Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-mkpzs Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-22T07:03:16.321Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Part V - Irony, Affect, and Related Figures

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  20 December 2023

Herbert L. Colston
Affiliation:
University of Alberta
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2023

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Apter, M., & Smith, K. C. P. (1977). The theory of humorous reversals. In Chapman, A. & Foot, H. (Eds.), It’s a funny thing, humour (pp. 95100). Pergamon.Google Scholar
Attardo, S. (2001). Humor, irony and their communication: From mode adoption to failure of detection. In Anolli, L., Ciceri, R., & Riva, G. (Eds.), Say not to say: New perspectives on miscommunication (pp. 159179). IOS Press.Google Scholar
Barbe, K. (1995). Irony in context. John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Bateson, G. (1972 [1955]). A theory of play and fantasy. In Bateson, G. (Ed.), Steps to an Ecology of Mind (pp. 177193). Chandler.Google Scholar
Baxter, J. (2014). Double-voicing at work. Palgrave.Google Scholar
Beckson, K., & Ganz, A. (1989). Literary terms: A dictionary (3rd ed.). Noonday.Google Scholar
Berger, A. A. (1993). An anatomy of humor. Transaction Publishers.Google Scholar
Bryant, G., & Gibbs, R. (2015). Behavioral complexities of ironic humor. In Brône, G., Feyaerts, K., & Veale, T. (Eds.), Cognitive linguistics and humor research (pp. 147166). Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Canestrari, C., Bianchi, I., & Cori, V. (2018). De-polarizing verbal irony. Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 30, 4362.Google Scholar
Colston, H. (2000). On necessary conditions for verbal irony comprehension. Pragmatics and Cognition, 8, 277324.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Colston, H. (2002). Contrast and assimilation in verbal irony. Journal of Pragmatics, 34, 111142.Google Scholar
Colston, H. L., & O’Brien, J. (2000). Contrast of kind versus contrast of magnitude: The pragmatic of accomplishments of irony and hyperbole. Discourse Processes, 30(2), 179199.Google Scholar
Dentith, S. (2000). Parody (the new critical idiom). Routledge.Google Scholar
Dews, S., Kaplan, J., & Winner, E. (1995). Why not say it directly? The social functions of irony. Discourse Processes, 19, 347367.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dynel, M. (2012). Humour on the house: Interactional construction of metaphor in film discourse. In Chovanec, J. & Ermida, I. (Eds.), Language and humour in the media (pp. 83106). Cambridge Scholars Publishing.Google Scholar
Dynel, M. (2013a). Irony from a neo-Gricean perspective: On untruthfulness and evaluative implicature. Intercultural Pragmatics, 10, 403431.Google Scholar
Dynel, M. (2013b). When does irony tickle the hearer? Towards capturing the characteristics of humorous irony. In Dynel, M. (Ed.), Developments in linguistic humour theory (pp. 298320). John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dynel, M. (2014). Isn’t it ironic? Defining the scope of humorous irony. Humor, 27, 619640.Google Scholar
Dynel, M. (2016). Two layers of overt untruthfulness: When irony meets metaphor, hyperbole, or meiosis. Pragmatics & Cognition, 23, 2, 259283.Google Scholar
Dynel, M. (2017a). Academics vs. American scriptwriters vs. Academics: A battle over the etic and emic “sarcasm” and “irony” labels. Language & Communication, 55, 6987.Google Scholar
Dynel, M. (2017b). But seriously: On conversational humour and (un)truthfulness. Lingua, 197, 83102.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dynel, M. (2017c). The irony of irony: Irony based on truthfulness. Corpus Pragmatics, 1, 3. https://doi.org/10.1007/s41701-016-0003-6Google Scholar
Dynel, M. (2018a). Irony, deception and humour: Seeking the truth about overt and covert untruthfulness. Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Dynel, M. (2018b). No child’s play: A philosophical pragmatic view of overt pretence as a vehicle for conversational humour. In Tsakona, V. & Chovanec, J. (Eds.), Creating and negotiating humor in everyday interactions (pp. 205228). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Dynel, M. (2018c). Deconstructing the myth of positively evaluative irony. In Jobert, M. and Sorlin, S. (Eds.), The pragmatics of irony and banter (pp. 4157). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Dynel, M. (2019). Irony in action and interaction. Language Sciences, 75, 114.Google Scholar
Dynel, M. (2020). Vigilante disparaging humour at r/IncelTears: Humour as critique of incel ideology. Language & Communication, 74, 114.Google Scholar
Ferguson, M., & Ford, T. (2008). Disparagement humor: A theoretical and empirical review of psychoanalytic, superiority, and social identity theories. Humor, 21, 283312.Google Scholar
Forabosco, G. (1992). Cognitive aspects of the humour process: The concept of incongruity. Humor, 5, 926.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Forabosco, G. (2008). Is the concept of incongruity still a useful construct for the advancement of humor research? Lodz Papers in Pragmatics, 4, 4562.Google Scholar
Fowler, H. W. (1965). Fowler’s modern English usage (2nd ed.). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Garmendia, J. (2010). Irony is critical. Pragmatics and Cognition, 18, 397421.Google Scholar
Garmendia, J. (2014). The clash: Humor and critical attitude in verbal irony. Humor, 27, 641659.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. (2000). Irony in talk among friends. Metaphor and Symbol, 15(1–2), 527.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. (2012). Are ironic acts deliberate? Journal of Pragmatics, 44, 104115.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R., Bryant, G., & Colston, H. (2014). Where’s the humor in irony? Humor, 27, 575595.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R., & Colston, H. (2001). The risks and rewards of ironic communication. In Anolli, L., Ciceri, R., & Riva, G. (Eds.), Say not to say: New perspectives on miscommunication (pp. 188200). IOS Press.Google Scholar
Giora, R. (2003). On our mind: Salience, context, and figurative language. Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Grice, H. P. (1989). Meaning revisited. In Studies in the Way of Words. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Haverkate, H. (1990). A speech act analysis of irony. Journal of Pragmatics, 14, 77109.Google Scholar
Hirsch, G. (2011). Between irony and humor: A pragmatic model. Pragmatics and Cognition, 19, 530561.Google Scholar
Hirsch, G. (2020). Irony, humor or both? In Livnat, Z., Shukrun-Nagar, P., & Hirsch, G. (Eds.), The discourse of indirectness: Cues, voices and functions (pp. 1738). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Holdcroft, D. (1983). Irony as trope, and irony as discourse. Poetics Today, 4, 493511.Google Scholar
Jorgensen, J. (1996). The functions of sarcastic irony in speech. Journal of Pragmatics, 26, 613634.Google Scholar
Kalbermatten, M. I. (2010). Humor in verbal irony. In Koike, D. & Rodríguez-Alfano, L. (Eds.), Dialogue in Spanish: Studies in functions and contexts (pp. 6988). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Kapogianni, E. (2011). Irony via “surrealism”. In Dynel, M. (Ed.), The pragmatics of humour across discourse domains (pp. 5168). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Kapogianni, E. (2014). Differences in use and function of verbal irony between real and fictional discourse: (Mis)interpretation and irony blindness. Humor, 27, 597618.Google Scholar
Kapogianni, E. (2016a). The ironist’s intentions: Communicative priority and manifestness. Pragmatics & Cognition, 23(1), 150173.Google Scholar
Kapogianni, E. (2016b). The ironic operation: Revisiting the components of ironic meaning. Journal of Pragmatics, 91, 1628.Google Scholar
Koestler, A. (1964). The Act of Creation. Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Kotthoff, H. (2003). Responding to irony in different contexts: On cognition in conversation. Journal of Pragmatics, 35, 13871411.Google Scholar
Kotthoff, H. (2007). Pragmatics of performance and the analysis of conversational humor. Humor, 19(3), 271304.Google Scholar
Kreuz, R., & Glucksberg, S. (1989). How to be sarcastic: The echoic reminder theory of verbal irony. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 118, 374386.Google Scholar
Kreuz, R., & Roberts, R. (1993). On satire and parody: The importance of being ironic. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 8, 97109.Google Scholar
Kumon-Nakamura, S., Glucksberg, S., & Brown, M. (1995). How about another piece of pie: The allusional pretense theory of discourse irony. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 321.Google Scholar
Littman, D., & Mey, J. (1991). The nature of irony: Toward a computational model of irony. Journal of Pragmatics, 15, 131151.Google Scholar
Lucariello, J. (1994). Situational irony: A concept of events gone awry. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123(2), 129145.Google Scholar
Martin, R. (2007). The psychology of humour. An integrative approach. ElsevierGoogle Scholar
Martin, R., & Ford, T. (2018). The psychology of humour. An integrative approach. Elsevier.Google Scholar
Mayerhofer, B. (2013). Perspective clashing as a humour mechanism. In Dynel, M. (Ed.), Developments in linguistic humour theory (pp. 211234). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
McGhee, P. (1979). Humor: Its origin and development. Freeman.Google Scholar
Morreall, J. (1983). Taking laughter seriously. State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Morreall, J. (1987). The philosophy of laughter and humor. State University of New York Press.Google Scholar
Morreall, J. (2008). Philosophy and religion. In Raskin, V. (Ed.), The Primer of Humor Research (pp. 211242). Mouton de Gruyter.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Muecke, D. (1969). The compass of irony. Methuen.Google Scholar
Norrick, N. (1993). Conversational joking: Humor in everyday talk. Indiana University Press.Google Scholar
Nunberg, G. (2001). The way we talk now. Houghton Mifflin.Google Scholar
Palmer, J. (1994). Taking humour seriously. Routledge.Google Scholar
Park-Ozee, D. (2019). Satire: An explication. Humor, 32, 585604.Google Scholar
Partington, A. (2006). The linguistics of laughter. A corpus-assisted study of laughter-talk. Routledge.Google Scholar
Partington, A. (2007). Irony and the reversal of evaluation. Journal of Pragmatics, 39, 15471569.Google Scholar
Piskorska, A. (2014). A relevance-theoretic perspective on humorous irony and its failure. Humor, 27, 661685.Google Scholar
Piskorska, A. (2016) Echo and inadequacy in ironic utterances. Journal of Pragmatics, 101, 5465.Google Scholar
Reimer, M. (2013). Grice on irony and metaphor: Discredited by the experimental evidence? International Review of Pragmatics, 5, 133.Google Scholar
Ritchie, G. (2004). The linguistic analysis of jokes. Routledge.Google Scholar
Rose, M. (1993). Parody: Ancient, modern and post-modern. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Rossen-Knill, D., & Henry, R. (1997). The pragmatics of verbal parody. Journal of Pragmatics, 27, 719752.Google Scholar
Shelley, C. (2001). The bicoherence theory of situational irony. Cognitive Science, 25, 775818.Google Scholar
Simpson, P. (2003). On the discourse of satire: Towards a stylistic model of satirical humor. John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Simpson, P. (2011). “That’s not ironic, that’s just stupid”: Towards an eclectic account of the discourse of irony. In Dynel, M. (Ed.), The pragmatics of humour across discourse domains (pp. 3350). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Sinkeviciute, V. (2022). Teasing. In F. Brisard, S. D’hondt, P. Gras and M. Vandenbroucke (Eds.), Handbook of pragmatics (pp. 156–176). John Benjamins Publishing.Google Scholar
Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (1981). Irony and the use-mention distinction. In Cole, P. (Ed.), Radical Pragmatics (pp. 295318). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Suls, J. (1972). A two-stage model for the appreciation of jokes and cartoons: An information processing analysis. In Goldstein, J. & McGhee, P. (Eds.), The psychology of humor (pp. 81100). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Suls, J. (1977). Cognitive and disparagement theories of humor: A theoretical and empirical synthesis. In Chapman, A. J. & Foot, H. C. (Eds.), It’s a funny thing, humor (pp. 4145). Pergamon Press.Google Scholar
Suls, J. (1983). Cognitive processes in humor appreciation. In McGhee, P. & Goldstein, J. (Eds.), Handbook of humor research, Vol. 1 (pp. 3957). Springer Verlag.Google Scholar
Taylor, C. (2016). Mock politeness in English and Italian. A corpus-assisted metalanguage analysis. John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Tsakona, V. (2018). Online joint fictionalization. In Tsakona, V. & Chovanec, J. (Eds.), The dynamics of interactional humor: creating and negotiating humor in everyday encounters (pp. 229255). John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Utsumi, A. (2000). Verbal irony as implicit display of ironic environment: Distinguishing ironic utterances from nonirony. Journal of Pragmatics, 32, 17771806.Google Scholar
Vandaele, J. (2002). Humor mechanisms in film comedy: Incongruity and superiority. Poetics Today, 23(2), 221249.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. (2004 [1967]). Imagination and creativity in childhood (trans. E. Sharp). Journal of Russian East European Psychology, 42, 997.Google Scholar
Wilson, D. (2017). Irony, hyperbole, jokes and banter. In Blochowiak, J., Grisot, C., Durrleman-Tame, S., & Laenzlinger, C. (Eds.), Formal models in the study of language (pp. 201220). Springer.Google Scholar
Yus, F. (2016). Humor and relevance. John BenjaminGoogle Scholar
Zillmann, D. (1983). Disparagement humor. In McGhee, P. and Goldstein, J. (Eds.), Handbook of Humor Research, Vol. 1 (pp. 85107). Springer Verlag.Google Scholar

References

Au-Yeung, S. K., Kaakinen, J. K., Liversedge, S. P., & Benson, V. (2015). Processing of written irony in autism spectrum disorder: An eye-movement study. Autism Research, 8, 749760.Google Scholar
Barnden, J. A. (2020). Uniting irony, metaphor and hyperbole in a pretence-based, affect-centred framework. In Athanasiadou, A. & Colston, H. L. (Eds.), The diversity of irony (pp. 1565). De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Barzy, M., Filik, R., Williams, D., & Ferguson, H. J. (2020). Emotional processing of ironic versus literal criticism in autistic and nonautistic adults: Evidence from eye-tracking. Autism Research, 13, 563578.Google Scholar
Blasko, D. G., & Kazmerski, V. A. (2006). ERP Correlates of individual differences in the comprehension of nonliteral language. Metaphor and Symbol, 21, 267284.Google Scholar
Blasko, D. G., Kazmerski, V. A., & Dawood, S. S. (2021). Saying that you don’t mean: A cross-cultural study of perceptions of sarcasm. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 75, 114119.Google Scholar
Bowes, A., & Katz, A. (2011). When sarcasm stings. Discourse Processes, 48, 215236.Google Scholar
Brown, P., & Levinson, S. (1987). Politeness. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Caffarra, S., Haeri, A. M., Michel, E., & Martin, C. D. (2019). When is irony influenced by communicative constraints? ERP evidence supporting interactive models. European Journal of Neuroscience, 50, 35663577.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Caillies, S., Gobin, P., Obert, A., Terrion, S., Coutté, A., Iakimova, G., & Besche-Richard, C. (2019). Asymmetry of affect in verbal irony understanding: What about the N400 and P600 components? Journal of Neurolinguistics, 51, 268277.Google Scholar
Clark, H., & Gerrig, R. (1984). On the pretense theory of irony. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 121126.Google Scholar
Colston, H. L. (2007). Salting a wound or sugaring a pill: The pragmatic functions of ironic criticism. In Gibbs, R. W. & Colston, H. L. (Eds.), Irony in language and thought (pp. 319338). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Colston, H. L., & Keller, S. B. (1998). “You’ll never believe this”: Irony and hyperbole in expressing surprise. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 27, 499513.Google Scholar
Colston, H., & O’Brien, J. (2000). Contrast and pragmatics in figurative language: Anything understatement can do, irony can do better. Journal of Pragmatics, 32, 15571583.Google Scholar
Davis, M. H. (1980). A multi-dimensional approach to individual differences in empathy. JSAS Catalog of Selected Documents in Psychology, 10, 85.Google Scholar
Del Goleto, S., Kostova, M., & Blanchet, A. (2016). Impaired context processing during irony comprehension in schizotypy: An ERPs study. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 105, 1725.Google Scholar
Dews, S., Kaplan, J., & Winner, E. (1995). Why not say it directly? The social functions of irony. Discourse Processes, 19, 347367.Google Scholar
Dews, S., & Winner, E. (1995). Muting the meaning: A social function of irony. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 10, 319.Google Scholar
Dress, M. L., Kreuz, R. J., Link, K. E., & Caucci, G. M. (2008). Regional variation in the use of sarcasm. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 27, 7185.Google Scholar
Filik, R., Brightman, E., Gathercole, C., & Leuthold, H. (2017). The emotional impact of verbal irony: Eye-tracking evidence for a two-stage process. Journal of Memory and Language, 93, 193202.Google Scholar
Filik, R., Howman, H., Ralph-Nearman, C., & Giora, R. (2018). The role of defaultness and personality factors in sarcasm interpretation: Evidence from eye-tracking during reading. Metaphor and Symbol, 33, 148162.Google Scholar
Filik, R., Hunter, C. M., & Leuthold, H. (2015). When language gets emotional: Irony and the embodiment of affect in discourse. Acta Psychologica, 156, 114125.Google Scholar
Filik, R., Leuthold, H., Wallington, K., & Page, J. (2014). Testing theories of irony processing using eye-tracking and ERPs. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 40, 811828.Google Scholar
Filik, R., & Moxey, L. M. (2010). The on-line processing of written irony. Cognition, 116, 421436.Google Scholar
Filik, R., Țurcan, A., Thompson, D., Harvey, N., Davies, H., & Turner, A. (2016). Sarcasm and emoticons: Comprehension and emotional impact. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 69, 21302146.Google Scholar
Garcia, C., Țurcan, A., Howman, H., & Filik, R. (2022). Emoji as a tool to aid the comprehension of written sarcasm: Evidence from younger and older adults. Computers in Human Behavior, 126, 106971.Google Scholar
Glenwright, M., & Agbayewa, A. S. (2012). Older children and adolescents with high-functioning autism spectrum disorders can comprehend verbal irony in computer-mediated communication. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 6, 628638.Google Scholar
Hancock, J. T. (2004). Verbal irony use in face-to-face and computer-mediated conversations. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 23(4), 447463.Google Scholar
Harris, M., & Pexman, P. M. (2003). Children’s perceptions of the social functions of verbal irony. Discourse Processes, 36, 147165.Google Scholar
Henry, J. D., Phillips, L. H., Ruffman, T., & Bailey, P. E. (2013). A meta-analytic review of age differences in theory of mind. Psychology and Aging, 28, 826839.Google Scholar
Howman, H., & Filik, R. (2020). The role of emoticons in sarcasm comprehension in younger and older adults: Evidence from an eye-tracking experiment. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 73, 17291744.Google Scholar
Ivanko, S. L., Pexman, P. M., & Olineck, K. M. (2004). How sarcastic are you? Individual differences and verbal irony. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 23, 244271.Google Scholar
Jiang, T., Li, H., & Hou, Y. (2019). Cultural differences in humour perception, usage, and implications. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 123123.Google Scholar
Jorgensen, J. (1996). The functions of sarcastic irony in speech. Journal of Pragmatics, 26, 613634.Google Scholar
Kaakinen, J. K., Olkoniemi, H., Kinnari, T., & Hyönä, J. (2014). Processing of written irony: An eye movement study. Discourse Processes, 51, 287311.Google Scholar
Kim, J., & Lantolf, J. P. (2018). Developing conceptual understanding of sarcasm in L2 English through explicit instruction. Language Teaching Research: LTR, 22, 208229.Google Scholar
Kowatch, K., Whalen, J. M., & Pexman, P. M. (2013). Irony comprehension in action: A new test of processing for verbal irony. Discourse Processes, 50, 301315.Google Scholar
Kreuz, R. J., Long, D. L., & Church, M. B. (1991). On being ironic: Pragmatic and mnemonic implications. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 6, 149162.Google Scholar
Kumon-Nakamura, S., Glucksberg, S., & Brown, M. (1995). How about another piece of pie: The allusional pretense theory of discourse irony. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 321.Google Scholar
Lampert, M. D., & Ervin-Tripp, S. M. (2006). Risky laughter: Teasing and self-directed joking among male and female friends. Journal of Pragmatics, 38, 5172.Google Scholar
Lee, C., & Katz, A. (1998). The differential role of ridicule in sarcasm and irony. Metaphor and Symbol, 13, 115.Google Scholar
Leech, G. N. (1983). Principles of pragmatics. Longman.Google Scholar
Leggitt, J. S., & Gibbs, R. W. (2000). Emotional reactions to verbal irony. Discourse Processes, 29, 124.Google Scholar
Matthews, J. K., Hancock, J. T., & Dunham, P. J. (2006). The role of politeness and humor in the asymmetry of affect in verbal irony. Discourse Processes, 41, 324.Google Scholar
Olkoniemi, H., Ranta, H., & Kaakinen, J. K. (2016). Individual differences in the processing of written sarcasm and metaphor: Evidence from eye movements. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42, 433450.Google Scholar
Olkoniemi, H., Strömberg, V., & Kaakinen, J. K. (2019). The ability to recognise emotions predicts the time-course of sarcasm processing: Evidence from eye movements. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 72, 12121223.Google Scholar
Pexman, P. M., Rostad, K. R., McMorris, C. A., Climie, E. A., Stowkowy, J., & Glenwright, M. R. (2011). Processing of ironic language in children with high-functioning autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorder, 41, 10971112.Google Scholar
Pexman, P. M., & Zvaigzne, M. T. (2004). Does irony go better with friends? Metaphor and Symbol, 19, 143163.Google Scholar
Phillips, L. H., Allen, R., Bull, R., Hering, A., Kliegel, M., & Channon, S. (2015). Older adults have difficulty in decoding sarcasm. Developmental Psychology, 51, 1840–1852.Google Scholar
Pickering, B., Thompson, D., & Filik, R. (2018). Examining the emotional impact of sarcasm using a virtual environment. Metaphor and Symbol, 33, 185197.Google Scholar
Pomareda, C., Simkute, A., & Phillips, L. H. (2019). Age-related differences in the ability to decode intentions from nonliteral language. Acta Psychologica, 198, 102865.Google Scholar
Regel, S., Coulson, S., & Gunter, T. C. (2010). The communicative style of a speaker can affect language comprehension? ERP evidence from the comprehension of irony. Brain Research, 1311, 121135.Google Scholar
Regel, S., Gunter, T. C., & Friederici, A. D. (2011). Isn’t It Ironic? An electrophysiological exploration of figurative language processing. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 23, 277293.Google Scholar
Regel, S., Meyer, L., & Gunter, T. C. (2014). Distinguishing neurocognitive processes reflected by P600 effects: Evidence from ERPs and neural oscillations. PLoS ONE, 9, e96840.Google Scholar
Roberts, R. M., & Kreutz, R. J. (1994). Why do people use figurative language? Psychological Science, 5, 159163.Google Scholar
Ruffman, T., Henry, J. D., Livingstone, V., & Phillips, L. H. (2008). A meta-analytic review of emotion recognition and aging: Implications for neuropsychological models of aging. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 32, 863881.Google Scholar
Slugoski, B., & Turnbull, W. (1988). Cruel to be kind and kind to be cruel: Sarcasm, banter, and social relations. Journal of Language & Social Psychology, 7, 101121.Google Scholar
Spotorno, N., Koun, E., Prado, J., Van Der Henst, J. B., & Noveck, I. A. (2012). Neural evidence that utterance-processing entails mentalizing: The case of irony. Neuroimage, 63, 2539.Google Scholar
Stone, V. E., Baron-Cohen, S., & Knight, R. T. (1998). Frontal lobe contributions to theory of mind. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 10, 640656.Google Scholar
Thompson, D., & Filik, R. (2016). Sarcasm in written communication: Emoticons are efficient markers of intention. Journal of Computer-mediated Communication, 21, 105120.Google Scholar
Thompson, D., Leuthold, H., & Filik, R. (2021). Examining the influence of perspective and prosody on expected emotional responses to irony: Evidence from event-related brain potentials. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 75, 107113.Google Scholar
Thompson, D., Mackenzie, I. G., Leuthold, H., & Filik, R. (2016). Emotional responses to irony and emoticons in written language: Evidence from EDA and facial EMG. Psychophysiology, 53, 10541062.Google Scholar
Toplak, M., & Katz, A. N. (2000). On the uses of sarcastic irony. Journal of Pragmatics, 32, 14671488.Google Scholar
Țurcan, A., & Filik, R. (2016). An eye-tracking investigation of written sarcasm comprehension: The roles of familiarity and context. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 42, 18671893.Google Scholar
T¸urcan, A., & Filik, R. (2017). Investigating sarcasm comprehension using eye- tracking during reading: What are the roles of literality, familiarity, and echoic mention? In Athanasiadou, A. & Colston, H. (Eds.), Irony in language use and communication (pp. 255276). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Țurcan, A., Howman, H., & Filik, R. (2020). Examining the role of context in written sarcasm comprehension: Evidence from eye-tracking during reading. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 46, 19661976.Google Scholar
Weissman, B., & Tanner, D. (2018). A strong wink between verbal and emoji-based irony: How the brain processes ironic emojis during language comprehension. PLoS ONE, 13, e0201727.Google Scholar
Whalen, J. M., Pexman, P. M., & Gill, A. J. (2009). “Should be fun—not!” Incidence and marking of verbal irony in email. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 28, 263280.Google Scholar
Zalla, T., Amsellem, F., Chaste, P., Ervas, F., Leboyer, M., & Champagne-Lavau, M. (2014). Individuals with autism spectrum disorders do not use social stereotypes in irony comprehension. PLoS ONE, 9, e95568.Google Scholar
Zhu, N., & Filik, R. (2023 ). Individual differences in sarcasm interpretation and use: Evidence from the UK and China. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 49(3), 445463.Google Scholar
Zhu, N., & Wang, Z. (2020). The paradox of sarcasm: Theory of mind and sarcasm use in adults. Personality and Individual Differences, 163, 110035.Google Scholar

References

Athanasiadou, A. (2017). Irony has a metonymic basis. In Athanasiadou, A. & Colston, H. (Eds.), Irony in language use and communication (pp. 201216). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Attardo, S. (2000a). Irony markers and functions. Rask – Int. J. Language and Communication, 12(1), 320.Google Scholar
Attardo, S. (2000b). Irony as relevant inappropriateness. J. Pragmatics, 32(6), 793826.Google Scholar
Baldick, C. (1990). Concise Oxford dictionary of literary terms. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Barnden, J. A. (1995). Simulative reasoning, common-sense psychology and artificial intelligence. In Davies, M. & Stone, T. (Eds.), Mental simulation: Evaluations and applications (pp. 247273). Blackwell.Google Scholar
Barnden, J. A. (2015). Open-ended elaborations in creative metaphor. In Besold, T. R., Schorlemmer, M., & Smaill, A. (Eds.), Computational creativity research: Towards creative machines (pp. 217242). Springer.Google Scholar
Barnden, J. A. (2016). Mixed metaphor: Its depth, its breadth, and a pretence-based approach. In Gibbs, R. W., Jr. (Ed.), Mixing metaphor (pp. 75111). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Barnden, J. A. (2017). Irony, pretence and fictively-elaborating hyperbole. In Athanasiadou, A. & Colston, H. L. (Eds.), Irony in language use and communication (pp. 145177). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Barnden, J. A. (2020a). Uniting irony, metaphor and hyperbole in a pretence-based, affect-centred framework. In Athanasiadou, A. & Colston, H. L. (Eds.), The diversity of irony (pp. 1565). De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Barnden, J. A. (2020b). The meta-dynamic nature of consciousness. Entropy, 22(12), 1433. https://doi.org/10.3390/e22121433Google Scholar
Barnden, J. A. (2022) Pre-reflective self-consciousness: A meta-causal approach. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 13(2), 397425. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-021-00603-zGoogle Scholar
Brdar-Szabó, R., & Brdar, M. (2010). “Mummy, I love you like a thousand ladybirds”: Reflections on the emergence of hyperbolic effects and the truth of hyperboles. In Burkhardt, A. & Nerlich, B. (Eds.), Tropical truth(s): The epistemology of metaphor and other tropes (pp. 383427). De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Burgers, C., & Steen, G. J. (2017). Introducing a three-dimensional model of verbal irony: Irony in language, in thought, and in communication. In Athanasiadou, A. & Colston, H. L. (Eds.), Irony in language use and communication (pp. 87108). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Camp, E. (2012). Sarcasm, pretence and the semantics/pragmatics distinction. Noûs, 46(4), 587634.Google Scholar
Carston, R., & Wearing, C. (2015). Hyperbolic language and its relation to metaphor and irony. Journal of Pragmatics, 79, 7992.Google Scholar
Clark, H. H., & Gerrig, R. J. (2007). On the pretense theory of irony. In Gibbs, R. W., Jr. & Colston, H. L. (Eds.), Irony in language and thought: A cognitive science reader (pp. 2533). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Reprinted from J. Experimental Psychology: General, 113, 121–126 (1984).Google Scholar
Colston, H. L. (2015). Interpreting figurative meaning. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Colston, H. L. (2017). Irony performance and perception: What underlies verbal, situational and other ironies? In Athanasiadou, A. & Colston, H. L. (Eds.), Irony in language use and communication (pp. 1941). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Colston, H. L. (2019). How language makes meaning: Embodiment and conjoined antonymy. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Colston, H. L. (2020). Eye-rolling, irony and embodiment. In Athanasiadou, A. & Colston, H. (Eds.), The diversity of irony (pp. 211235). De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Colston, H. L., & Carreno, A. (2020). Sources of pragmatic effects in irony and hyperbole. In Baicchi, A. (Ed.), Figurative meaning construction in thought and language (pp. 187208). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Colston, H. L., & Keller, S. B. (1998). You’ll never believe this: Irony and hyperbole in expressing surprise. J. Psycholinguistic Research, 27(4), 499513.Google Scholar
Currie, G. (2006). Why irony is pretence. In Nichols, S. (Ed.), The architecture of the imagination (pp. 111133). Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Currie, G. (2010). Echo et feintise: quelle est la difference et qui a raison? Philosophiques, 35(1), 1223.Google Scholar
Davies, M., & Stone, T. (Eds.). (1995). Mental simulation: Evaluations and applications. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Dynel, M. (2018). Deconstructing the myth of positively evaluative irony. In Jobert, M. & Sorlin, S. (Eds.), The pragmatics of irony and banter (pp. 4157). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Fogelin, R. J. (2011). Figuratively speaking (rev. ed.). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. W., Jr. (2007). Irony in talks among friends. In Gibbs, R. W., Jr. & Colston, H. L. (Eds.), Irony in language and thought: A cognitive science reader (pp. 339360). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Reprinted from Metaphor and Symbol, 15, 5–27 (2000).Google Scholar
Jing-Schmidt, Z. (2008). Negativity bias in language: A cognitive-affective model of emotive intensifiers. Cognitive Linguistics, 18(3), 417443.Google Scholar
Kao, J. T., Wu, J. Y., Bergen, L., & Goodman, N. D. (2014). Nonliteral understanding of number words. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 111(33), 1200212007. https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.1407479111Google Scholar
Kreuz, R. J., & Johnson, A. A. (2020). In Barnden, J. A. & Gargett, A. (Eds.), Producing figurative expression: Theoretical, experimental and practical perspectives (pp. 263295). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Kreuz, R. J., & Roberts, R. M. (1995). Two cues for verbal irony: Hyperbole and the ironic tone of voice. Metaphor & Symbol, 10(1), 2131.Google Scholar
Kumon-Nakamura, S., Glucksberg, S., & Brown, M. (2007). How about another piece of pie: The allusional pretense theory of irony. In Gibbs, R. W., Jr. & Colston, H. L. (Eds.), Irony in language and thought: A cognitive science reader (pp. 5795). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. Reprinted from J. Experimental Psychology: General, 124, 3–21 (1995).Google Scholar
Matlin, M. W., Stang, D. J., Gawron, V. J., Freedman, A., & Derby, P. L. (1979). Evaluative meaning as a determinant of spew position. J. General Psychology, 100, 311.Google Scholar
McCarthy, M., & Carter, R. (2004). “There’s millions of them”: Hyperbole in everyday conversation. Journal of Pragmatics, 36(2), 149184.Google Scholar
Musolff, A. (2017). Irony and sarcasm in follow-ups of metaphorical slogans. In Athanasiadou, A. & Colston, H. (Eds.), Irony in language use and communication (pp. 127141). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Musolff, A., & Wong, S. T. D. (2020). England is an appendix; corrupt officials are like hairs on a nation’s arm: Sarcasm, irony and self-irony in figurative political discourse. In Athanasiadou, A. & Colston, H. (Eds.), The diversity of irony (pp. 162182). De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Peña, M. S., & Ruiz de Mendoza, F. J. (2017). Construing and constructing hyperbole. In Athanasiadou, A. (Ed.), Studies in figurative thought and language (pp. 4273). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Popa-Wyatt, M. (2014). Pretence and echo: Towards an integrated account of verbal irony International Review of Pragmatics, 6(1), 127168.Google Scholar
Popa-Wyatt, M. (2020a). Hyperbolic figures. In Athanasiadou, A. & Colston, H. (Eds.), The diversity of irony (pp. 91106). De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Popa-Wyatt, M. (2020b). Mind the gap: Expressing affect with hyperbole and hyperbolic figures. In Barnden, J. A. & Gargett, A. (Eds.), Producing figurative expression: Theoretical, experimental and practical perspectives (pp. 449467). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Récanati, F. (2007). Indexicality, context and pretence: A speech-act theoretic account. In Burton-Roberts, N. (Ed.), Advances in pragmatics (pp. 213229). Palgrave-Macmillan.Google Scholar
Ruiz de Mendoza, F. (2014). Mapping concepts. Understanding figurative thought from a cognitive-linguistic perspective. Revista Española de Lingüística Aplicada, 27(1), 187207.Google Scholar
Ruiz de Mendoza, F. (2017). Cognitive modeling and irony. In Athanasiadou, A. & Colston, H. (Eds.), Irony in language use and communication (pp. 179200). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Ruiz de Mendoza, F. J., & Lozano-Palacio, I. (2021). On verbal and situational irony. In Soares da Silva, A. (Ed.), Figurative language: Intersubjectivity and usage (pp. 213240). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Sperber, D. (1984). Verbal irony: Pretense or echoic mention? J. Experimental Psychology: General, 113(1), 130136.Google Scholar
Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (1981). Irony and the use-mention distinction. In Cole, P. (Ed.), Radical pragmatics (pp. 295318). Academic Press.Google Scholar
Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (1995). Relevance: Communication and cognition (2nd ed.). Blackwell.Google Scholar
Tabacaru, S. (2020). Faces of sarcasm: Exploring raised eyebrows with sarcasm in French political debates. In Athanasiadou, A. & Colston, H. (Eds.), The diversity of irony (pp. 236277). De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Walton, K. L. (1990). Mimesis as make-believe: On the foundations of the representational arts. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Watling, G. (2020). Denying the salient contrast: Speaker’s attitude in hyperbole. In Athanasiadou, A. & Colston, H. (Eds.), The diversity of irony (pp. 107130). De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Willison, R. (2017). In defense of an ecumenical approach to irony. In Athanasiadou, A. & Colston, H. L. (Eds.), Irony in language use and communication (pp. 6183). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Wilson, D. (2006). The pragmatics of verbal irony: Echo or pretence? Lingua, 116, 17221743.Google Scholar
Wilson, D. (2013). Irony comprehension: A developmental perspective. Journal of Pragmatics, 59, 4056. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2012.09.016Google Scholar
Wilson, D. (2017). Irony, hyperbole, jokes and banter. In Blochowlak, J., Grisot, C., Durrleman, S., & Laenzlinger, C. (Eds.), Formal models in the study of language (pp. 201219). Springer.Google Scholar
Wilson, D., & Sperber, D. (2012). Explaining irony. In Wilson, D. & Sperber, D. (Eds.), Meaning and relevance (pp. 123145). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

References

Attardo, S. (2000). Irony as relevant inappropriateness. Journal of Pragmatics, 32(6), 793826.Google Scholar
Barnden, J. (2020). Uniting irony, hyperbole and metaphor in an affect-centred, pretence-based framework. In Colston, H. L. & Athanasiadou, A. (Eds.), The diversity of irony (pp. 1565). De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Burgers, C., Renardel de Lavalette, K. Y., & Steen, G. J. (2018). Metaphor, hyperbole, and irony: Uses in isolation and in combination in written discourse. Journal of Pragmatics, 127, 7183.Google Scholar
Caffi, C. (1999). On mitigation. Journal of Pragmatics, 31(7), 881909.Google Scholar
Caffi, C. (2007). Mitigation. Elsevier.Google Scholar
Cano Mora, L. (2006). ‘How to make a mountain out of a molehill’: A corpus-based pragmatic and conversational analysis study of hyperbole in interaction (doctoral dissertation, Universitat de València). www.tdx.cat/bitstream/handle/10803/9790/cano.pdf?sequence=1Google Scholar
Carston, R., & Wearing, C. (2015). Hyperbolic language and its relation to metaphor and irony. Journal of Pragmatics, 79, 7992.Google Scholar
Claridge, C. (2011). Hyperbole in English: A corpus-based study of exaggeration. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Colston, H. L. (1997). Salting a wound or sugaring a pill: The pragmatic functions of ironic criticism. Discourse Processes, 23(1), 2545.Google Scholar
Colston, H. L. (2015). Using figurative language. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Colston, H. L., & Carreno, A. (2020). Sources of pragmatic effects in irony and hyperbole. In Baicchi, M. (Guest Ed.), Athanasiadou, A. & Colston, H. (Eds.), Figurative meaning construction in thought and language (Vol. 9) (pp. 187208). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Colston, H. L., & Gibbs, R. W. (2002) Are irony and metaphor understood differently? Metaphor and Symbol, 17(1), 5780.Google Scholar
Colston, H. L., & Gibbs, R. W. (2007). A brief history of irony. In Gibbs, R. W. & Colston, H. L. (Eds.), Irony in language and thought: A cognitive science reader (pp. 321). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Curcó, C. (2000). Irony: Negation, echo, and metarepresentation. Lingua, 110, 257280.Google Scholar
Dews, S., Kaplan, J., & Winner, E. (1995). Why not say it directly? The social functions of irony. Discourse Processes, 19(3), 347367.Google Scholar
Dews, S., & Winner, E. (1995). Muting the meaning. A social function of irony. Metaphor and Symbolic Activity, 10(1), 319.Google Scholar
Dynel, M. (2013a). Irony from a neo-Gricean perspective: On untruthfulness and evaluative implicature. Intercultural Pragmatics, 10(3), 403431.Google Scholar
Dynel, M. (2013b). When does irony tickle the hearer? Towards capturing the characteristics of humourous irony. In Dynel, M. (Ed.), Developments in linguistic humour theory (pp. 289320). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Dynel, M. (2016a). Comparing and combining covert and overt untruthfulness. On lying, deception, irony and metaphor. Pragmatics & Cognition, 23(1), 174208.Google Scholar
Dynel, M. (2016b). Two layers of overt untruthfulness: When irony meets metaphor, hyperbole or meiosis. Pragmatics & Cognition, 23(2), 259283.Google Scholar
Eviatar, Z., & Just, M. A. (2006). Brain correlates of discourse processing: An fMRI investigation of irony and conventional metaphor comprehension. Neuropsychologia, 44(12), 23482359.Google Scholar
Fogelin, R. J. (2011). Figuratively speaking (rev. ed.). Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Garmendia, J. (2010). Irony is critical. Pragmatics & Cognition, 18(2), 397421.Google Scholar
Garmendia, J. (2011). She’s (not) a fine friend: “Saying” and criticism in irony. Intercultural Pragmatics, 8(1), 4165.Google Scholar
Garmendia, J. (2015). A (Neo)Gricean account of irony: An answer to Relevance Theory. International Review of Pragmatics, 7(1), 4079.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. W. (1986). On the psycholinguistics of sarcasm. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 115(1), 315.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. W. (1999). The poetics of mind: Figurative thought, language, and understanding. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. W. (2000). Irony in talk among friends. Metaphor and Symbol, 15(1–2), 527.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. W. (Ed.). (2008). The Cambridge handbook of metaphor and thought. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. W., & Colston, H. L. (2012). Interpreting figurative meaning. Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. W., & Colston, H. L. (2002). The risk and rewards of ironic communication. In Riva, G., Anolli, L., & Ciceri, R. (Eds.), Say not to say: New perspectives on miscommunication (pp. 188199). IOS Press.Google Scholar
Giora, R. (1995). On irony and negation. Discourse Processes, 19(2), 239264.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Giora, R. (2003). On our mind. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Giora, R., Fein, O., Ganzi, J., Levi, N. A., & Sabah, H. (2005). On negation as mitigation: The case of negative irony. Discourse Processes, 39(1), 81100.Google Scholar
Grice, H. P. (1989a). Logic and conversation. In Grice, H. P. (Ed.), Studies in the way of words (pp. 2240). Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Grice, H. P. (1989b). Further notes on logic and conversation. In Grice, H. P. (Ed.), Studies in the Way of Words (pp. 4157). Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Groeben, N., & Scheele, B. (1986). Produktion und Rezeption von Ironie: Pragmalinguistische Beschreibung und psycholinguistische Erklärungshypothesen. Narr.Google Scholar
Haiman, J. (1998). Talk is cheap: Sarcasm, alienation, and the evolution of language. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Haverkate, H. (1990). A speech act analysis of irony. Journal of Pragmatics, 14, 77109.Google Scholar
Holdcroft, D. (1983). Irony as a trope, and irony as discourse. Poetics Today, 4(3), 493511.Google Scholar
Huang, Y. (2012). The Oxford dictionary of pragmatics. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Hübler, A. (1983). Understatements and hedges in English. John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Israel, M. (2006). Saying less and meaning less. In Birner, B. J. & Ward, G. L. (Eds.), Drawing the boundaries of meaning: Neo-Gricean studies in pragmatics and semantics in honor of Laurence R. Horn (pp. 137156). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Kapogianni, E. (2011). Irony via “surrealism,” In Dynel, M. (Ed.), The pragmatics of humour across discourse domains (pp. 5168). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Katz, A. N., & Pexman, P. M. (1997). Interpreting figurative statements: Speaker occupation can change metaphor to irony. Metaphor and Symbol, 12(1), 1941.Google Scholar
Kreuz, R. J., & Link, K. E. (2002). Asymmetries in the use of verbal irony. Journal of Language and Social Psychology, 21(2), 127143.Google Scholar
Kreuz, R. J., & Roberts, R. M. (1993). The empirical study of figurative language in literature. Poetics, 22, 151169.Google Scholar
Kreuz, R. J., Roberts, R. M., Johnson, B. K., & Bertus, E. L. (1996) Figurative languga occurrende and co-occurrence in contemporary literature. In Kreuz, R. J. (Ed.), Empirical approaches to literature and aesthetics (pp. 8397). Ablex Publishing.Google Scholar
Kumon-Nakamura, S., Glucksberg, S., & Brown, M. (1995). How about another piece of pie: The allusional pretense theory of discourse irony. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 124(1), 321.Google Scholar
Lausberg, H., & Orton, D. E. (1998). Handbook of literary rhetoric: A foundation for literary study. Brill.Google Scholar
Leech, G. N. (1983). Principles of pragmatics. Longman.Google Scholar
Leech, G. N. (2014). The pragmatics of politeness. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Levinson, S. C. (2000). Presumptive meanings: The theory of generalized conversational implicature. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Livnat, Z. (2011). Quantity, truthfulness and ironic effect. Language Sciences, 33(2), 305315.Google Scholar
McDonald, S. (2000). Neuropsychological studies of sarcasm. Metaphor and Symbol, 15(1–2), 8598.Google Scholar
Meibauer, J. (2012) What is a context? Theoretical and empirical evidence. In Finkbeiner, R., Schumacher, P., & Meibauer, J. (Eds.), What is a context? Linguistic approaches and challenges (pp. 932). John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Musolff, A. (2017) Metaphor, irony and sarcasm in public discourse. Journal of Pragmatics, 109, 95104.Google Scholar
Neuhaus, L. (2016a). Four potential meanings of double negation: The pragmatics of nicht un-constructions. International Review of Pragmatics, 8, 5581.Google Scholar
Neuhaus, L. (2016b). On the relation of irony, understatement, and litotes. Pragmatics & Cognition, 23(1), 117149.Google Scholar
Neuhaus, L. (2019). Linguistik der Litotes im Deutschen. Syntax, Semantik und Pragmatik einer “nicht uninteressanten” Redefigur. De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Partington, A. (2007). Irony and reversal of evaluation. Journal of Pragmatics, 39, 15471569.Google Scholar
Popa, M. (2011). Ironic metaphor interpretation. Toronto Working Papers in Linguistics, 33(1). University of Toronto. https://twpl.library.utoronto.ca/index.php/twpl/article/view/6913Google Scholar
Popa-Wyatt, M. (2020). Hyperbolic figures In Colston, H. L. , A. Athanasiadou, (Eds.), The diversity of irony (pp. 91106). De Gruyter Mouton.Google Scholar
Quintilian, M. F. ([35–100 AD] 1959). Institutes of oratory (trans. H. E. Butler). University of Toronto.Google Scholar
Roberts, R. M., & Kreuz, R. J. (1994). Why do people use figurative language? Psychological Science, 5(3), 159163.Google Scholar
Schmude, M. P. (2001). Minutio. In Ueding, G. (Ed.), Historisches Wörterbuch der Rhetorik: Vol. 5 L-Musi (pp. 13601366). Niemeyer.Google Scholar
Semino, E., & Demjén, Z. (Eds.). (2016). The Routledge handbook of metaphor and language. Routledge.Google Scholar
Sperber, D., & Wilson, D. (1986). Relevance: Communication and cognition. Blackwell.Google Scholar
Stalnaker, R. (2002). Common ground. Linguistics and Philosophy, 25, 701721.Google Scholar
Wilson, D. (2009). Irony and metarepresentation. UCL Working Papers in Linguistics, 21, 183226.Google Scholar
Wilson, D. (2017). Irony, hyberbole, jokes and banter. Blochowiak, J., Grisot, C., Durrleman-Tame, S., & Laenzlinger, C. (Eds.), Formal models in the study of language (pp. 201219). Springer.Google Scholar
Wilson, D., & Sperber, D. (1992). On verbal irony. Lingua, 87(1–2), 5376.Google Scholar
Wilson, D., & Sperber, D. (2012). Explaining irony. In Wilson, D. & Sperber, D. (Eds.), Meaning and relevance (pp. 123145). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Winner, E. (1988). The point of words: Children’s understanding of metaphor and irony. Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Winner, E., & Gardner, H. (1993): Metaphor and irony: Two levels of understanding. Ortony, A. (Ed.), Metaphor and Thought (2nd ed., pp. 425443). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar

References

Amarasingam, A. (Ed.). (2011). The Stewart/Colbert effect: Essays on the real impacts of fake news. McFarland.Google Scholar
Athanasiadou, A., & Colston, H. L. (Eds.). (2017). Irony in language use and communication. John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Attardo, S. (2000). Irony as relevant inappropriateness. Journal of Pragmatics, 32(6), 793826. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0378-2166(99)00070-3Google Scholar
Averbeck, J. M., & Hample, D. (2008). Ironic message production: How and why we produce ironic messages. Communication Monographs, 75(4), 396410. https://doi.org/10.1080/03637750802512389Google Scholar
Baumgartner, J. C., Morris, J. S., & Walth, N. L. (2012). The Fey effect: Young adults, political humor, and perceptions of Sarah Palin in the 2008 presidential election campaign. Public Opinion Quarterly, 76(1), 95104. https://doi.org/10.1093/poq/nfr060Google Scholar
Baym, G. (2005). The Daily Show: Discursive integration and the reinvention of political journalism. Political Communication, 22(3), 259276. https://doi.org/10.1080/10584600591006492Google Scholar
Baym, G., & Jones, J. P. (2013). News parody and political satire across the globe. Routledge.Google Scholar
Becker, A. B. (2018). Live from New York, it’s Trump on Twitter! The effect of engaging with Saturday Night Live on perceptions of authenticity and the salience of trait ratings. International Journal of Communication, 12, 17361757.Google Scholar
Becker, A. B., & Bode, L. (2018). Satire as a source for learning? The differential impact of news versus satire exposure on net neutrality knowledge gain. Information, Communication & Society, 21(4), 612625. https://doi.org/10.1080/1369118X.2017.1301517Google Scholar
Becker, A. B., & Waisanen, D. J. (2013). From funny features to entertaining effects: Connecting approaches to communication research on political comedy. Review of Communication, 13(3), 161183. https://doi.org/10.1080/15358593.2013.826816Google Scholar
Boukes, M. (2019). Agenda-setting with satire: How political satire increased TTIP’s saliency on the public, media, and political agenda. Political Communication, 36(3), 426451. https://doi.org/10.1080/10584609.2018.1498816Google Scholar
Boukes, M., & Hameleers, M. (2020). Shattering populists’ rhetoric with satire at elections times: The effect of humorously holding populists accountable for their lack of solutions. Journal of Communication, 70(4), 574597. https://doi.org/10.1093/joc/jqaa020Google Scholar
Brewer, P. R., & Marquardt, E. (2007). Mock news and democracy: Analyzing The Daily Show. Atlantic Journal of Communication, 15(4), 249267. https://doi.org/10.1080/15456870701465315Google Scholar
Brugman, B. C., & Burgers, C. (2021). Sounds like a funny joke: Effects of vocal pitch and speech rate on satire liking. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology, 75(2), 221227. http://doi.org/10.1037/cep0000226Google Scholar
Brugman, B. C., Burgers, C., Beukeboom, C. J., & Konijn, E. A. (2022). Satirical news from left to right: Discursive integration in written online satire. Journalism, 23(8). https://doi.org/10.1177/1464884920979090Google Scholar
Buckley, C. (2020, 5 August). Are we living in a golden age of political satire? The Spectator. https://thespectator.com/topic/living-golden-age-political-satire-christopher-buckley/.Google Scholar
Burgers, C., Konijn, E. A., & Steen, G. J. (2016). Figurative framing: Shaping public discourse through metaphor, hyperbole, and irony. Communication Theory, 26(4), 410430. https://doi.org/10.1111/comt.12096Google Scholar
Burgers, C., & van Mulken, M. (2017). Humor markers. In Attardo, S. (Ed.), The Routledge handbook of language and humor (pp. 385399). Routledge. www.doi.org/10.4324/9781315731162-27Google Scholar
Che, M. (Writer), & King, D. P. (Director). (2020, April 25). Dr. Anthony Fauci Cold Open [Television series episode]. In L. Michaels (Executive producer), Saturday night live. New York: NBC. www.youtube.com/watch?v=uW56CL0pk0gGoogle Scholar
Chozik, A. (2015, 4 October). Hillary Clinton does “Saturday Night Live” with Doppelgänger. New York Times. www.nytimes.com/politics/first-draft/2015/10/04/hillary-clinton-does-saturday-night-live-with-doppelgnger/Google Scholar
Clark, H. H., & Gerrig, R. J. (1984). On the pretense theory of irony. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113(1), 121126. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.113.1.121Google Scholar
Colston, H. L. (1999). The pragmatic functions of rebuttal analogy. Metaphor and Symbol, 14(4), 259280. https://doi.org/10.1207/S15327868MS1404_2Google Scholar
Colston, H. L. (2000). On necessary conditions for verbal irony comprehension. Pragmatics & Cognition, 8(2), 277324. https://doi.org/10.1075/pc.8.2.02colGoogle Scholar
Colston, H. L. (2017). Irony performance and perception: What underlies verbal, situational and other ironies? In Athanasiadou, A. & Colston, H. L. (Eds.), Irony in language use and communication (pp. 1942). John Benjamins. https://doi.org/10.1075/ftl.1.02colGoogle Scholar
Cooper, S. (2020, 10 July). How to cognitive. www.youtube.com/watch?v=omTd0bvQT_MGoogle Scholar
De Speld (2020, 1 September). We vroegen Hugo de Jonge hoe hij de pandemie combineert met het vaderschap [We asked Hugo de Jonge how he combines the pandemic with fatherhood]. https://speld.nl/2020/09/01/we-vroegen-hugo-de-jonge-hoe-hij-de-pandemie-combineert-met-het-vaderschap-2/Google Scholar
Dews, S., & Winner, E. (1995). Muting the meaning: A social function of irony. Metaphor and Symbol, 10(1), 319. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327868ms1001_2Google Scholar
Droog, E., Burgers, C., & Steen, G. J. (2020). How satirists alternate between discursive modes: An introduction of the Humoristic Metaphors in Satirical News (HMSN) typology. International Journal of Communication, 14, 39643968.Google Scholar
Dynel, M. (2016). Comparing and combining covert and overt untruthfulness: On lying, deception, irony and metaphor. Pragmatics & Cognition, 23(1), 174208. https://doi.org/10.1075/pc.23.1.08dynGoogle Scholar
Egelhofer, J. L., & Lecheler, S. (2019). Fake news as a two-dimensional phenomenon: A framework and research agenda. Annals of the International Communication Association, 43(2), 97116. https://doi.org/10.1080/23808985.2019.1602782Google Scholar
Fonseca, P., Pascual, E., & Oakley, T. (2020). “Hi, Mr. President!”: Fictive interaction blends as a unifying rhetorical strategy in satire. Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 18(1), 180212. https://doi.org/10.1075/rcl.00056.fonGoogle Scholar
Fox, J. R., Koloen, G., & Sahin, V. (2007). No joke: A comparison of substance in The Daily Show with Jon Stewart and broadcast network television coverage of the 2004 presidential election campaign. Journal of Broadcasting & Electronic Media, 51(2), 213227. https://doi.org/10.1080/08838150701304621Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. W., Jr. (2012). Are ironic acts deliberate? Journal of Pragmatics, 44(1), 104115. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pragma.2011.11.001Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. W., Jr. (2021). “Holy cow, my irony detector just exploded!” Calling out irony during the coronavirus pandemic. Metaphor and Symbol, 36(1), 4560. https://doi.org/10.1080/10926488.2020.1855944Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. W., Jr., Bryant, G. A., & Colston, H. L. (2014). Where is the humor in verbal irony? Humor, 27(4), 575595. https://doi.org/10.1515/humor-2014-0106Google Scholar
Gilbert, C. J. (2019). Argumentum ad carricare as a mode of character attack: Alec Baldwin as Donald Trump on Saturday Night Live. In Samoilenko, S. A., Icks, M., Keohane, J., & Shiraev, E. (Eds.), Routledge handbook of character assassination and reputation management (pp. 209224). Routledge. https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315150178-16Google Scholar
Grice, H. P. (2002). Logic and conversation (1975). In Levitin, D. J. (Ed.), Foundations of cognitive psychology: Core readings (pp. 719732). MIT Press.Google Scholar
Grice, H. P. (2008). Further notes on logic and conversation (1978). In Adler, J. E. & Rips, L. J. (Eds.), Reasoning: Studies of human inference and its foundations (pp. 765773). Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Holbert, R. L. (2016). Entertainment television and political campaigns: The political satire appropriateness (PSA) model. In. Benoit, W. L. (ed.), Praeger handbook of political campaigning in the United States (pp. 171190). ABC-CLIO.Google Scholar
Holbert, R. L., Hmielowski, J., Jain, P., Lather, J., & Morey, A. (2011). Adding nuance to the study of political humor effects: Experimental research on Juvenalian satire versus Horatian satire. American Behavioral Scientist, 55(3), 187211. https://doi.org/10.1177/0002764210392156Google Scholar
Hurley, L. (2021, 11 January). U. S. Supreme Court shuns election-related disputes. Reuters.com. www.reuters.com/article/us-usa-court-election-idUSKBN29G1UFGoogle Scholar
Iwinski, G. (Writer), & Werner, C. (Director). (2020, 2 August). U.S. history [Television series episode]. In T. Carvell (Executive producer), Last Week Tonight with John Oliver. www.youtube.com/watch?v=hsxukOPEdggGoogle Scholar
Julius, R., & van den Berg, J. (2020, 25 September). Nederlandse rechter overleden, geen enkel gevolg voor verkiezingen [Dutch judge passed away, no consequences for elections whatsoever]. De Speld. https://speld.nl/2020/09/25/nederlandse-rechter-overleden-geen-enkel-gevolg-voor-verkiezingen/Google Scholar
Katz, A. N., & Pexman, P. M. (1997). Interpreting figurative statements: Speaker occupation can change metaphor to irony. Metaphor and Symbol, 12(1), 1941. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327868ms1201_3Google Scholar
Knobloch-Westerwick, S., & Lavis, S. M. (2017). Selecting serious or satirical, supporting or stirring news? Selective exposure to partisan versus mockery news online videos. Journal of Communication, 67(1), 5481. https://doi.org/10.1111/jcom.12271Google Scholar
Kreuz, R. J., & Roberts, R. M. (1993). On satire and parody: The importance of being ironic. Metaphor and Symbol, 8(2), 97109. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327868ms0802_2Google Scholar
LaMarre, H. L., Landreville, K. D., & Beam, M. A. (2009). The irony of satire: Political ideology and the motivation to see what you want to see in the Colbert Report. The International Journal of Press/Politics, 14(2), 212231. https://doi.org/10.1177/1940161208330904Google Scholar
Matthes, J., & Rauchfleisch, A. (2013). The Swiss “Tina Fey effect”: The content of late-night political humor and the negative effects of political parody on the evaluation of politicians. Communication Quarterly, 61(5), 596614. https://doi.org/10.1080/01463373.2013.822405Google Scholar
Maurer, J., Twiss, J., & Weiner, J. (Writers). (2017, 26 February). Obamacare replacement [Television series episode]. In J. Taylor (Executive producer), Last Week Tonight with John Oliver. www.youtube.com/watch?v=YEGpriv2TAcGoogle Scholar
McCarthy, T. (2020, 19 September). Ruth Bader Ginsburg: Trump given chance to replace liberal lion with young conservative. The Guardian. www.theguardian.com/law/2020/sep/19/trump-supreme-court-pick-replace-ruth-bader-ginsburgGoogle Scholar
Myers Roy, A. (1978). Irony in conversation. University Microfilms International.Google Scholar
Pearce, D. (2020, 18 April). Bank CEO reassures worried Canadians: “I’m going to be okay.” The Beaverton. www.thebeaverton.com/2020/04/bank-ceo-reassures-worried-canadians-im-going-to-be-okay/Google Scholar
Peifer, J. T., & Myrick, J. G. (2021). Risky satire: Examining how a traditional news outlet’s use of satire can affect audience perceptions and future engagement with the news source. Journalism, 22(7). https://doi.org/10.1177/1464884919833259Google Scholar
Poniewozik, J. (2020, 27 May). Trump said: “I have the best words.” Now they’re hers. The New York Times. www.nytimes.com/2020/05/27/arts/television/trump-sarah-cooper.htmlGoogle Scholar
Popa-Wyatt, M. (2014). Pretence and echo: Towards an integrated account of verbal irony. International Review of Pragmatics, 6(1), 127168. https://doi.org/10.1163/18773109-00601007Google Scholar
Ruiz de Mendoza Ibáñez, F. J., & Lozano-Palacio, I. (2019). Unraveling irony: From linguistics to literary criticism and back. Cognitive Semantics, 5(1), 147173. https://doi.org/10.1163/23526416-00501006Google Scholar
Shao, L., & Liu, D. (2019). The road to cynicism: The political consequences of online satire exposure in China. Political Studies, 67(2), 517536. https://doi.org/10.1177/0032321718791373Google Scholar
Skalicky, S., & Crossley, S. (2014). A statistical analysis of satirical Amazon.com product reviews. The European Journal of Humour Research, 2(3), 6685. http://doi.org/10.7592/EJHR2014.2.3.skalickiGoogle Scholar
Skalicky, S., & Crossley, S. A. (2019). Examining the online processing of satirical newspaper headlines. Discourse Processes, 56(1), 6176. https://doi.org/10.1080/0163853X.2017.1368332Google Scholar
Symons, A. (2019). Trump and satire: America’s carnivalesque President and his war on television comedians. In Happer, C., Hoskins, A., & Merrin, W. (Eds.), Trump’s media war (pp. 183197). Palgrave Macmillan. http://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-94069-4_12Google Scholar
Test, G. A. (1991). Satire: Spirit and art. University of South Florida Press.Google Scholar
The Daily Show with Trevor Noah (2020). The world’s fakest news team. www.comedycentral.co.uk/shows/wwfvz8/the-daily-show-with-trevor-noahGoogle Scholar
The Onion (2017, 26 October). Puerto Ricans without power for month can only assume this leading story across national news media. www.theonion.com/puerto-ricans-without-power-for-month-can-only-assume-t-1819877413Google Scholar
van Mulken, M., Burgers, C., & van der Plas, B. (2010). Wolves, confederates, and the happy few: The influence of comprehension, agreement, and group membership on the attitude toward irony. Discourse Processes, 48(1), 5068. https://doi.org/10.1080/01638531003692177Google Scholar
Waisanen, D. J. (2018). The rise of advocacy satire. In Baumgartner, J. C. & Becker, A. B. (Eds.), Political humor in a changing media landscape: A new generation of research (pp. 1127). Lexington Books.Google Scholar
Wilson, D. (2006). The pragmatics of verbal irony: Echo or pretence? Lingua, 116(10), 17221743. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.lingua.2006.05.001Google Scholar
Wolf, R. (2020, 27 October). New Associate Justice Amy Coney Barrett could have immediate impact on American democracy. USA Today. https://eu.usatoday.com/story/news/politics/2020/10/27/amy-coney-barrett-elections-health-care-abortion-await-new-justice/6041792002/Google Scholar
Young, D. G. (2019). Irony and outrage: The polarized landscape of rage, fear, and laughter in the United States. Oxford University PressGoogle Scholar
Young, D. G., Bagozzi, B. E., Goldring, A., Poulsen, S., & Drouin, E. (2019). Psychology, political ideology, and humor appreciation: Why is satire so liberal? Psychology of Popular Media Culture, 8(2), 134147. https://doi.org/10.1037/ppm0000157Google Scholar

References

Alicke, M., Gordon, E., & Rose, D. (2013). Hypocrisy: What counts? Philosophical Psychology, 26(5), 673701.Google Scholar
Alter, A., & Harris, E. A. (2021, March 4). Dr. Seuss books are pulled, and a “cancel culture” controversy erupts. New York Times. www.nytimes.com/2021/03/04/books/dr-seuss-books.htmlGoogle Scholar
Armecin, C. (2021, June 18). Meghan Markle, Prince Harry face backlash over deal with company selling skin-whitening cream. International Business Times. www.ibtimes.com/meghan-markle-prince-harry-face-backlash-over-deal-company-selling-skin-whitening-3201341Google Scholar
Aronson, E. (1999). Dissonance, hypocrisy, and the self-concept. In Harmon-Jones, E. & Mills, J., Cognitive dissonance: Progress on a pivotal theory in social psychology (pp. 103126). American Psychological Association.Google Scholar
Bartel, C. (2019). Hypocrisy as either deception or akrasia. The Philosophical Forum, 50(2), 269281.Google Scholar
Batson, C. D., & Collins, E. C. (2011). Moral hypocrisy: A self-enhancement/self-protective motive in the moral domain. In Alicke, M. D. & Sedikides, C. (Eds.), Handbook of self-enhancement and self-protection (pp. 92111). Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Batson, C. D., Kobrynowicz, D., Dinnerstein, J. L., Kampf, H. C., & Wilson, A. D. (1997). In a very different voice: Unmasking moral hypocrisy. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 72(6), 13351348.Google Scholar
Batson, C. D., Thompson, E. R., Seuferling, G., Whitney, H., & Strongman, J. A. (1999). Moral hypocrisy: Appearing moral to oneself without being so. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 77(3), 525537.Google Scholar
Bensinger, G. (2021, April 30). Stopping the manipulation machines. New York Times. www.nytimes.com/2021/04/30/opinion/dark-pattern-internet-ecommerce-regulation.htmlGoogle Scholar
Colbert, S. (2010). Season 6, episode 101. The Colbert Report. Comedy Central, New York.Google Scholar
Colston, H. L., & Gibbs, R. W. (2007). A brief history of irony. In Gibbs, R. W. & Colston, H. L. (Eds.), Irony in language and thought: A cognitive science reader (pp. 321). Psychology Press.Google Scholar
Cottle, M. (2021, May 4). Who cares about hypocrisy? New York Times. www.nytimes.com/2021/05/04/opinion/republicans-biden-hypocrisy.htmlGoogle Scholar
Coulson, S. (2005). Sarcasm and the space structuring model. In Coulson, S. & Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk, (Eds.), The literal and nonliteral in language and thought (pp. 129144). Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Crisp, R., & Cowton, C. (1994). Hypocrisy and moral seriousness. American Philosophical Quarterly, 31(4), 343349.Google Scholar
Dervish-O’Kane, R. (2021, May 17). You get abuse for showing skin & covering up: Women who run are in a catch-22. Yahoo Style UK. https://uk.style.yahoo.com/abuse-showing-skin-covering-women-095000443.htmlGoogle Scholar
Dickerson, C., Thibodeau, R., Aronson, E., & Miller, D. (1992). Using cognitive dissonance to encourage water conservation. Journal of Applied Psychology, 22(11), 841854.Google Scholar
Effron, D. A., & Miller, D. T. (2015). Do as I say, not as I’ve done: Suffering for a misdeed reduces the hypocrisy of advising others against it. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 131, 1632.Google Scholar
Eyal, N. (2014). Hooked: How to build habit-forming products. Penguin.Google Scholar
Eyal, N. (2021, May 14). The New York Times derides the very dark patterns it uses. OneZero. https://onezero.medium.com/new-york-times-writer-unwittingly-calls-out-manipulative-design-at-his-own-paper-99e50c660d59Google Scholar
Fauconnier, G. (1985). Mental spaces: Aspects of meaning construction in natural language. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Fauconnier, G. (1997). Mappings in thought and language. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Gibbs, R. W. (2021). “Holy cow, my irony detector just exploded!” Calling out irony during the coronavirus pandemic. Metaphor and Symbol, 36(1), 4560.Google Scholar
Graham, J., Meindl, P., Koleva, S., Iyer, R., & Johnson, K. M. (2015). When values and behavior conflict: Moral pluralism and intrapersonal moral hypocrisy. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 9(3), 158170.Google Scholar
Holt, J. (2001). Springfield hypocrisy. In Irwin, W., Conard, M. T., & Skoble, A. J. (Eds.), The Simpsons and philosophy (pp. 179190). Open Court.Google Scholar
Kihara, Y. (2005). The mental space structure of verbal irony. Cognitive Linguistics, 16(3), 513530.Google Scholar
Kittay, E. F. (1982). On hypocrisy. Metaphilosophy, 13(3&4), 277289.Google Scholar
Kreuz, R. (2020). Irony and sarcasm. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Kunda, Z., & Thagard, P. (1996). Forming impressions from stereotypes, traits, and behaviors: A parallel-constraint-satisfaction theory. Psychological Review, 103(2), 284308.Google Scholar
Laurent, S. M., & Clark, B. A. (2019). What makes hypocrisy? Folk definitions, attitude/behavior combinations, attitude strength, and private/public distinctions. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 41(2), 104121.Google Scholar
Lerner, M., & Miller, D. (1978). Just world research and the attribution process: Looking back and ahead. Psychological Bulletin, 85(5), 10301051.Google Scholar
Littman, D. C., & Mey, J. L. (1991). The nature of irony: Toward a computational model of irony. Journal of Pragmatics, 15(2), 131151.Google Scholar
Lucariello, J. (1994). Situational irony: A concept of events gone awry. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 123(2), 129145.Google Scholar
McKinnon, C. (1991). Hypocrisy, with a note on integrity. American Philosophical Quarterly, 28(4), 321330.Google Scholar
Muecke, D. C. (1982). Irony and the ironic: The critical idiom (2nd ed.). Methuen.Google Scholar
Newman, J. (1986). Fanatics and hypocrites. Prometheus Press.Google Scholar
Powell, C. A., & Smith, R. H. (2013). Schadenfreude caused by exposure of hypocrisy in others. Self and Identity, 12(4), 413431.Google Scholar
Prinz, J. (2007). The emotional construction of morals. Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Richardson, J. H. (2010, August 10). Newt Gingrich: The indispensible Republican. Esquire. www.esquire.com/news-politics/a8131/newt-gingrich-0910/Google Scholar
Ryle, G. (1949). The concept of mind. Hutchinson.Google Scholar
Shelley, C. (2001). The bicoherence theory of situational irony. Cognitive Science, 25(5), 775818.Google Scholar
Shelley, C. (2006). Hypocrisy as irony: Toward a cognitive model of hypocrisy. Metaphor and Symbol, 21(3), 169190.Google Scholar
Shklar, J. N. (1984). Ordinary vices. Belknap.Google Scholar
Simonite, T. (2021, January 29). Lawmakers take aim at insidious digital “dark patterns.” Wired. www.wired.com/story/lawmakers-take-aim-insidious-digital-dark-patterns/Google Scholar
Sinay, D. (2021, June 17). Meghan Markle losing the title “Duchess” would automatically get her a Princess title instead. Microsoft News. www.msn.com/en-us/lifestyle/lifestyle-buzz/meghan-markle-losing-the-name-duchess-would-automatically-get-her-a-princess-title-instead/ar-BB1gPABwGoogle Scholar
Singh, A. (2021, May 5). Meghan Markle writes children’s book about father-son relationship; royal experts stress irony of subject. International Business Times. www.ibtimes.co.uk/meghan-markle-writes-childrens-book-about-father-son-relationship-royal-experts-stress-irony-1690025Google Scholar
Stone, J., & Fernandez, N. C. (2008). To practice what we preach: The use of hypocrisy and cognitive dissonance to motivate behavior change. Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2(2), 10241051.Google Scholar
Szabados, B. (1979). Hypocrisy. Canadian Journal of Philosophy, 9(2), 195210.Google Scholar
Szabados, B., & Soifer, E. (1999). Hypocrisy, change of mind, and weakness of will: How to do moral philosophy with examples. Metaphilosophy, 30(1&2), 6078.Google Scholar
Taylor, S. E., & Fiske, S. T. (1978). Salience, attention, and attribution: Top of the head phenomena. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 11, 249288.Google Scholar
Thagard, P. (2000). Coherence in thought and action. MIT Press.Google Scholar
Turner, D. (1990). Hypocrisy. Metaphilosophy, 21(3), 262269.Google Scholar
Warach, B., Josephs, L., & Gorman, B. S. (2019). Are cheaters sexual hypocrites? Sexual hypocrisy, the self-serving bias, and personality style. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 45(10), 14991511.Google Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×