Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gbm5v Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-28T21:01:46.708Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

17 - Latent State–Trait Analyses for Process Models of Implicit Measures

from Section IV - Improving Measurement and Theorizing About Implicit Bias

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  21 December 2024

Jon A. Krosnick
Affiliation:
Stanford University, California
Tobias H. Stark
Affiliation:
Utrecht University, The Netherlands
Amanda L. Scott
Affiliation:
The Strategy Team, Columbus, Ohio
Get access

Summary

A recent debate on implicit measures of racial attitudes has focused on the relative roles of the person, the situation, and their interaction in determining the measurement outcomes. The chapter describes process models for assessing the roles of the situation and the person-situation interaction on the one hand and stable person-related components on the other hand in implicit measures. Latent state-trait models allow one to assess to what extent the measure is a reliable measure of the person and/or the situation and the person-situation interaction (Steyer, Geiser, & Fiege, 2012). Moreover, trait factor scores as well as situation-specific residual factor scores can be computed and related to third variables, thereby allowing one to assess to what extent the implicit measure is a valid measure of the person and/or the situation and the person-situation interaction. These methods are particularly helpful when combined with a process decomposition of implicit-measure data such as a diffusion-model analysis of the IAT (Klauer, Voss, Schmitz, & Teige-Mocigemba, 2007).

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2025

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Arbuckle, J. L. (2017). Amos (Version 25.0) [Computer Program]. Chicago, IL: IBM SPSS.Google Scholar
Asendorpf, J. B. (2020). Personality as a situation: A target-centered perspective on social situations. In J. F. Rauthmann, R. A. Sherman, & Funder, D. C. (Eds.), The Oxford Handbook of Psychological Situations (pp. 313). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Carpenter, B., Gelman, A., Hoffman, M., et al. (2017). Stan: A probabilistic programming language. Journal of Statistical Software, Articles, 76, 132. https://doi.org/10.18637/jss.v076.i01Google Scholar
Conrey, F. R., Sherman, J. W., Gawronski, B., et al. (2005). Separating multiple processes in implicit social cognition: The Quad-model of implicit task performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 469487. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.89.4.469CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Dentale, F., Vecchione, M., Ghezzi, V., et al. (2016). Applying the Latent State-Trait analysis to decompose state, trait, and error components of the self-esteem Implicit Association Test. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 35, 78–85. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759/a000378Google Scholar
Donnellan, M. B., Lucas, R. E., & Fleeson, W. (2009). Introduction to personality and assessment at age 40: Reflections on the legacy of the person-situation debate and the future of person-situation integration. Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 117119. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jrp.2009.02.010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dutilh, G., Annis, J., Brown, S. D., et al. (2019). The quality of response time data inference: A blinded, collaborative assessment of the validity of cognitive models. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26, 10511069. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-017-1417-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fazio, R. H., Jackson, J. R., Dunton, B. C., et al. (1995). Variability in automatic activation as an unobtrusive measure of racial attitudes: A bona fide pipeline? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 10131027. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.69.6.1013CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gawronski, B., & Bodenhausen, G. V. (2017). Beyond persons and situations: An interactionist approach to understanding implicit bias. Psychological Inquiry, 28(4), 268272. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2017.1373546CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gawronski, B., Morrison, M., Phills, C. E., et al. (2017). Temporal stability of implicit and explicit measures: A longitudinal analysis. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 43, 300312.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gawronski, B., Morrison, M., Phills, C. E., et al. (2019). Corrigendum. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 45, 15281528. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167219832334Google Scholar
Geiser, C., Litson, K., Bishop, J., et al. (2015). Analyzing person, situation and person × situation interaction effects: Latent state-trait models for the combination of random and fixed situations. Psychological Methods, 20, 165192. https://doi.org/10.1037/met0000026CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., McGhee, D. E., & Schwartz, J. L. K. (1998). Measuring individual differences in implicit cognition: The implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 74, 14641480. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.74.6.1464CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. (2003). Understanding and using the implicit association test: I. An improved scoring algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 197216. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.197CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hofmann, W., Gawronski, B., Geschwendner, T., et al. (2005). A meta-analysis on the correlation between the implicit association test and explicit self-report measures. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 13691385. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167205275613CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ito, T. A., Friedman, N. P., Bartholow, B. D., et al. (2015). Toward a comprehensive understanding of executive cognitive function in implicit racial bias. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 108, 187218. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0038557CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kaplan, D. (2000). Structural Equation Modeling: Foundations and Extensions. Thousand Oaks, CA: SAGE Publications.Google Scholar
Klauer, K. C. (2015). Model testing and selection, theory of. In Wright, J. D. (Ed.), International Encyclopedia of Social and Behavioral Sciences (2nd ed., Vol. 15, pp. 653658). Oxford: Elsevier.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Klauer, K. C., Schmitz, F., Teige-Mocigemba, S., et al. (2010). Understanding the role of executive control in the implicit association test: Why flexible people have small IAT effects. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 63, 595619. https://doi.org/10.1080/17470210903076826CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klauer, K. C., Stahl, C., & Voss, A. (2012). Multinomial models and diffusion models. In Klauer, K. C., Voss, A., & Stahl, C. (Eds.), Cognitive Methods in Social Psychology. Abridged edition (pp. 331354). New York, NY: Guilford Press.Google Scholar
Klauer, K. C., Voss, A., Schmitz, F., et al. (2007). Process components of the Implicit Association Test: A diffusion-model analysis. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 353368. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.93.3.353CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lane, K. A., Banaji, M. R., Nosek, B. A., et al. (2007). Understanding and using the implicit association test: What we know (so far) about the method. In Wittenbrink, B. & Schwarz, N. (Eds.), Implicit Measures of Attitudes. New York, NY: Guilford Press, pp. 59102.Google Scholar
Lemmer, G., Gollwitzer, M., Schiller, E.-M., et al. (2015). On the psychometric properties of the aggressiveness-IAT for children and adolescents. Aggressive Behavior, 41, 8495. https://doi.org/10.1002/ab.21575CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lord, F., Novick, M., & Birnbaum, A. (1968). Statistical Theories of Mental Test Scores. Oxford: Addison-Wesley.Google Scholar
Machery, E. (2017). Do indirect measures of biases measure traits or situations? Psychological Inquiry, 28, 288291. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2017.1373557CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Mierke, J., & Klauer, K. C. (2003). Method-specific variance in the implicit association test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 85, 1180(pp. 59102)1192. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.85.6.1180CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Payne, B. K., Burkley, M. A., & Stokes, M. B. (2008). Why do implicit and explicit attitude tests diverge? The role of structural fit. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 1631. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.94.1.16CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Payne, B. K., Cheng, C. M., Govorun, O., et al. (2005). An inkblot for attitudes: Affect misattribution as implicit measurement. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 277293. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.89.3.277CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Payne, B. K., Vuletich, H. A., & Lundberg, K. B. (2017). The bias of crowds: How implicit bias bridges personal and systemic prejudice. Psychological Inquiry, 28, 233248. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2017.1335568CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rae, J. R., & Greenwald, A. G. (2017). Persons or situations? Individual differences explain variance in aggregated implicit race attitudes. Psychological Inquiry, 28, 297300. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2017.1373548CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ratcliff, R. (1978). A theory of memory retrieval. Psychological Review, 85, 59108. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.85.2.59CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Samayoa, J. A. G., & Fazio, R. H. (2017). Who starts the wave? Let’s not forget the role of the individual. Psychological Inquiry, 28, 273277. https://doi.org/10.1080/1047840X.2017.1373554CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schermelleh-Engel, K., Moosbrugger, H., & Müller, H. (2003). Evaluating the fit of structural equation models: Tests of significance and descriptive goodness-of-fit measures. MPR-online, 8, 2374.Google Scholar
Schmukle, S. C., & Egloff, B. (2005). A Latent State-Trait analysis of implicit and explicit personality measures. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 21, 100107. https://doi.org/10.1027/1015-5759.21.2.100CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Steyer, R., Geiser, C., & Fiege, C. (2012). Latent state-trait models. In Cooper, H., Camic, P. M., Long, D. L., Panter, A. T., Rindskopf, D., & Sher, K. J. (Eds.), APA Handbook of Research Methods in Psychology, Vol 3: Data Analysis and Research Publication. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, pp. 291308. https://doi.org/10.1037/13621-014Google Scholar
Wilson, T. D., Lindsey, S., & Schooler, T. Y. (2000). A model of dual attitudes. Psychological Review, 107, 101126. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.107.1.101CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×