published research findings are false. PLoS Medicine, 2(8), Article e124.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jin, K., & Baillargeon, R. (2017). Infants possess an abstract expectation of ingroup support. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 114(31), 81998204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jin, K.-S., Houston, J. L., Baillargeon, R., Groh, A. M., & Roisman, G. I. (2018). Young infants expect an unfamiliar adult to comfort a crying baby: Evidence from a standard violation-of-expectation task and a novel infant-triggered-video task. Cognitive Psychology, 102, 120.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Josephs, M., Kushnir, T., Gräfenhain, M., & Rakoczy, H. (2016). Children protest moral and conventional violations more when they believe actions are freely chosen. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 141, 247255.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Joyce, R. (2007). The evolution of morality. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kanakogi, Y., Inoue, Y., Matsuda, G., Butler, D., Hiraki, K., & Myowa-Yamakoshi, M. (2017). Preverbal infants affirm third-party interventions that protect victims from aggressors. Nature Human Behaviour, 1(2), 17.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kanakogi, Y., Okumura, Y., Inoue, Y., Kitazaki, M., & Itakura, S. (2013). Rudimentary sympathy in preverbal infants: Preference for others in distress. PLoS ONE, 8(6), Article e65292.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Katz, L. D. (2000). Evolutionary origins of morality: Cross-disciplinary perspectives. Imprint Academic.Google Scholar
Kidd, C., Piantadosi, S. T., & Aslin, R. N. (2012). The Goldilocks effect: Human infants allocate attention to visual sequences that are neither too simple nor too complex. PLoS ONE, 7(5), Article e36399.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Killen, M., & Smetana, J. G. (2014). Handbook of moral development. Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Kohlberg, L. (1969). Stage and sequence: The cognitive-developmental approach to socialization. Rand McNally.Google Scholar
Kominsky, J. F., Lucca, K., Thomas, A. J., Frank, M. C., & Hamlin, K. (2022). Simplicity and validity in infant research. Cognitive Development, 63, Article 101213.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Köster, M., Ohmer, X., Nguyen, T. D., & Kärtner, J. (2016). Infants understand others’ needs. Psychological Science, 27(4), 542548.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kuhlmeier, V., Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2003). Attribution of dispositional states by 12-month-olds. Psychological Science, 14(5), 402408.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lamm, H., & Schwinger, T. (1980). Norms concerning distributive justice: Are needs taken into consideration in allocation decisions? Social Psychology Quarterly, 43(4), 425429.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lee, Y., Yun, J. E., Kim, E. Y., & Song, H. (2015). The development of infants’ sensitivity to behavioral intentions when inferring others’ social preferences. PLoS ONE, 10(9), Article e0135588.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Leventhal, G. S., & Michaels, J. W. (1969). Extending the equity model: Perception of inputs and allocation of reqard as a function of duration and quantity of performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 12(4), 303309.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Loheide-Niesmann, L., de Lijster, J., Hall, R., van Bakel, H., & Cima, M. (2021). Toddlers’ preference for prosocial versus antisocial agents: No associations with empathy or attachment security. Social Development, 30(2), 410427.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lucca, K., Yuen, F., Wang, Y., Alessandroni, N., Allison, O., Alvarez, M., Axelsson, E. L., Baumer, J., Baumgartner, H. A., Bertels, J., Bhavsar, M., Byers-Heinlein, K., Capelier-Mourguy, A., Chijiiwa, H., Chin, C. S. S., Christner, N., Cirelli, L. K., Corbit, J., Daum, M. M., … Hamlin, J. K. (in press). Infants’ social evaluation of helpers and hinderers: A large-scale, multi-lab, coordinated replication study. Developmental Science.Google Scholar
Lucca, K., Pospisil, J., & Sommerville, J. A. (2018). Fairness informs social decision making in infancy. PLoS ONE, 13(2), Article e0192848.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Malle, B. F. (1999). How people explain behavior: A new theoretical framework. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 3(1), 2348.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Margoni, F., & Surian, L. (2018). Infants’ evaluation of prosocial and antisocial agents: A meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 54(8), 14451455.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meristo, M., & Surian, L. (2013). Do infants detect indirect reciprocity? Cognition, 129(1), 102113.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meristo, M., & Surian, L. (2014). Infants distinguish antisocial actions directed towards fair and unfair agents. PLoS ONE, 9(10), Article e110553.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mikhail, J. (2007). Universal moral grammar: Theory, evidence and the future. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(4), 143152.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nielsen, M., & Haun, D. (2016). Why developmental psychology is incomplete without comparative and cross-cultural perspectives. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 371(1686), Article 20150071.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nielsen, M., Haun, D., Kärtner, J., & Legare, C. H. (2017). The persistent sampling bias in developmental psychology: A call to action. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 162, 3138.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nowak, M. A. (2006). Five rules for the evolution of cooperation. Science, 314(5805), 15601563.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Gorman, R., Henrich, J., & Van Vugt, M. (2009). Constraining free riding in public goods games: Designated solitary punishers can sustain human cooperation. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 276(1655), 323329.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perner, J., & Ruffman, T. (2005). Infants’ insight into the mind: How deep? Science, 308(5719), 214216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piaget, J. (1932). The moral judgment of the child. Harcourt, Brace.Google Scholar
Premack, D., & Premack, A. J. (1994). Moral belief: Form versus content. In Hirschfeld, L. A. & Gelman, S. A. (Eds.), Mapping the mind: Domain specificity in cognition and culture (pp. 149168). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Premack, D., & Premack, A. J. (1997). Infants attribute value to the goal-directed actions of self-propelled objects. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 9(6), 848856.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pun, A., Birch, S. A. J., & Baron, A. S. (2021). The power of allies: Infants’ expectations of social obligations during intergroup conflict. Cognition, 211, Article 104630.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rakoczy, H., Warneken, F., & Tomasello, M. (2008). The sources of normativity: Young children’s awareness of the normative structure of games. Developmental Psychology, 44(3), 875881.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Salvadori, E., Blazsekova, T., Volein, A., Karap, Z., Tatone, D., Mascaro, O., & Csibra, G. (2015). Probing the strength of infants’ preference for helpers over hinderers: Two replication attempts of Hamlin and Wynn (2011). PLoS ONE, 10(11), Article e0140570.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scarf, D., Imuta, K., Colombo, M., & Hayne, H. (2012). Social evaluation or simple association? Simple associations may explain moral reasoning in infants. PLoS ONE, 7(8), Article e42698.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schlingloff, L., Csibra, G., & Tatone, D. (2020). Do 15-month-old infants prefer helpers? A replication of Hamlin et al. (2007). Royal Society Open Science, 7(4), Article 191795.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmidt, M. F. H., & Sommerville, J. A. (2011). Fairness expectations and altruistic sharing in 15-month-old human infants. PLoS ONE, 6(10), Article e23223.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Scola, C., Holvoet, C., Arciszewski, T., & Picard, D. (2015). Further evidence for infants’ preference for prosocial over antisocial behaviors. Infancy, 20(6), 684692.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shimizu, Y., Senzaki, S., & Uleman, J. S. (2018). The influence of maternal socialization on infants’ social evaluation in two cultures. Infancy, 23(5), 748766.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sloane, S., Baillargeon, R., & Premack, D. (2012). Do infants have a sense of fairness? Psychological Science, 23(2), 196204.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sommerville, J. A., Schmidt, M. F. H., Yun, J., & Burns, M. (2013). The development of fairness expectations and prosocial behavior in the second year of life. Infancy, 18(1), 4066.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Spelke, E. S., & Kinzler, K. D. (2007). Core knowledge. Developmental Science, 10(1), 8996.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Surian, L., Ueno, M., Itakura, S., & Meristo, M. (2018). Do infants attribute moral traits? Fourteen-month-olds’ expectations of fairness are affected by agents’ antisocial actions. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, Article 1649.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tafreshi, D., Thompson, J. J., & Racine, T. P. (2014). An analysis of the conceptual foundations of the infant preferential looking paradigm. Human Development, 57(4), 222240.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tasimi, A., & Wynn, K. (2016). Costly rejection of wrongdoers by infants and children. Cognition, 151, 7679.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ting, F., & Baillargeon, R. (2021). Toddlers draw broad negative inferences from wrongdoers’ moral violations. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 118(39), Article e2109045118.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turiel, E. (1983). The development of social knowledge: Morality and convention. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Vaish, A., Grossmann, T., & Woodward, A. (2008). Not all emotions are created equal: The negativity bias in social-emotional development. Psychological Bulletin, 134(3), 383403.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van de Vondervoort, J. W., Aknin, L. B., Kushnir, T., Slevinsky, J., & Hamlin, J. K. (2018). Selectivity in toddlers’ behavioral and emotional reactions to prosocial and antisocial others. Developmental Psychology, 54(1), 114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wang, Y., & Henderson, A. (2018). Just rewards: 17-month-old infants expect agents to take resources according to the principles of distributive justice. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 172, 2540.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woo, B. M., Steckler, C. M., Le, D. T., & Hamlin, J. K. (2017). Social evaluation of intentional, truly accidental, and negligently accidental helpers and harmers by 10-month-old infants. Cognition, 168, 154163.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wynn, K., & Bloom, P. (2013). The moral baby. Routledge Handbooks Online.Google Scholar
Young, L., Cushman, F., Hauser, M., & Saxe, R. (2007). The neural basis of the interaction between theory of mind and moral judgment. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(20), 82358240.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ziv, T., & Sommerville, J. A. (2017). Developmental differences in infants’ fairness expectations from 6 to 15 months of age. Child Development, 88(6), 19301951.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ziv, T., Whiteman, J. D., & Sommerville, J. A. (2021). Toddlers’ interventions toward fair and unfair individuals. Cognition, 214, Article 104781.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

References

Alexander, K. W., O’Hara, K. D., Bortfeld, H. V., Anderson, S. J., Newton, E. K., & Kraft, R. H. (2010). Memory for emotional experiences in the context of attachment and social interaction style. Cognitive Development, 25(4), 325338.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baird, A. A. (2007). Adolescent moral reasoning: The integration of emotion and cognition. In Sinnott-Armstrong, W. (Ed.), Moral psychology, Vol. 3. The neuroscience of morality: Emotion, brain disorders, and development (pp. 323342). MIT Press.Google Scholar
Barber, N. (1995). The evolutionary psychology of physical attractiveness: Sexual selection and human morphology. Ethology and Sociobiology, 16(5), 395424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barrett, L. F., & Satpute, A. B. (2019). Historical pitfalls and new directions in the neuroscience of emotion. Neuroscience Letters, 693, 918.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Batson, C. D. (2009). These things called empathy: Eight related but distinct phenomena. In Decety, J. & Ickes, W. (Eds.), The social neuroscience of empathy (pp. 315). Boston Review.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bechara, A. (2001). Neurobiology of decision-making: Risk and reward. Seminars in Clinical Neuropsychiatry, 6(3), 205216.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bechara, A., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (2003). Role of the amygdala in decision‐making. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences, 985(1), 356369.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berardi, N., Sale, A., & Maffei, L. (2015). Brain structural and functional development: Genetics and experience. Developmental Medicine and Child Neurology, 57(Suppl. 2), 49.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Berger, C., Deutsch, N., Cuadros, O., Franco, E., Rojas, M., Roux, G., & Sánchez, F. (2020). Adolescent peer processes in extracurricular activities: Identifying developmental opportunities. Children and Youth Services Review, 118, Article 105457.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blakemore, S.-J. (2008). The social brain in adolescence. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 9(4), 267277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blakemore, S.-J., & Mills, K. L. (2014). Is adolescence a sensitive period for sociocultural processing? Annual Review of Psychology, 65(1), 187207.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boele, S., Van der Graaff, J., de Wied, M., Van der Valk, I. E., Crocetti, E., & Branje, S. (2019). Linking parent–child and peer relationship quality to empathy in adolescence: A multilevel meta-analysis. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 48(6), 10331055.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss: Vol. 1. Attachment (2nd ed.). Basic Books.Google Scholar
Bronfenbrenner, U. (1994). Ecological models of human development. International Encyclopedia of Education, 3(2), 3743.Google Scholar
Brown, B. B. (2004). Adolescents’ relationships with peers. In Lerner, R. M. & Steinberg, L. (Eds.), Handbook of adolescent psychology (2nd ed., pp. 363394). John Wiley & Sons, Inc.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, B. B., & Bakken, J. P. (2011). Parenting and peer relationships: Reinvigorating research on family–peer linkages in adolescence. Journal of Research on Adolescence, 21(1), 153165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brown, B. B., Mounts, N., Lamborn, S. D., & Steinberg, L. (1993). Parenting practices and peer group affiliation in adolescence. Child Development, 64(2), 467482.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Campbell, A. (2010). Oxytocin and human social behavior. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 14(3), 281295.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carter, C. S. (2017). The oxytocin-vasopressin pathway in the context of love and fear. Frontiers in Endocrinology, 8, Article 356.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casey, B. J., Trainor, R., Giedd, J., Vauss, Y., Vaituzis, C. K., Hamburger, S., Kozuch, P., & Rapoport, J. L. (1997). The role of the anterior cingulate in automatic and controlled processes: A developmental neuroanatomical study. Developmental Psychobiology: The Journal of the International Society for Developmental Psychobiology, 30(1), 6169.3.0.CO;2-T>CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chein, J., Albert, D., O’Brien, L., Uckert, K., & Steinberg, L. (2011). Peers increase adolescent risk taking by enhancing activity in the brain’s reward circuitry. Developmental Science, 14(2), 110.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chugani, H. T., Phelps, M. E., & Mazziotta, J. C. (1987). Positron emission tomography study of human brain functional development. Annals of Neurology, 22(4), 487497.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ciaunica, A., Constant, A., Preissl, H., & Fotopoulou, K. (2021). The first prior: From co-embodiment to co-homeostasis in early life. Consciousness and Cognition, 91, Article 103117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Clearfield, M. W., & Nelson, N. M. (2006). Sex differences in mothers’ speech and play behavior with 6-, 9-, and 14-month-old infants. Sex Roles, 54(1), 127137.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Constantinidis, C., & Luna, B. (2019). Neural substrates of inhibitory control maturation in adolescence. Trends in Neurosciences, 42(9), 604616.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Critchley, H. D., Mathias, C. J., & Dolan, R. J. (2001). Neural activity in the human brain relating to uncertainty and arousal during anticipation. Neuron, 29(2), 537545.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Crone, E. A., & Dahl, R. E. (2012). Understanding adolescence as a period of social– affective engagement and goal flexibility. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 13(9), 636650.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cross, C. P., Copping, L. T., & Campbell, A. (2011). Sex differences in impulsivity: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 137(1), 97130.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cross, C. P., Cyrenne, D.-L. M., & Brown, G. R. (2013). Sex differences in sensation-seeking: A meta-analysis. Scientific Reports, 3(1), Article 2486.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dahl, A., & Killen, M. (2018). Moral reasoning: Theory and research in developmental science. In The Stevens’ handbook of experimental psychology and cognitive neuroscience (Vol. 4, pp. 323353). Wiley.Google Scholar
Dahl, R., & Suleiman, A. (2017). Adolescent brain development: Windows of opportunity. In The adolescent brain: A second window of opportunity. A compendium (pp. 2128). UNICEF.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. (1994). Descartes’ error: Emotion, rationality and the human brain. Putnam.Google Scholar
Damasio, A. R. (1995). Review: Toward a neurobiology of emotion and feeling: Operational concepts and hypotheses. The Neuroscientist, 1(1), 1925.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man and selection in relation to sex. The Modern Library.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1872). On the expression of the emotions in man and animals. John Murray.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Decety, J., & Cowell, J. M. (2014). The complex relation between morality and empathy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(7), 337339.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Diamond, A. (1988). Abilities and neural mechanisms underlying AB performance. Child Development, 59(2), 523527.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ding, Y., Wang, E., Zou, Y., Song, Y., Xiao, X., Huang, W., & Li, Y. (2017). Gender differences in reward and punishment for monetary and social feedback in children: An ERP study. PLoS ONE, 12(3), Article e0174100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dumas, T. M., Ellis, W. E., & Wolfe, D. A. (2012). Identity development as a buffer of adolescent risk behaviors in the context of peer group pressure and control. Journal of Adolescence, 35(4), 917927.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duncan, S., & Barrett, L. F. (2007). Affect is a form of cognition: A neurobiological analysis. Cognition and Emotion, 21(6), 11841211.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, N., & Fabes, R. A. (1998). Prosocial development. In Eisenberg, N. & Damon, W. (Eds.), Handbook of child psychology: Social, emotional, and personality development (5th ed., Vol. 3, pp. 701778). John Wiley & Sons, Inc.Google Scholar
Eisenberg, N., Fabes, R. A., Murphy, B., Karbon, M., Maszk, P., Smith, M., O’Boyle, C., & Suh, K. (1994). The relations of emotionality and regulation to dispositional and situational empathy-related responding. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(4), 776797.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eisenberg, N., & Shell, R. (1986). Prosocial moral judgment and behavior in children: The mediating role of cost. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 12(4), 426433.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellemers, N., van der Toorn, J., Paunov, Y., & van Leeuwen, T. (2019). The psychology of morality: A review and analysis of empirical studies published from 1940 through 2017. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 23(4), 332366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fabes, R. A., Carlo, G., Kupanoff, K., & Laible, D. (1999). Early adolescence and prosocial/moral behavior I: The role of individual processes. Journal of Early Adolescence, 19(1), 516.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fariborz, A., Thomas, L., Richard, L., Alan, L., Gordon, B., Teresa, M., & Schiff, E. Z. (1996). Affect, attachment, memory: Contributions toward psychobiologic integration. Psychiatry, 59(3), 213239.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fausto-Sterling, A., Crews, D., Sung, J., García-Coll, C., & Seifer, R. (2015). Multimodal sex-related differences in infant and in infant-directed maternal behaviors during months three through twelve of development. Developmental Psychology, 51(10), 13511366.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fonagy, P., & Bateman, A. W. (2016). Adversity, attachment, and mentalizing. Comprehensive Psychiatry, 64, 5966.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Foulkes, L., & Blakemore, S.-J. (2018). Studying individual differences in human adolescent brain development. Nature Neuroscience, 21(3), 315323.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fraley, R. C., Hudson, N. W., Heffernan, M. E., & Segal, N. (2015). Are adult attachment styles categorical or dimensional? A taxometric analysis of general and relationship-specific attachment orientations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 109(2), 354368.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
French, J. R. P., Jr., & Raven, B. (1959). The bases of social power. In Cartwright, D. (Ed.), Studies in social power (pp. 150167). Institute for Social Research.Google Scholar
Friedman, W. J., Robinson, A. B., & Friedman, B. L. (1987). Sex differences in moral judgments? A test of Gilligan’s theory. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 11(1), 3746.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Galván, A. (2013). The teenage brain: Sensitivity to rewards. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(2), 8893.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gamer, M., Zurowski, B., & Büchel, C. (2010). Different amygdala subregions mediate valence-related and attentional effects of oxytocin in humans. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 107(20), 94009405.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Giedd, J. N., Blumenthal, J., Jeffries, N. O., Castellanos, F. X., Liu, H., Zijdenbos, A., Paus, T., Evans, A. C., & Rapoport, J. L. (1999). Brain development during childhood and adolescence: A longitudinal MRI study. Nature Neuroscience, 2(10), 861863.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Gilligan, C. (1982). In a different voice: Psychological theory and women’s development. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Glicksohn, J., Naor-Ziv, R., & Leshem, R. (2018). Sensation seeking and risk-taking. In Martel, M. M. (Ed.), Developmental pathways to disruptive, impulse-control and conduct disorders (pp. 183208). Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gluckman, P. D., & Hanson, M. A. (2006). Changing times: The evolution of puberty. Molecular and Cellular Endocrinology, 254, 2631.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldin, P. R., McRae, K., Ramel, W., & Gross, J. J. (2008). The neural bases of emotion regulation: Reappraisal and suppression of negative emotion. Biological Psychiatry, 63(6), 577586.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Groh, A. M., Narayan, A. J., Bakermans-Kranenburg, M. J., Roisman, G. I., Vaughn, B. E., Fearon, R. M. P., & van IJzendoorn, M. H. (2017). Attachment and temperament in the early life course: A meta-analytic review. Child Development, 88(3), 770795.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haidt, J. (2008). Morality. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(1), 6572.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Halim, M. L. D. (2016). Princesses and superheroes: Social-cognitive influences on early gender rigidity. Child Development Perspectives, 10(3), 155160.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Halim, M. L., Ruble, D., Tamis-LeMonda, C., & Shrout, P. E. (2013). Rigidity in gender-typed behaviors in early childhood: A longitudinal study of ethnic minority children. Child Development, 84(4), 12691284.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Harter, S., Waters, P., & Whitesell, N. R. (1998). Relational self‐worth: Differences in perceived worth as a person across interpersonal contexts among adolescents. Child Development, 69(3), 756766.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Heidegger, M. (1992). Being and time. Blackwell. (Original work published 1927)Google Scholar
Hines, M., Constantinescu, M., & Spencer, D. (2015). Early androgen exposure and human gender development. Biology of Sex Differences, 6(1), Article 3.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hinson, J. M., Jameson, T. L., & Whitney, P. (2002). Somatic markers, working memory, and decision making. Cognitive, Affective, & Behavioral Neuroscience, 2(4), 341353.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoeve, M., Stams, G. J. J. M., van der Put, C. E., Dubas, J. S., van der Laan, P. H., & Gerris, J. R. M. (2012). A meta-analysis of attachment to parents and delinquency. Journal of Abnormal Child Psychology, 40(5), 771785.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hoffman, M. L. (1991). Is empathy altruistic? Psychological Inquiry, 2(2), 131133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Huttenlocher, P. R. (1979). Synaptic density in human frontal cortex-developmental changes and effects of aging. Brain Research, 163(2), 195205.Google Scholar
Hwang, K., Ghuman, A. S., Manoach, D. S., Jones, S. R., & Luna, B. (2016). Frontal preparatory neural oscillations associated with cognitive control: A developmental study comparing young adults and adolescents. NeuroImage, 136, 139148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hwang, K., Velanova, K., & Luna, B. (2010). Strengthening of top-down frontal cognitive control networks underlying the development of inhibitory control: A functional magnetic resonance imaging effective connectivity study. Journal of Neuroscience, 30(46), 1553515545.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hyde, J. S., Bigler, R. S., Joel, D., Tate, C. C., & van Anders, S. M. (2019). The future of sex and gender in psychology: Five challenges to the gender binary. American Psychologist, 74(2), 171193.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Insel, T. R. (1997). A neurobiological basis of social attachment. American Journal of Psychiatry, 154(6), 726735.Google ScholarPubMed
Jacobs, J. E., Vernon, M. K., & Eccles, J. S. (2004). Relations between social self- perceptions, time use, and prosocial or problem behaviors during adolescence. Journal of Adolescent Research, 19(1), 4562.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
James, W. (2014). The dilemma of determinism. In The will to believe and other essays in popular philosophy (Reprint, pp. 145183). Cambridge University Press. (Originally published in 1897)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jones, J. D., Cassidy, J., & Shaver, P. R. (2015). Parents’ self-reported attachment styles: A review of links with parenting behaviors, emotions, and cognitions. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 19(1), 4476.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kagan, J. (1994). Galen’s prophecy: Temperament in human nature. Basic Books.Google Scholar
Kagan, J. (1997). Temperament and the reactions to unfamiliarity. Child Development, 68(1), 139143.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kagan, J. (2018). Perspectives on two temperamental biases. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 373, Article 20170158.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kagan, J., & Snidman, N. (2004). The long shadow of temperament. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Kalsoom, F., Behlol, M. G., Kayani, M. M., & Kaini, A. (2012). The moral reasoning of adolescent boys and girls in the light of Gilligan’s theory. International Education Studies, 5(3), 1523.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kandler, C., Bratko, D., Butković, A., Hlupić, T. V., Tybur, J. M., Wesseldijk, L. W., de Vries, R. E., Jean, P., & Lewis, G. J. (2021). How genetic and environmental variance in personality traits shift across the life span: Evidence from a cross-national twin study. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 121(5), 10791094.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kiesner, J., Cadinu, M., Poulin, F., & Bucci, M. (2002). Group identification in early adolescence: Its relation with peer adjustment and its moderator effect on peer influence. Child Development, 73(1), 196208.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kilford, E. J., Garrett, E., & Blakemore, S. J. (2016). The development of social cognition in adolescence: An integrated perspective. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 70, 106120.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Killen, M., & Cooley, S. (2013). Morality, exclusion, and prejudice. In Killen, M. & Smetana, J. G. (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 340360). Psychology Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kohlberg, L. (1969). Stage and sequence: The cognitive-developmental approach to socialization. In Goslin, D. (Ed.), Handbook of socialization theory and research (pp. 347480). Rand McNally.Google Scholar
Kraemer, G. W. (1992). A psychobiological theory of attachment. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 15(3), 493511.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Larsen, B., & Luna, B. (2018). Adolescence as a neurobiological critical period for the development of higher-order cognition. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 94, 179195.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lantz, B., & Knapp, K. G. (2024). Trends in juvenile offending: What you need to know. Council on Criminal Justice. https://counciloncj.org/trends-in-juvenile-offending-what-you-need-to-know/Google Scholar
Leaper, C., & Farkas, T. (2015). The socialization of gender during childhood and adolescence. In Grusec, J. E. & Hastings, P. D. (Eds.), Handbook of socialization: Theory and research (2nd ed., pp. 541565). The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Lewis, M. (2007). Self-conscious emotional development. In Tracy, J. L., Robins, R. W., & Tangney, J. P. (Eds.), The self-conscious emotions: Theory and research (pp. 134149). The Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Lin, Y.-T., & Hsu, K.-S. (2018). Oxytocin receptor signaling in the hippocampus: Role in regulating neuronal excitability, network oscillatory activity, synaptic plasticity and social memory. Progress in Neurobiology, 171, 114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luna, B., Marek, S., Larsen, B., Tervo-Clemmens, B., & Chahal, R. (2015). An integrative model of the maturation of cognitive control. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 38, 151170.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Luna, B., Paulsen, D. J., Padmanabhan, A., & Geier, C. (2013). Cognitive control and motivation. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(2), 94100.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Martin, C. L., & Ruble, D. N. (2010). Patterns of gender development. Annual Review of Psychology, 61, 353381.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Masten, C. L., Telzer, E. H., Fuligni, A. J., Lieberman, M. D., & Eisenberger, N. I. (2012). Time spent with friends in adolescence relates to less neural sensitivity to later peer rejection. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 7(1), 106114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McConchie, J., Hite, B. J., Blackard, M. B., & Cheung, R. C. M. (2022). With a little help from my friends: Development and validation of the positive peer influence inventory. Applied Developmental Science, 26(1), 7493.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McEwen, B. S. (2012). Brain on stress: How the social environment gets under the skin. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 109(Suppl. 2), 1718017185.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Meyers-Levy, J., & Loken, B. (2015). Revisiting gender differences: What we know and what lies ahead. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 25(1), 129149.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Milner, B., Corkin, S., & Teuber, H. L. (1968). Further analysis of the hippocampal amnesic syndrome: 14-year follow-up study of H. M. Neuropsychologia, 6(3), 215234.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Moffitt, T. E. (1993). Adolescence-limited and life-course-persistent antisocial behavior: A developmental taxonomy. Psychological Review, 100(4), 674701.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Muuss, R. E. (1988). Carol Gilligan’s theory of sex differences in the development of moral reasoning during adolescence. Adolescence, 23(89), 229243.Google ScholarPubMed
Narvaez, D., & Lapsley, D. (2014). Becoming a moral person – moral development and moral character education as a result of social interactions. In Christen, M., van Schaik, C., Fischer, J., Huppenbauer, M., & Tanner, C. (Eds.), Empirically informed ethics: Morality between facts and norms (Vol. 32, pp. 227238). Springer International Publishing.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nelson, E. E., Lau, J. Y. F., & Jarcho, J. M. (2014). Growing pains and pleasures: How emotional learning guides development. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(2), 99108.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Newport, E. L., Bavelier, D., & Neville, H. J. (2001). Critical thinking about critical periods: Perspectives on a critical period for language acquisition. In Dupoux, E. (Ed.), Language, brain, and cognitive development: Essays in honor of Jacques Mehler (pp. 481502). MIT Press.Google Scholar
Oliveira, P., & Fearon, P. (2019). The biological bases of attachment. Adoption & Fostering, 43(3), 274293.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Patterson, G. R., Dishion, T. J., & Yoerger, K. (2000). Adolescent growth in new forms of problem behavior: Macro-and micro-peer dynamics. Prevention Science, 1(1), 313.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Paulus, M. P., Rogalsky, C., Simmons, A., Feinstein, J. S., & Stein, M. B. (2003). Increased activation in the right insula during risk-taking decision making is related to harm avoidance and neuroticism. Neuroimage, 19(4), 14391448.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Perino, M. T., Miernicki, M. E., & Telzer, E. H. (2016). Letting the good times roll: Adolescence as a period of reduced inhibition to appetitive social cues. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 11(11), 17621771.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piaget, J. (1965). The moral judgment of the child. The Free Press. (Original work published 1932)Google Scholar
Poppa, T., & Bechara, A. (2018). The somatic marker hypothesis: Revisiting the role of the ‘body-loop’ in decision-making. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 19, 6166.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Preckel, F., Niepel, C., Schneider, M., & Brunner, M. (2013). Self-concept in adolescence: A longitudinal study on reciprocal effects of self-perceptions in academic and social domains. Journal of Adolescence, 36(6), 11651175.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Reber, P. J. (2013). The neural basis of implicit learning and memory: A review of neuropsychological and neuroimaging research. Neuropsychologia, 51(10), 20262042.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rokicki, J., Kaufmann, T., De Lange, A. M. G., van der Meer, D., Bahrami, S., Sartorius, A. M., Haukvik, U. K., Steen, N. E., Schwarz, E., Stein, D. J., Nærland, T., Andreassen, A. O., Westlye, L. T., & Quintana, D. S. (2022). Oxytocin receptor expression patterns in the human brain across development. Neuropsychopharmacology, 47(8), 15501560.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Savin-Williams, R. C., & Berndt, T. J. (1990). Friendship and peer relations. In Feldman, S. S. & Elliott, G. R. (Eds.), At the threshold: The developing adolescent (pp. 277307). Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Schwartz, C. E., Kunwar, P. S., Greve, D. N., Kagan, J., Snidman, N. C., & Bloch, R. B. (2012). A phenotype of early infancy predicts reactivity of the amygdala in male adults. Molecular Psychiatry, 17(10), 10421050.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schwartz, C. E., Kunwar, P. S., Greve, D. N., Moran, L. R., Viner, J. C., Covino, J. M., Kagan, J., Stewart, S. E., Snidman, N. C., Vangel, M. G., & Wallace, S. R. (2010). Structural differences in adult orbital and ventromedial prefrontal cortex predicted by infant temperament at 4 months of age. Archives of General Psychiatry, 67(1), 7884.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shih, J. H., & Auerbach, R. P. (2010). Gender and stress generation: An examination of interpersonal predictors. International Journal of Cognitive Therapy, 3(4), 332344.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sobhani, M., & Bechara, A. (2011). A somatic marker perspective of immoral and corrupt behavior. Social Neuroscience, 6(5–6), 640652.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Somerville, L. H. (2013). The teenage brain: Sensitivity to social evaluation. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 22(2), 121127.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Somerville, L. H., Jones, R. M., & Casey, B. J. (2010). A time of change: Behavioral and neural correlates of adolescent sensitivity to appetitive and aversive environmental cues. Brain and Cognition, 72(1), 124133.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sowell, E. R., Thompson, P. M., Holmes, C. J., Batth, R., Jernigan, T. L., & Toga, A. W. (1999). Localizing age-related changes in brain structure between childhood and adolescence using statistical parametric mapping. Neuroimage, 9(6), 587597.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Spenser, K., Bull, R., Betts, L., & Winder, B. (2020). Underpinning prosociality: Age related performance in theory of mind, empathic understanding, and moral reasoning. Cognitive Development, 56, Article 100928.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steinberg, L. (2005). Cognitive and affective development in adolescence. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 9(2), 6974.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steinberg, L., & Monahan, K. C. (2007). Age differences in resistance to peer influence. Developmental Psychology, 43(6), 15311543.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Steinberg, L., Icenogle, G., Shulman, E. P., Breiner, K., Chein, J., Bacchini, D., Chang, L., Chaudhary, N., Giunta, L. D., Dodge, K. A., Fanti, K. A., Lansford, J. E., Malone, P. S., Oburu, P., Pastorelli, C., Skinner, A. T., Sorbring, E., Tapanya, S., Tirado, L. M. U., … Takash, H. M. S. (2018). Around the world, adolescence is a time of heightened sensation seeking and immature self-regulation. Developmental Science, 21(2), Article e12532.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stifter, C. A., Cipriano, E., Conway, A., & Kelleher, R. (2009). Temperament and the development of conscience: The moderating role of effortful control. Social Development, 18(2), 353374.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Strathearn, L. (2011). Maternal neglect: Oxytocin, dopamine and the neurobiology of attachment: Maternal neglect: Oxytocin, dopamine and attachment. Journal of Neuroendocrinology, 23(11), 10541065.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Takesian, A. E., & Hensch, T. K. (2013). Balancing plasticity/stability across brain development. Progress in Brain Research, 207, 334.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tanti, C., Stukas, A. A., Halloran, M. J., & Foddy, M. (2011). Social identity change: Shifts in social identity during adolescence. Journal of Adolescence, 34(3), 555567.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Tenenbaum, H. R., & Leaper, C. (2002). Are parents’ gender schemas related to their children’s gender-related cognitions? A meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 38(4), 615630.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and development. Brunner/Mazel.Google Scholar
Thomas, R. M. (1997). Moral development theories – secular and religious: A comparative study (No. 68). Greenwood Publishing Group.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tobin, D. D., Menon, M., Menon, M., Spatta, B. C., Hodges, E. V. E., & Perry, D. G. (2010). The intrapsychics of gender: A model of self-socialization. Psychological Review, 117(2), 601622.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Topping, K. J. (2005). Trends in peer learning. Educational Psychology, 25(6), 631645.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tsui, T. Y. L., Lahat, A., & Schmidt, L. A. (2017). Linking temperamental shyness and social anxiety in childhood and adolescence: Moderating influences of sex and age. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 48(5), 778785.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turiel, E. (2013). Morality: Epistemology, development, and social opposition. In Killen, M. & Smetana, J. G. (Eds.), Handbook of moral development (pp. 322). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Van der Graaff, J., Carlo, G., Crocetti, E., Koot, H. M., & Branje, S. (2018). Prosocial behavior in adolescence: Gender differences in development and links with empathy. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 47(5), 10861099.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Vasileva, O., & Balyasnikova, N. (2019). (Re) Introducing Vygotsky’s thought: From historical overview to contemporary psychology. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, Article 1515.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Villanueva‐Moya, L., & Expósito, F. (2021). Gender differences in decision‐making: The effects of gender stereotype threat moderated by sensitivity to punishment and fear of negative evaluation. Journal of Behavioral Decision Making, 34(5), 706717.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vogt, B. A., Finch, D. M., & Olson, C. R. (1992). Functional heterogeneity in cingulate cortex: The anterior executive and posterior evaluative regions. Cerebral Cortex, 2(6), 435443.Google ScholarPubMed
Vygotsky, L. S. (1978). Mind in society: The development of higher psychological processes. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Waite-Stupiansky, S. (2017). Jean Piaget’s constructivist theory of learning. In Cohen, L. E. & Waite-Stupiansky, S. (Eds.), Theories of early childhood education: Developmental, behaviorist, and critical (pp. 317). Routledge.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walker, D. M., Bell, M. R., Flores, C., Gulley, J. M., Willing, J., & Paul, M. J. (2017). Adolescence and reward: Making sense of neural and behavioral changes amid the chaos. Journal of Neuroscience, 37(45), 1085510866.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walker, L. J., Hennig, K. H., & Krettenauer, T. (2000). Parent and peer contexts for children’s moral reasoning development. Child Development, 71(4), 10331048.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Walker, L. J., & Taylor, J. H. (1991). Family interactions and the development of moral reasoning. Child Development, 62(2), 264283.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wanless, S. B., McClelland, M. M., Lan, X., Son, S.-H., Cameron, C. E., Morrison, F. J., Chen, F.-M., Chen, J.-L., Li, S., Lee, K., & Sung, M. (2013). Gender differences in behavioral regulation in four societies: The United States, Taiwan, South Korea, and China. Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 28(3), 621633.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ward, M. J., Lee, S. S., & Lipper, E. G. (2000). Failure‐to‐thrive is associated with disorganized infant–mother attachment and unresolved maternal attachment. Infant Mental Health Journal, 21(6), 428442.3.0.CO;2-B>CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Whedon, J. (Writer & Director). (1997, November 3). Lie to me (Season 2, Episode 7) [TV series episode]. In G. Berman, S. Gallin, D. Greenwalt, F. Rubel Kuzui, K. Kuzui, & J. Whedon (Executive Producers), G. Davies (Producer), Buffy the Vampire Slayer. Mutant Enemy; Kuzui Enterprises; Sandollar Television; 20th Century Fox Television Productions.Google Scholar
Wright, H. F., Barker, R. G., Nall, J., & Schoggen, P. (1951). Toward a psychological ecology of the classroom. Journal of Educational Research, 45(3), 187200.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zahn-Waxler, C., & Polanichka, N. (2004). All things interpersonal: Socialization and female aggression. In Putallaz, M. & Bierman, K. L. (Eds.), Aggression, antisocial behavior, and violence among girls: A developmental perspective. (pp. 4868). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Zeifman, D. M. (2019). Attachment theory grows up: A developmental approach to pair bonds. Current Opinion in Psychology, 25, 139143.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zinchenko, O., & Arsalidou, M. (2018). Brain responses to social norms: Meta-analyses of fMRI studies. Human Brain Mapping, 39(2), 955970.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

References

Bauman, Z. (1988). Freedom. Open University Press.Google Scholar
Beal, B. (2020). What are the irreducible basic elements of morality? A critique of the debate over monism and pluralism in moral psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 15(2), 273290.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beldo, L. (2014). The unconditional ‘ought’: A theoretical model for the anthropology of morality. Anthropological Theory, 14(3), 263279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beldo, L. (2019). Contesting Leviathan: Activists, hunters, and state power in the Makah whaling conflict. University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Benedict, R. (1934). Anthropology and the abnormal. Journal of General Psychology, 10(1), 5982.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Berlin, I. (1997). The counter-enlightenment. In Hardy, H. & Hausheer, R. (Eds.), The proper study of mankind: An anthology of essays (pp. 243268). Farrar, Straus and Giroux.Google Scholar
Briggs, J. L. (1998). Inuit morality play: The emotional education of a three-year-old. Yale University Press.Google Scholar
Brightman, R. A. (1993). Grateful prey: Rock cree human-animal relationships. University of California Press.Google Scholar
Cassaniti, J. L. (2014). Moralizing emotion: A breakdown in Thailand. Anthropological Theory, 14, 280300.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cassaniti, J. L., & Hickman, J. R. (2014). New directions in the anthropology of morality. Anthropological Theory, 14(3), 251262.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cassaniti, J. L., & Menon, U. (Eds.). (2017). Universalism without uniformity: Explorations in mind and culture. University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
D’Andrade, R. (1995). Moral models in anthropology. Current Anthropology, 36, 399408.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Das, V. (2006). Life and words: Violence and the descent into the ordinary. University of California Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Das, V. (2012). Ordinary ethics. In Fassin, D. (Ed.), A companion to moral anthropology (pp. 133149). Wiley-Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Durkheim, E. (1953). The determination of moral facts. In Durkheim, E., Sociology and philosophy (pp. 3562). Routledge. (Original work published 1906)Google Scholar
Firth, R. (2013). We the Tikopia: A sociological study of kinship in primitive Polynesia. Routledge. (Original work published 1936)CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fiske, A. P. (1992). The four elementary forms of sociality: Framework for a unified theory of social relations. Psychological Review, 99(4), 689723.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fiske, A. P., & Rai, T. S. (2015). Virtuous violence: Hurting and killing to create, sustain, end, and honor social relationships. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Geertz, C. (2000). Available light: Anthropological reflections on philosophical topics. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Gergen, K. J. (1985). The social constructionist movement in modern psychology. American Psychologist, 40(3), 266275.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gergen, K. J. (2001). Psychological science in a postmodern context. American Psychologist, 56(10), 803813.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haidt, J. (2012). The righteous mind: Why good people are divided by politics and religion. Vintage Books.Google Scholar
Haidt, J., & Graham, J. (2007). When morality opposes justice: Conservatives have moral intuitions that liberals may not recognize. Social Justice Research, 20(1), 98116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Haidt, J., & Joseph, C. (2007). The moral mind: How five sets of innate intuitions guide the development of many culture-specific virtues, and perhaps even modules. In Carruthers, P., Laurence, S., & Stich, S. P. (Eds.), The innate mind (pp. 367391). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hickman, J. R. (2007). “Is it the spirit or the body?”: Syncretism of health beliefs among Hmong immigrants to Alaska. NAPA Bulletin (Annals of Anthropological Practice), 27(1), 176195.Google Scholar
Hickman, J. R. (2014). Ancestral personhood and moral justification. Anthropological Theory, 14(3), 317335.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hickman, J. R., & Fasioli, A. D. (2015). The dynamics of ethical co-occurrence in Hmong and American evangelical families: New directions for Three Ethics research. In Jensen, L. A. (Ed.), Moral development in a global world: Research from a cultural-developmental perspective (pp. 141169). Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jensen, L. A. (2008). Through two lenses: A cultural-developmental approach to moral reasoning. Developmental Review, 28(3), 289315.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jensen, L. A. (Ed.). (2011). Bridging cultural and developmental approaches to psychology: New syntheses in theory, research, and policy. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Jensen, L. A. (Ed.). (2015). Moral development in a global world: Research from a cultural-developmental perspective. Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kant, I. (1787). Kritik der reinen Vernunft (2nd ed.). Johann Friedrich Hartknoch.Google Scholar
Keltner, D., Kogan, A., Piff, P. K., & Saturn, S. R. (2014). The sociocultural appraisals, values, and emotions (SAVE) framework of prosociality: Core processes from gene to meme. Annual Review of Psychology, 65, 425460.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kohlberg, L. (1981). The philosophy of moral development: Moral stages and the idea of justice: Vol. 1. Essays on moral development. Harper & Row.Google Scholar
Ladd, J. (1957). The structure of a moral code: A philosophical analysis of ethical discourse applied to the ethics of the Navaho Indians. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Laidlaw, J. (2002). For an anthropology of ethics and freedom. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, 8(2), 311332.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Laidlaw, J. (2014). The subject of virtue: An anthropology of ethics and freedom. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Lambek, M. (Ed.). (2010a). Ordinary ethics: Anthropology, language, and action. Fordham University Press.Google Scholar
Lambek, M. (2010b). Toward an ethics of the act. In Lambek, M. (Ed.), Ordinary ethics: Anthropology, language, and action (pp. 3963). Fordham University Press.Google Scholar
Mahmood, S. (2004). Politics of piety: The Islamic revival and the feminist subject. Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Mattingly, C. (2014). Moral laboratories: Family peril and the struggle for a good life. University of California Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mattingly, C., & Throop, J. (2018). The anthropology of ethics and morality. Annual Review of Anthropology, 47, 475492.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Miller, J. G. (1984). Culture and the development of everyday social explanation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46(5), 961978.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, J. G., & Bersoff, D. M. (1992). Culture and moral judgment: How are conflicts between justice and interpersonal responsibilities resolved? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62(4), 541554.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Miller, J. G., Das, R., & Chakravarthy, S. (2011). Culture and the role of choice in agency. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 101(1), 4661.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Nietzsche, F. (1974). The gay science (W. Kaufmann, Ed. and Trans.). Random House. (Original work published 1882)Google Scholar
Nuckolls, C. W. (1993). The anthropology of explanation. Anthropological Quarterly, 66(1), 121.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nuckolls, C. W. (1998). Culture: A problem that cannot be solved. University of Wisconsin Press.Google Scholar
Prinz, J. J. (2007). The emotional construction of morals. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Rai, T. S., & Fiske, A. P. (2011). Moral psychology is relationship regulation: Moral motives for unity, hierarchy, equality, and proportionality. Psychological Review, 118(1), 5775.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rescher, N. (1993). Pluralism: Against the demand for consensus. Clarendon Press.Google Scholar
Robbins, J. (2007). Between reproduction and freedom: Morality, value, and radical cultural change. Ethnos: Journal of Anthropology, 72(3), 293314.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robbins, J. (2012). Cultural values. In Fassin, D. (Ed.), A companion to moral anthropology (pp. 115132). Wiley-Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Robbins, J. (2016). What is the matter with transcendence? On the place of religion in the new anthropology of ethics. Journal of the Royal Anthropological Institute, 22(4), 767781.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shweder, R. A. (1982). Liberalism as destiny. Contemporary Psychology, 27(6), 421424.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shweder, R. A. (1994). Are moral intuitions self-evident truths? Criminal Justice Ethics, 13(2), 2431.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shweder, R. A. (2003). Toward a deep cultural psychology of shame. Social Research, 70(4), 11091130.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shweder, R. A. (2004). Moral realism without the ethnocentrism: Is it just a list of empty truisms? In Sajó, A. (Ed.), Human rights with modesty: The problem of universalism (pp. 65102). M. Nijhoff Publishers.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shweder, R. A. (2009). Shouting at the Hebrews: Imperial liberalism v liberal pluralism and the practice of male circumcision. Law, Culture and the Humanities, 5(2), 247265.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shweder, R. A., Jensen, L. A., & Goldenstein, W. M. (1995). Who sleeps by whom revisited: A method for extracting the moral goods implicit in practice. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 1995 (67), 2139.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shweder, R. A., Mahapatra, M., & Miller, J. G. (1987). Culture and moral development. In Kagan, J. & Lamb, S. (Eds.), The emergence of morality in young children (pp. 183). University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Shweder, R. A., & Much, N. (1991). Determinations of meaning: Discourse and moral socialization. In Shweder, R. A. (Ed.), Thinking through cultures (pp. 186240). Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Shweder, R. A., Much, N. C., Mahapatra, M., & Park, L. (2003). The ‘Big Three’ of morality (autonomy, community and divinity), and the ‘Big Three’ explanations of suffering. In Shweder, R. A. (Ed.), Why do men barbecue? Recipes for cultural psychology (pp. 74133). Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Sidgwick, H. (1884). The methods of ethics (3rd ed.). MacMillan.Google Scholar
Simpson, E. L. (1974). Moral development research: A case study of scientific cultural bias. Human Development, 17(2), 81106.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Smit, H. (2000). Kant on Marks and the immediacy of intuition. Philosophical Review, 109(2), 235266.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tangney, J. P., Stuewig, J., & Mashek, D. J. (2007). Moral emotions and moral behavior. Annual Review of Psychology, 58, 345372.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Turiel, E. (2002). The culture of morality: Social development, context, and conflict. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Weber, M., Roth, G., & Wittich, C. (1978). Economy and society: An outline of interpretive sociology. University of California Press.Google Scholar
Zigon, J. (2008). Morality: An anthropological perspective. Berg Publishers.Google Scholar
Zigon, J. (2009). Phenomenological anthropology and morality: A reply to Robbins. Ethnos, 74(2), 286288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zigon, J., & Throop, C. J. (2014). Moral experience: Introduction. Ethos, 42(1), 115.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×