Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-jkksz Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-20T18:52:46.688Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

9 - Tracking the Ironical Eye: Eye Tracking Studies on Irony and Sarcasm

from Part IV - Irony in Linguistic Communication

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 December 2023

Herbert L. Colston
Affiliation:
University of Alberta
Get access

Summary

This chapter describes how people read and interpret ironical language. Tracking people’s rapid eye movements as they read can be an informative measure of the underling cognitive and linguistic processes operating during online written language comprehension. Attardo introduces some of the technologies employed in measuring eye movements during reading and suggests why these assessments can provide critical insights into how irony interpretation rapidly unfolds word-by-word as one reads. He reviews various experimental studies on irony and sarcasm understanding that provide explicit empirical tests of different theories of irony (e.g., multistate models, graded salience, parallel-constraint models predictive processing models). He also explores what the study of eye tracking reveals about the influence of contextual factors and individual differences in irony interpretation, as well as the phenomenon known as “gaze aversion” when listeners momentarily look away from speakers’ faces when hearing ironic language. Attardo closes his chapter with an important discussion of the sometimes contentious relations between psycholinguistic experiments and philosophical arguments on the ways people use and interpret irony in discourse.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2023

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adler, R. M. (2018) Toward a psycholinguistic model of irony comprehension (unpublished PhD dissertation, University of Maryland).Google Scholar
Adler, R. M., Novick, J. M., & Huang, Y. T. (2016). The time course of verbal irony comprehension and context integration. Pre-proceedings of Trends in Experimental Pragmatics, 19.Google Scholar
Aguert, M. (2022). Paraverbal expression of verbal irony: Vocal cues matter and facial cues even more. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 46(1), 4570.Google Scholar
Alba-Juez, L., & Attardo, S. (2014). The evaluative palette of verbal irony. In Alba-Juez, L., & Thompson, G. (Eds.), Evaluation in Context (pp. 93115). Benjamins.Google Scholar
Attardo, S. (2013). Intentionality and irony. In Ruiz-Gurillo, L. & Alvarado-Ortega, M. B. (Eds.), Irony and humor: From pragmatics to discourse (pp. 3957). Benjamins.Google Scholar
Attardo, S. (2020). The linguistics of humor: An introduction. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Attardo, S., Eisterhold, J., Hay, J., & Poggi, I. (2003). Multimodal markers of irony and sarcasm. Humor: International Journal of Humor Research, 16(2), 243260.Google Scholar
Au-Yeung, S. K., Kaakinen, J. K., Liversedge, S. P., & Benson, V. (2015). Processing of written irony in autism spectrum disorder: An eye-movement study. Autism Research, 8. 749760. https://doi.org/10.1002/aur.1490Google Scholar
Bach, K. (2006). The top 10 misconceptions about implicature. In Birner, B. J. & Ward, G. (Eds.), Drawing the boundaries of meaning: Neo-Gricean studies in pragmatics and semantics in honor of Laurence R. Horn (pp. 2130). Benjamins.Google Scholar
Baptista, N. I., Macedo, E. C., & Boggio, P. S. (2015). Looking more and at different things: Differential gender eye-tracking patterns on an irony comprehension task. Psychology & Neuroscience, 8(2), 157167.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barzy, M., Filik, R., Williams, D., & Ferguson, H. J. (2020). Emotional processing of ironic versus literal criticism in autistic and nonautistic adults: Evidence from eye-tracking. Autism Research, 13(4), 563578.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bezuidenhout, A., & Cutting, J. C. (2002). Literal meaning, minimal propositions, and pragmatic processing. Journal of Pragmatics, 34(4), 433456.Google Scholar
Brône, G. (2021). The multimodal negotiation of irony and humor in interaction. In Soares da Silva, A. (Ed.), Figurative language – Intersubjectivity and usage (pp. 109135). Benjamins.Google Scholar
Bruntsch, R., & Ruch, W. (2017). Studying irony detection beyond ironic criticism. Let’s include ironic praise. Frontiers in Psychology, 8,7589.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bryant, G. A. (2010). Prosodic contrasts in ironic speech. Discourse Processes, 47(7), 545566.Google Scholar
Bryant, G. A., & Fox Tree, J. E. (2005). Is there an ironic tone of voice? Language and Speech, 48(3), 257277.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chen, H. C., Chan, Y. C., Dai, R. H., Liao, Y. J., & Tu, C. H. (2017). Neurolinguistics of humor. In Attardo, S. (Ed.). The Routledge handbook of language and humor (pp. 282294). Routledge.Google Scholar
Climie, E. A., & Pexman, P. M. (2008). Eye gaze provides a window on children’s understanding of verbal irony. Journal of Cognition and Development, 9(3), 257285.Google Scholar
Colston, H. L. (2002). Contrast and assimilation in verbal irony. Journal of Pragmatics, 34(2), 111142.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colston, H. L. (2020). Eye-rolling, irony and embodiment. In Athanasiadou, A. & Colston, H. (Eds.), The Diversity of Irony (pp. 211235). De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Cosme, G., Rosa, P. J., Lima, C. F., Tavares, V., Scott, S., Chen, S., Wilcockson, T. D. W., Crawford, T. J., & Prata, D. (2021). Pupil dilation reflects the authenticity of received nonverbal vocalizations. Scientific Reports, 11(1), 114.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
De Beugher, S., Brône, G., & Goedemé, T. (2018). A semi-automatic annotation tool for unobtrusive gesture analysis. Language Resources and Evaluation, 52(2), 433460.Google Scholar
de Vries, C., Oben, B., & Brône, G. (2021). Exploring the role of the body in communicating ironic stance. Languages and Modalities, 1, 6580.Google Scholar
Deliens, G., Antoniou, K., Clin, E., Ostashchenko, E., & Kissine, M. (2018). Context, facial expression and prosody in irony processing. Journal of Memory and Language, 99, 3548.Google Scholar
Demberg, V., & Sayeed, A. (2016). The frequency of rapid pupil dilations as a measure of linguistic processing difficulty. PloS One, 11(1), e0146194.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Doherty-Sneddon, G., & Phelps, F. G. (2005). Gaze aversion: A response to cognitive or social difficulty? Memory & Cognition, 33(4), 727733.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dress, M. L., Kreuz, R. J., Link, K. E., & Caucci, G. M. (2008). Regional variation in the use of sarcasm. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 27(1), 7185.Google Scholar
Fabry, R. E. (2019). Getting it: A predictive processing approach to irony comprehension. Synthese, 135.Google Scholar
Filik, R., Brightman, E., Gathercole, C., & Leuthold, H. (2017). The emotional impact of verbal irony: Eye-tracking evidence for a two-stage process. Journal of Memory and Language, 93, 193202.Google Scholar
Filik, R., Howman, H., Ralph-Nearman, C., & Giora, R. (2018). The role of defaultness and personality factors in sarcasm interpretation: Evidence from eye-tracking during reading. Metaphor and Symbol, 33(3), 148162.Google Scholar
Filik, R., Leuthold, H., Wallington, K., & Page, J. (2014). Testing theories of irony processing using eye-tracking and ERPs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40(3), 811.Google Scholar
Filik, R., & Moxey, L. M. (2010). The on-line processing of written irony. Cognition, 116(3), 421436.Google Scholar
Forabosco, G. (1992). Cognitive aspects of the humor process: The concept of incongruity. Humor: International Journal of Humor Research, 5(1–2), 4568.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Fox Tree, J. E., D’Arcey, J. T., Hammond, A. A., & Larson, A. S. (2020). The sarchasm: Sarcasm production and identification in spontaneous conversation. Discourse Processes, 127.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. W. (1986). On the psycholinguistics of sarcasm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115(1), 315.Google Scholar
Gibbs, Jr., R. W. (1994). The poetics of mind: Figurative thought, language, and understanding. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. W., & Colston, H. (2012). Interpreting figurative language. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Giora, R. (1995). On irony and negation. Discourse Processes, 19(2), 239264.Google Scholar
Giora, R. (2003). On our mind: Salience, context, and figurative language. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Giora, R., Fein, O., Kronrod, A., Elnatan, I., Shuval, N., & Zur, A. (2004). Weapons of mass distraction: Optimal innovation and pleasure ratings. Metaphor and Symbol, 19(2), 115141.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giora, R., Givoni, S., & Fein, O. (2015). Defaultness reigns: The case of sarcasm. Metaphor and Symbol, 30(4), 290313.Google Scholar
Giora, R., Givoni, S., Heruti, V., & Fein, O. (2017a). The role of defaultness in affecting pleasure: The optimal innovation hypothesis revisited. Metaphor and Symbol, 32(1), 118.Google Scholar
Giora, R., Meytes, D., Tamir, A., Givoni, S., Heruti, V., & Fein, O. (2017b). Defaultness shines while affirmation pales. In Athanasiadou, A.& Colston, H. (Eds.), Irony in language use and communication (pp. 219236). Benjamins.Google Scholar
Gironzetti, E. (2017) Multimodal and eye-tracking evidence in the negotiation of pragmatic intentions in dyadic conversations: The case of humorous discourse (unpublished PhD dissertation, Texas A&M University-Commerce).Google Scholar
Gironzetti, E. (2022) The multimodal performance of conversational humor. Benjamins.Google Scholar
Grice, H. P. (1989). Studies in the way of words. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Howman, H. E., & Filik, R. (2020). The role of emoticons in sarcasm comprehension in younger and older adults: Evidence from an eye-tracking experiment. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 73(11), 17291744. https://doi.org/10.1177/1747021820922804Google Scholar
Huang, Y. T., & Snedeker, J. (2009). Online interpretation of scalar quantifiers: Insight into the semantics–pragmatics interface. Cognitive Psychology, 58(3), 376415.Google Scholar
Ivanko, S. L., & Pexman, P. M. (2003). Context incongruity and irony processing. Discourse Processes, 35(3), 241279.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jacob, H., Kreifelts, B., Nizielski, S., Schütz, A., & Wildgruber, D. (2016). Effects of emotional intelligence on the impression of irony created by the mismatch between verbal and nonverbal cues. PloS One, 11(10), e0163211.Google Scholar
Janisse, M. P. (1977). Pupillometry: The psychology of the pupillary response. Wiley.Google Scholar
Just, M. A., & Carpenter, P. A. (1980). A theory of reading: From eye fixations to comprehension. Psychological Review, 87(4), 329354.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kaakinen, J. K., Olkoniemi, H., Kinnari, T., & Hyönä, J. (2014). Processing of written irony: An eye movement study. Discourse Processes, 51(4), 287311.Google Scholar
Katz, A. (2017). The standard experimental approach to the study of irony: Let us not be hasty in throwing out the baby with the bath water. In Athanasiadou, A. & Colston, H. (Eds.), Irony in language use and communication (pp. 237254). Benjamins.Google Scholar
Katz, A. N., & Ferretti, T. R. (2001). Moment-by-moment reading of proverbs in literal and nonliteral contexts. Metaphor and Symbol, 16(3–4), 193221.Google Scholar
Kennison, S. M. (2020). The cognitive neuroscience of humor. American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Kowatch, K., Whalen, J. M., & Pexman, P. M. (2013). Irony comprehension in action: A new test of processing for verbal irony. Discourse Processes, 50, 301315.Google Scholar
Kreuz, R. (2020). Irony and sarcasm. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Noveck, I. (2018). Experimental pragmatics: The making of a cognitive science. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Olkoniemi, H. (2019) Individual differences in processing written irony (PhD dissertation, University of Turku).Google Scholar
Olkoniemi, H., Johander, E., & Kaakinen, J. K. (2019a). The role of look-backs in the processing of written sarcasm. Memory & Cognition, 47(1), 87105.Google Scholar
Olkoniemi, H., & Kaakinen, J. K. (2021). Processing of irony in text: A systematic review of eye tracking studies. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 75(2), 99106.Google Scholar
Olkoniemi, H., Ranta, H., & Kaakinen, J. K. (2016). Individual differences in the processing of written sarcasm and metaphor: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42(3), 433.Google Scholar
Olkoniemi, H., Strömberg, V., & Kaakinen, J. K. (2019b). The ability to recognise emotions predicts the time-course of sarcasm processing: Evidence from eye movements. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72(5), 12121223.Google Scholar
Panzeri, F., Giustolisi, B., Mantovan, L., & Calderone, C. (2019). Joker face. Recognizing irony in the visual mode in spoken and signed language. Proceedings of the 22nd Amsterdam Colloquium, Institute for Logic, Language and Computation (ILLC) (pp. 603611).Google Scholar
Paterson, K. B., Sanford, A. J., Moxey, L. M., & Dawydiak, E. (1998). Quantifier polarity and referential focus during reading. Journal of Memory and Language, 39(2), 290306.Google Scholar
Pexman, P. M. (2008). It’s fascinating research: The cognition of verbal irony. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 17(4), 286290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pexman, P. M., Ferretti, T. R., & Katz, A. N. (2000). Discourse factors that influence online reading of metaphor and irony. Discourse Processes, 29(3), 201222.Google Scholar
Pexman, P. M., Rostad, K. R., McMorris, C. A., Climie, E. A., Stowkowy, J., & Glenwright, M. R. (2011). Processing of ironic language in children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 41(8), 10971112.Google Scholar
Pickering, L., Corduas, M., Eisterhold, J., Seifried, B., Eggleston, A., & Attardo, S. (2009). Prosodic markers of saliency in humorous narratives. Discourse Processes, 46(6), 517540.Google Scholar
Randall, J. G., Oswald, F. L., & Beier, M. E. (2014). Mind-wandering, cognition, and performance: A theory-driven meta-analysis of attention regulation. Psychological Bulletin, 140(6), 14111431.Google Scholar
Rockwell, P. (2000). Lower, slower, louder: Vocal cues of sarcasm. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 29(5), 483495.Google Scholar
Ruytenbeek, N. (2017). The comprehension of indirect requests: Previous work and future directions. In Depraetere, I. & Salkie, R. (Eds.), Semantics and pragmatics: Drawing a line (pp. 293322). Springer.Google Scholar
Schwoebel, J., Dews, S., Winner, E., & Srinivas, K. (2000). Obligatory processing of the literal meaning of ironic utterances: Further evidence. Metaphor and Symbol, 15(1–2), 4761.Google Scholar
Serlin, G. (2012). Gaze allocation to the speaker’s face during ironic and sincere statements: Relations to features of the broad autism phenotype in typically developing children and adults (unpublished PhD dissertation, City University of New York).Google Scholar
Simarro Vázquez, M., El Khatib, N., Hamrick, P., & Attardo, S. (2020). On the order of processing of humorous tweets with visual and verbal elements. Internet Pragmatics. https://doi.org/10.1075/ip.00060.simGoogle Scholar
Simons, D. J., & Chabris, C. F. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28, 10591074.Google Scholar
Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (1981). Irony and the use-mention distinction. In Cole, P. (Ed.), Radical Pragmatics (pp. 295318). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Tabacaru, S., & Lemmens, M. (2014). Raised eyebrows as gestural triggers in humour: The case of sarcasm and hyper-understanding. The European Journal of Humour Research, 2(2), 1131.Google Scholar
Titz, J., Scholz, A., & Sedlmeier, P. (2018). Comparing eye trackers by correlating their eye-metric data. Behavior Research Methods, 50(5), 18531863.Google Scholar
Tromp, J., Hagoort, P., & Meyer, A. S. (2016). Pupillometry reveals increased pupil size during indirect request comprehension. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69(6), 10931108.Google Scholar
Trott, S., Reed, S., Ferreira, V., & Bergen, B. (2019). Prosodic cues signal the intent of potential indirect requests. Proceedings of the 41st Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 1142–1148), Montreal, Canada.Google Scholar
Turcan, A., & Filik, R. (2016). An eye-tracking investigation of written sarcasm comprehension: The roles of familiarity and context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42(12), 18671893.Google Scholar
Turcan, A., & Filik, R. (2017). Investigating sarcasm comprehension using eye-tracking during reading: What are the roles of literality, familiarity, and echoic mention? In Athanasiadou, A. & Colston, H. L. (Eds.), Irony in language use and communication (pp. 255276). Benjamins.Google Scholar
Țurcan, A., Howman, H., & Filik, R. (2020). Examining the role of context in written sarcasm comprehension: Evidence from EYE-TRACKING DURING READING. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 46(10), 19661976. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000910Google Scholar
Utsumi, A. (2000). Verbal irony as implicit display of ironic environment: Distinguishing ironic utterances from nonirony. Journal of Pragmatics, 32, 17771806.Google Scholar
van der Wel, P., & van Steenbergen, H. (2018). Pupil dilation as an index of effort in cognitive control tasks: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 25(6), 20052015.Google Scholar
Vrij, A., & Fisher, R. P. (2020). Unravelling the misconception about deception and nervous behavior. Frontiers in Psychology, 11(1377), 18.Google Scholar
Vulchanova, M., Chahboun, S., Galindo-Prieto, B., & Vulchanov, V. (2019). Gaze and motor traces of language processing: Evidence from autism spectrum disorders in comparison to typical controls. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 36(7–8), 383409.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Whalen, J. M., Doyle, A., & Pexman, P. M. (2020). Sarcasm between siblings: Children’s use of relationship information in processing ironic remarks. Journal of Pragmatics, 156, 149159.Google Scholar
Williams, J. A., Burns, E. L., & Harmon, E. A. (2009). Insincere utterances and gaze: Eye contact during sarcastic statements. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 108(2), 565572.Google Scholar
Wrench, J. S., & McCroskey, J. C. (2001). A temperamental understanding of humor communication and exhilaratability. Communication Quarterly, 49(2), 142159.Google Scholar
Younis, M. H., Salik, M., Gul, S. T., & Aleem, A. (2021, July). Automatic human facial affect classification using computational intelligence techniques. In 2021 Mohammad Ali Jinnah University International Conference on Computing (MAJICC) (pp. 1–7). IEEE.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×