Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-q99xh Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T15:41:42.914Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

Part IV - Gestures in Relation to Cognition

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 May 2024

Alan Cienki
Affiliation:
Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam
Get access

Summary

Image of the first page of this content. For PDF version, please use the ‘Save PDF’ preceeding this image.'
Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2024

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

References

Blumenthal, A. (Ed. and Trans.). (1970). Language and psychology: Historical aspects of psycholinguistics. New York, NY: John Wiley and Sons Ltd.Google Scholar
Bosker, H. R., & Peeters, D. (2021). Beat gestures influence which speech sounds you hear. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 288(1943), Article 20202419. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2020.2419Google ScholarPubMed
Bühler, K. (1982). The deictic field of language and deictic words, in Jarvella, R. J. & Klein, W. (Eds.), Speech, place, and action (pp. 930). Chichester, UK: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.Google Scholar
Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the theory of syntax. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Efron, D. (1941). Gesture and environment. Morningside Heights, NJ: King’s Crown Press.Google Scholar
Ekman, P., & Friesen, W. (1969). The repertoire of non-verbal behavior: Categories, origins, usage, and coding. Semiotica, 1, 4998. https://doi.org/10.1515/semi.1969.1.1.49CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Firbas, J. (1971). On the concept of communicative dynamism in the theory of functional sentence perspective. Philologica Pragensia, 8, 135144.Google Scholar
Furuyama, N., & Sekine, K. (2007). Forgetful or strategic? The mystery of the systematic avoidance of reference in the cartoon story narrative, In Duncan, S. D., Cassell, J., & Levy, E. T. (Eds.), Gesture and the dynamic dimension of language, pp. 7581. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glick, J. (1983). Piaget, Vygotsky, and Werner. In Wapner, S. & Kaplan, B. (Eds.), Toward a holistic developmental psychology (pp. 3552). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Goldberg, A. (1995). Constructions: A construction approach to argument structure. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Goodwin, C., & Duranti, A. (1992). Rethinking context: An introduction. In Duranti, A. & Goodwin, C. (Eds.), Rethinking context: Language as an interactive phenomenon (pp. 142). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Gullberg, M. (2013). So you think gestures are compensatory? Reflections based on child and adult learner data. In Mattsson, A. F. & Norrby, C. (Eds.), Language acquisition and use in multilingual contexts: Theory and practice (pp. 39–49). Lund, Sweden: Travaux de l’Institut de linguistique de Lund 52.Google Scholar
Humboldt, W. von. (1999). On language. Heath, P. (Trans.), Losonsky, M (Ed.). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Huth, A., de Heer, G., Griffiths, W. A., Theunissen, T. L., F. E., & Gallant, J. L. (2016). Natural speech reveals the semantic maps that tile human cerebral cortex. Nature, 532, 453458. https://doi.org/10.1038/nature17637CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iverson, J. M. & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1997). What’s communication got to do with it? Gesture in congenitally blind children. Developmental Psychology, 33, 453467.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jakobson, R. 1960. Concluding statement: Linguistics and poetics. In Sebeok, T. (Ed.), Style in language (pp. 350377). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
James, W. (1890). Psychology (Vol. 1, Ch. IX). New York, NY: Holt.Google Scholar
Jouravlev, O., Zheng, D., Balewski, Z., Pongos, A. L., Le, A., Levan, Z., … & Fedorenko, E. (2019). Speech-accompanying gestures are not processed by the language processing mechanisms. Neuropsychologia, 132, 107132. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2019.107132CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendon, A. (1972). Some relationships between body motion and speech: An analysis of an example. In Siegman, A. & Pope, B. (Eds.), Studies in dyadic communication (pp. 177210). Elmsford, NY: Pergamon.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendon, A. (1975). Gesticulation, speech, and the gesture theory of language origin. Sign Language Studies, 9(2), 349373. https://doi.org/10.1353/sls.1975.0016CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendon, A. (1980). Gesticulation and speech: Two aspects of the process of utterance. In Ritchie Key, M. (Ed.), The relationship of verbal and nonverbal communication (pp. 207227). The Hague, the Netherlands: Mouton and Co.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendon, A. (2008). Some reflections of the relationship between “gesture” and “sign”. Gesture, 8 (3), 348366. https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.8.3.05kenCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krauss, R., Chen, Y., & Gottesman, R. F. (2000). Lexical gestures and lexical access: A process model. In McNeill, D. (Ed.), Language and gesture (pp. 261283). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levelt, W. J. M. (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press/Bradford Books.Google Scholar
Levy, E. T., & McNeill, D. (1992). Speech, gesture, and discourse. Discourse Processes, 15(3), 277301. https://doi.org/10.1080/01638539209544813CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Levy, E. T., & McNeill, D. (2015). The narrative development of young children: Explorations in the embodiment of cohesion. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. (2005). Gesture and thought. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D. (2012). How language began. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D. (2015). Speech-gesture mimicry in performance: An actor → audience, author → actor, audience → actor triangle. Journal for Cultural Research, 19(1), 1529. https://doi.org/10.1080/14797585.2014.920184CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D. (2016). Why we gesture. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
McNeill., D. (2018). Recurrent gestures: How the mental reflects the social. Gesture, 17(2), 229244. https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.18012.mcnCrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D., & Duncan, S. D. (2000). Growth points in thinking for speaking. In McNeill, D. (Ed.), Language and gesture (pp. 141161). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D., & Levy, E. T. (1993). Cohesion and gesture. Discourse Processes, 16(4), 363386. https://doi.org/10.1080/01638539309544845CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Magnusson, M. S. (2000). Discovering hidden time patterns in behavior: T-patterns and their detection. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 32, 93110. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03200792CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Marslen-Wilson, W. D. (1987). Functional parallelism in spoken word-recognition. Cognition, 25(1), 71102. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(87)90005-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mayberry, R., & Jaques, J. (2000). Gesture production during stuttered speech: Insights into the nature of gesture-speech integration. In McNeill, D. (Ed.), Language and gesture (pp. 199214). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Merleau-Ponty, M. (1962). Phenomenology of perception (C. Smith, Trans.). London, UK: Routledge.Google Scholar
Müller, C. (2017). How recurrent gestures mean: Conventionalized contexts-of-use and embodied motivation. Gesture, 16 (2), 276303. https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.16.2.05mulCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nobe, S. (1996). Representational gestures, cognitive rhythms, and acoustic aspects of speech: A network/threshold model of gesture production. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation.) University of Chicago, Chicago, IL.Google Scholar
Parrill, F. (2008). Subjects in the hands of speakers: An experimental study of syntactic subject and speech-gesture integration. Cognitive Linguistics, 19(2), 283299. https://doi.org/10.1515/COG.2008.011CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Parrill, F. (2009). Dual viewpoint gestures. Gesture, 9(3), 271289. https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.9.3.01parCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ramachandran, V. & Blakeslee, S. (1998). Phantoms in the brain: Probing the mysteries of the human mind. New York, NY: William Morrow.Google Scholar
Saussure, F. de. (1959). Course in general linguistics (Bally, C. & Sechehaye, A., Eds., W. Baskin, Trans.). New York, NY: The Philosophical Library.Google Scholar
Schegloff, E. A. (1984). On some gestures’ relation to talk. In Atkinson, I. M. & Heritage, I. (Eds.), Structures of social action, pp. 266298. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Sekine, K., Schoechl., C., Mulder, K., Holler, J., Kelly, S., … & Özyürek, A. (2020). Evidence for children’s online integration of simultaneous information from speech and iconic gestures: An ERP study. Language, Cognition and Neuroscience, 35(10), 12831294. https://doi.org/10.1080/23273798.2020.1737719CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Simon, H. A. (1996). The sciences of the artificial (3rd ed.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Slobin, D. I. (1987). Thinking for speaking. In Aske, J., Beery, N., Michaelis, L., & Filip, H. (Eds.), Berkeley Linguistics Society, proceedings of the 13th annual meeting (pp. 435445). Berkeley, CA: Berkeley Linguistics Society.Google Scholar
Talmy, L. (2000). Toward a cognitive semantics. Vol. 2: Typology and process in concept structuring. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1986). Thought and language (edited and translated by Hanfmann, E. and Vakar, G., revised and edited by A. Kozulin). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.Google Scholar
Vygotsky, L. S. (1987). The collected works of L. S. Vygotsky. Volume 1: Problems of general psychology. Including the volume “Thinking and speech” (edited by Rieber, R. W. and Carton, A. S., intro. and trans. by N. Minick). New York, NY: Plenum.Google Scholar
Werner, H., & Kaplan, B. (1963). Symbol formation. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons Ltd.Google Scholar
Winter, B., & Duffy, S. E. (2020). Can co-speech gestures alone carry the mental time line? Journal of Experimental Psychology, Learning, Memory, and Cognition 46(9),17681781. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000836CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed

References

Alibali, M. W., Church, R. B., Kita, S., & Hostetter, A. B. (2014). Embodied knowledge in the development of conservation of quantity: Evidence from gesture. In Edwards, L., Ferrara, F., & Moore-Russo, D. (Eds.), Emerging perspectives on gesture and embodiment in mathematics (pp. 2749). Charlotte, NC: Information Age Publishing.Google Scholar
Alibali, M. W., & DiRusso, A. A. (1999). The function of gesture in learning to count: More than keeping track. Cognitive Development, 14 (1), 3756. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0885-2014(99)80017-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alibali, M. W., & Hostetter, A. B. (2010). Mimicry and simulation in gesture comprehension. (Commentary on P. Niedenthal, M. Maringer, M. Mermillod, & U. Hess, The Simulation of Smiles (SIMS) model: Embodied simulation and the meaning of facial expression). Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 33, 433434. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0140525x10000865CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alibali, M. W., & Kita, S. (2010). Gesture highlights perceptually present information for speakers. Gesture, 10 (1), 328. https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.10.1.02aliCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alibali, M. W., Kita, S., & Young, A. (2000). Gesture and the process of speech production: We think, therefore we gesture. Language & Cognitive Processes, 15, 593613. https://doi.org/10.1080/016909600750040571CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alibali, M. W., McNeill, N. M., & Perrott, M. A. (1998). What makes children change their minds? Changes in encoding lead to changes in strategy selection. In Gernsbacher, M. A. & Derry, S. (Eds.), Proceedings of the Twentieth Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society (pp. 3641). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Alibali, M. W., Spencer, R. C., Knox, L., & Kita, S. (2011). Spontaneous gestures influence strategy choices in problem solving. Psychological Science, 2 (9), 11381144. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611417722CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alibali, M. W., Yeo, A., Hostetter, A. B., & Kita, S. (2017). Representational gestures help speakers package information for speaking. In Church, R. B., Alibali, M. W., & Kelly, S. D. (Eds.), Why gesture? How the hands function in speaking, thinking, and communicating (pp. 1537). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barsalou, L. W., Simmons, W. K., Barbey, A. K., & Wilson, C. D. (2003). Grounding conceptual knowledge in modality-specific systems. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7 (2), 8491. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1364-6613(02)00029-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bavelas, J. B., Chovil, N., Lawrie, D. A., & Wade, A. (1992). Interactive gestures. Discourse Processes, 15, 469489. https://doi.org/10.1080/01638539209544823CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bavelas, J., & Healing, S. (2013). Reconciling the effects of mutual visibility on gesturing: A review. Gesture, 13 (1), 6392. https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.13.1.03bavCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Beattie, G., & Coughlan, J. (1999). An experimental investigation of the role of iconic gestures in lexical access using the tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon. British Journal of Psychology, 90, 3556. https://doi.org/10.1348/000712699161251CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Beilock, S., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2010). Gesturing changes thought by grounding it in action. Psychological Science, 21 (11), 16051610. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797610385353CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bone, M. B., St-Laurent, M., Dant, C., McQuiggan, D. A., Ryan, J. D., & Buchsbaum, B. R. (2019). Eye movement reinstatement and neural reactivation during mental imagery. Cerebral Cortex, 29 (3), 10751089. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhy014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brookes, H. (2014). Gesture in the communicative ecology of a South African township. In Seyfeddinipur, M. & Gullberg, M. (Eds.), From gesture in conversation to visible action as utterance: Essays in honor of Adam Kendon (pp. 5974). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: John Benjamins.Google Scholar
Brooks, N., Barner, D., Frank, M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2017). The role of gesture in supporting mental representations: The case of mental abacus arithmetic. Cognitive Science, 42 (2), 554575. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12527CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Casasanto, D. (2013). Different bodies, different minds: The body-specificity of language and thought. In Caballero, R. & Díaz Vera, J. (Eds.), Sensuous cognition – Explorations into human sentience (pp. 917). Berlin, Germany: de Gruyter Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cho, P. S., & So, W. C. (2018). A feel for numbers: The changing role of gesture in manipulating the mental representation of an abacus among children at different skill levels. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, Article 1267. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01267CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chu, M., & Kita, S. (2008). Spontaneous gestures during mental rotation tasks: Insights into the microdevelopment of the motor strategy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 137 (4), 706723. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0013157CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chu, M., & Kita, S. (2011). The nature of gestures’ beneficial role in spatial problem solving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140 (1), 102116. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0021790CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Chu, M., & Kita, S. (2016). Co-thought and co-speech gestures are generated by the same action generation process. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, & Cognition, 42 (2), 257270. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000168Google ScholarPubMed
Cienki, A. (2013). Image schemas and mimetic schemas in cognitive linguistics and gesture studies. Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 11(2), 417432. https://doi.org/10.1075/rcl.11.2.13cieCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cook, S. W., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2006). The role of gesture in learning: Do children use their hands to change their minds? Journal of Cognition and Development, 7, 211232. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327647jcd0702_4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cook, S. W., & Fenn, K. M. (2017). The function of gesture in learning and memory. In Church, R. B., Alibali, M. W., & Kelly, S. D. (Eds.), Why gesture? How the hands function in speaking, thinking and communicating (pp. 129153). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cook, S. W., & Tanenhaus, M. K. (2009). Embodied communication: Speakers’ gestures affect listeners’ actions. Cognition, 113 (1), 98104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2009.06.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cook, S. W., Yip, T. K., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2010). Gesture makes memories that last. Journal of Memory and Language, 63 (4), 465475. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2010.07.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cook, S. W., Yip, T. K., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2012). Gestures, but not meaningless movements, lighten working memory load when explaining math. Language and Cognitive Processes, 27, 594610. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690965.2011.567074CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cowan, N. (2010). The magical mystery four: How is working memory capacity limited, and why? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 19 (1), 5157. https://doi.org/10.1177/0963721409359277CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dargue, N., Sweller, N., & Jones, M. P. (2019). When our hands help us understand: A meta-analysis into the effects of gesture on comprehension. Psychological Bulletin, 145 (8), 765784. https://doi.org/10.1037/bul0000202CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Droll, J. A., & Hayhoe, M. M. (2007). Trade-offs between gaze and working memory use. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 33 (6), 13521365. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.33.6.1352Google ScholarPubMed
Eielts, C., Pouw, W., Ouwehand, K., van Gog, T., Zwaan, R. A., & Paas, F. (2020). Co-thought gesturing supports more complex problem solving in subjects with lower visual working-memory capacity. Psychological Research, 84, 502513. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1065-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Enfield, N. J. (2005). The body as a cognitive artifact in kinship representations. Hand gesture diagrams by speakers of Lao. Current Anthropology, 46 (1), 5181. https://doi.org/10.1086/425661CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Engelkamp, J., & Zimmer, H. D. (1994). Motor similarity in subject-performed tasks. Psychological Research-Psychologische Forschung, 57, 4753. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00452995CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Eskritt, M., & Ma, S. (2014). Intentional forgetting: Note-taking as a naturalistic example. Memory & Cognition, 42, 237246. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13421-013-0362-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flood, V. J. (2018). Multimodal revoicing as an interactional mechanism for connecting scientific and everyday concepts. Human Development, 61 (3), 145173. https://doi.org/10.1159/000488693CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frick-Horbury, D., & Guttentag, R. E. (1998). The effects of restricting hand gesture production on lexical retrieval and free recall. The American Journal of Psychology, 111 (1), 4362. https://doi.org/10.2307/1423536CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Glenberg, A. M. (1997). What memory is for. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 20, 155. http://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X97000010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Glenberg, A. M., Witt, J. K., & Metcalfe, J. (2013). From the revolution to embodiment: 25 years of Cognitive Psychology. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 8 (5), 573585. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691613498098CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Göksun, T., Goldin-Meadow, S., Newcombe, N., & Shipley, T. F. (2013). Individual differences in mental rotation: What does gesture tell us? Cognitive Processing, 14, 153162. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10339-013-0549-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldin-Meadow, S., & Beilock, S. L. (2010). Action’s influence on thought: The case of gesture. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 5 (6), 664674. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691610388764CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldin-Meadow, S., Nusbaum, H., Kelly, S. D., & Wagner, S. (2001). Explaining math: Gesturing lightens the load. Psychological Science, 12, 516522. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-9280.00395CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodwin, C. (2000). Action and embodiment in human interaction. Journal of Pragmatics, 32 (10), 14891522. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0378-2166(99)00096-xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodwin, M. H., & Goodwin, C. (1986). Gesture and coparticipation in the activity of searching for a word. Semiotica, 62 (1–2), 5175. https://doi.org/10.1515/semi.1986.62.1-2.51Google Scholar
Graham, T. A. (1999). The role of gesture in children’s learning to count. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 74 (4), 333355. https://doi.org/10.1006/jecp.1999.2520CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Guillot, A., Collet, C., Nguyen, V. A., Malouin, F., Richards, C., & Doyon, J. (2009). Brain activity during visual versus kinesthetic imagery: An fMRI study. Human Brain Mapping, 30 (7), 21572172. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20658CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haviland, J. B. (1993). Anchoring, iconicity and orientation in Guugu Yimithirr pointing gestures. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology, 3 (1), 345. https://doi.org/10.1525/jlin.1993.3.1.3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Holler, J., & Wilkin, K. (2011). Co-speech gesture mimicry in the process of collaborative referring during face-to-face dialogue. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 35, 133153. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-011-0105-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hostetter, A. B. (2011). When do gestures communicate? A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 137 (2), 297315. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022128CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hostetter, A. B., & Alibali, M. W. (2008). Visible embodiment: Gestures as simulated action. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 495514. https://doi.org/10.3758/pbr.15.3.495CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hostetter, A. B., & Alibali, M. W. (2010). Language, gesture, action! A test of the Gesture as Simulated Action framework. Journal of Memory and Language, 63 (2), 245257. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2010.04.003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hostetter, A. B., & Alibali, M. W. (2019). Gesture as simulated action? Revisiting the framework. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 26, 721752. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-018-1548-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hostetter, A. B., Alibali, M. W., & Kita, S. (2007). I see it in my hands’ eye: Representational gestures reflect conceptual demands. Language & Cognitive Processes, 22, 313336. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960600632812CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Iani, F., Burin, D., Salatino, A., Pia, L., Ricci, R., & Bucciarelli, M. (2018). The beneficial effect of a speaker’s gestures on the listener’s memory for action phrases: The pivotal role of the listener’s premotor cortex. Brain & Language, 180–182, 813. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2018.03.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Johnson, M. (1987). The body in the mind: The bodily basis of meaning, imagination, and reason. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jordan, N., Kaplan, D., Ramineni, C., & Locuniak, M. (2008). Development of number combination skill in the early school years: When do fingers help? Developmental Science, 11 (5), 662668. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00715.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kamermans, K. L., Pouw, W., Fassi, L., Aslanidou, A., Paas, F., & Hostetter, A. B. (2019). The role of gesture as simulated action in reinterpretation of mental imagery. Acta Psychologica, 197, 131142. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.actpsy.2019.05.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelly, S. D., Byrne, K., & Holler, J. (2011). Raising the ante of communication: Evidence for enhanced gesture in high stakes situations. Information, 2 (4), 579593. https://doi.org/10.3390/info2040579CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kendon, A. (2004). Gesture: Visible action as utterance. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kimbara, I. (2006). On gestural mimicry. Gesture, 6 (1), 3961. http://doi.org/10.1075/gest.6.1.03kimCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kimbara, I. (2008). Gesture form convergence in joint description. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 32, 123131. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-007-0044-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kirk, E., & Lewis, C. (2017). Gesture facilitates children’s creative thinking. Psychological Science, 28 (2), 225232. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797616679183CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kita, S. (2000). How representational gestures help speaking. In McNeill, D. (Ed.), Language and gesture (pp. 162185). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kita, S., Alibali, M. W., & Chu, M. (2017). How do gestures influence thinking and speaking? The gesture for conceptualization hypothesis. Psychological Review, 124 (3), 245266. https://doi.org/10.1037/rev0000059CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kita, S., & Özyürek, A. (2003). What does cross-linguistic variation in semantic coordination of speech and gesture reveal? Evidence for an interface representation of spatial thinking and speaking. Journal of Memory and Language, 48 (1), 1632. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0749-596x(02)00505-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krauss, R. M. (1998). Why do we gesture when we speak? Current Directions in Psychological Science, 7 (2), 5460. https://doi.org/10.1111/1467-8721.ep13175642CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krauss, R. M., Chen, Y., & Gottesman, R. (2000). Lexical gestures and lexical access: A process model. In McNeill, D. (Ed.), Language and gesture (pp. 261283). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ladewig, S. H. (2014). Recurrent gestures. In Müller, C., Cienki, A., Fricke, E., Ladewig, S., McNeill, D., & Bressem, J. (Eds.), Body - language - communication: An international handbook on multimodality in human interaction (Vol. 2, pp. 15581575). Berlin, Germany: De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Lavergne, J., & Kimura, D. (1987). Hand movement asymmetry during speech: No effect of speaking topic. Neuropsychologia, 25 (4), 689693. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(87)90060-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Levinson, S. C. (1997). Language and cognition: The cognitive consequences of spatial description in Guugu Yimithirr. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology, 7 (1), 98131. https://doi.org/10.1525/jlin.1997.7.1.98CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Liddell, S. K., & Metzger, M. (1998). Gesture in sign language discourse. Journal of Pragmatics, 30 (6), 657697. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0378-2166(98)00061-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McClave, E. (1994). Gestural beats: The rhythm hypothesis. Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 23, 4566. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf02143175CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. (2005). Gesture and thought. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Marghetis, T., & Bergen, B. K. (2014). Embodied meaning, inside and out: The coupling of gesture and mental simulation. In Müller, C., Cienki, A., Fricke, E., Ladewig, S., McNeill, D., & Bressem, J. (Eds.), Body - language - communication: An international handbook on multimodality in human interaction (Vol. 2, pp. 20002007). Berlin, Germany: De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Masson-Carro, I., Goudbeek, M., & Krahmer, E. (2017). How what we see and what we know influence iconic gesture production. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 41, 367394. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-017-0261-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Matsumoto, D., & Hwang, H. C. (2013). Cultural similarities and differences in emblematic gestures. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 37, 127. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-012-0143-8CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, D., Collett, P., Marsh, P., & O’Shaughnessy, M. (1979). Gestures: Their origin and distribution. New York, NY: Stein & Day.Google Scholar
Moulton, S. T., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2009). Imagining predictions: Mental imagery as mental emulation. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 364 (1521), 12731280. https://doi.org/10.1098/rstb.2008.0314CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Müller, C. (1998). Iconicity and gesture. In Cavé, C., Guaïtella, I., & Santi, S. (Eds.), Oralité et gestualité: Interactions et comportements multimodaux dans la communication [Orality and gestuality: Multimodal interaction and behavior in communication]. Actes du colloque [Proceedings of the meeting of] ORAGE 2001 (pp. 321328). Paris, France: L’Harmattan.Google Scholar
Müller, C. (2014). Gestural modes of representation as techniques of depiction. In Müller, C., Cienki, A., Fricke, E., Ladewig, S., McNeill, D., & Bressem, J. (Eds.), Body - language - communication: An international handbook on multimodality in human interaction (Vol. 2, pp. 16871701). Berlin, Germany: De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Müller, C. (2017). How recurrent gestures mean: Conventionalized contexts-of-use and embodied motivation. Gesture, 16 (2), 276303. https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.16.2.05mulCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Nathan, M. J., & Walkington, C. (2017). Grounded and embodied mathematical cognition: Promoting mathematical insight and proof using action and language. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 2, 9. https://doi.org/10.1186/s41235-016-0040-5Google ScholarPubMed
Payrató, L., & Teßendorf, S. (2014). Pragmatic gestures. In Müller, C., Cienki, A., Fricke, E., Ladewig, S., McNeill, D., & Bressem, J. (Eds.), Body - language - communication: An international handbook on multimodality in human interaction (Vol. 2, pp. 15311539). Berlin, Germany: De Gruyter.Google Scholar
Pine, K. J., Bird, H., & Kirk, E. (2007). The effects of prohibiting gestures on children’s lexical retrieval ability. Developmental Science, 10 (6), 747754. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2007.00610.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ping, R., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2010). Gesturing saves cognitive resources when talking about non-present objects. Cognitive Science, 34 (4), 602619. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1551-6709.2010.01102.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ping, R., Goldin-Meadow, S., & Beilock, S. (2014). Understanding gesture: Is the listener’s motor system involved? Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 143(1), 195204. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0032246CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pouw, W. T. J. L., de Nooijer, J. A., van Gog, T., Zwaan, R. A., & Paas, F. (2014). Toward a more embedded/extended perspective on the cognitive function of gestures. Frontiers in Psychology, 5, Article 359. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2014.00359CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pouw, W. T. J. L., & Hostetter, A. (2016). Gesture as predictive action. Reti, Saperi, Linguaggi: Italian Journal of Cognitive Sciences, 3, 5780. https://doi.org/10.12832/83918Google Scholar
Pouw, W., Wassenburg, S. I., Hostetter, A. B., de Koning, B. B., & Paas, F. (2020). Does gesture strengthen sensorimotor knowledge of objects? The case of the size-weight illusion. Psychological Research, 84, 966980. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00426-018-1128-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Pyers, J. E., Magid, R., Gollan, T. H., & Emmorey, K. (2021). Gesture helps, only if you need it: Inhibiting gesture reduces tip‐of‐the‐tongue resolution for those with weak short-term memory. Cognitive Science, 45 (1), e12914. https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12914CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Radford, L., Demers, S., Guzmán, J., & Cerulli, M. (2003). Calculators, graphs, gestures, and the production of meaning. In Pateman, N., Dougherty, B., & Zilliox, J. (Eds.), Proceedings of the 27the Annual Conference of the International Group for the Psychology of Mathematics Education (Vol. 4, pp. 5562). Honolulu, HI: University of Hawaii.Google Scholar
Ravizza, S. (2003). Movement and lexical access: Do noniconic gestures aid in retrieval? Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 10, 610615. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03196522CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Richarz, A. (2017). Why Japan’s rail workers can’t stop pointing at things. Retrieved from https://www.atlasobscura.com/articles/pointing-and-calling-japan-trains, October 27, 2020.Google Scholar
Richland, L. E. (2015). Linking gestures: Cross-cultural variation during instructional analogies. Cognition and Instruction, 33, 295321. https://doi.org/10.1080/07370008.2015.1091459CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rimé, B., Shiaratura, L., Hupet, M., & Ghysselinckx, A. (1984). Effects of relative immobilization on the speaker’s nonverbal behavior and on the dialogue imagery level. Motivation & Emotion, 8, 311325. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00991870CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Risko, E. F., & Gilbert, S. J. (2016). Cognitive offloading. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20 (9), 676688. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2016.07.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shattuck-Hufnagel, S., & Ren, A. (2018). The prosodic characteristics of non-referential co-speech gestures in a sample of academic-lecture-style speech. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1514. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01514CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Shepard, R. N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. Science, 171 (3972), 701703. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.171.3972.701CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Solyu, F., Lester, F. K., & Newman, S. D. (2018). You can count on your fingers: The role of fingers in early mathematical development. Journal of Numerical Cognition, 4 (1), 107135. https://doi.org/10.5964/jnc.v4i1.85CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Streeck, J. (2009). Gesturecraft: The manu-facture of meaning. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Vest, N. A., Fyfe, E. R., Nathan, M. J., & Alibali, M. W. (2020). Learning from an avatar video instructor: Gesture mimicry supports middle school students’ algebra learning. Gesture, 19 (1), 128155. https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.18019.vesCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wagner, S. M., Nusbaum, H., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2004). Probing the mental representation of gesture: Is handwaving spatial? Journal of Memory and Language, 50 (4), 395407. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2004.01.002CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Walkington, C., Woods, D., Nathan, M. J., Chelule, G., & Wang, M. (2019). Does restricting hand gestures impair mathematical reasoning? Learning and Instruction, 64, Article 101225. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2019.101225CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilson, M. (2002). Six views of embodied cognition. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 625636. https://doi.org/10.3758/bf03196322CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wolfgram, M. (2014). Gesture and the communication of mathematical ontologies in classrooms. Journal of Linguistic Anthropology, 24, 216237. https://doi.org/10.1111/jola.12049CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Zlatev, J. (2014). Image schemas, mimetic schemas and children’s gestures. Cognitive Semiotics, 7 (1), 329. https://doi.org/10.1515/cogsem-2014-0002CrossRefGoogle Scholar

References

Argyriou, P., Mohr, C., & Kita, S. (2017). Hand matters: Left-hand gestures enhance metaphor explanation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 43 (6), 874886. https://doi.org/10.1037/xlm0000337Google ScholarPubMed
Balconi, M., Crivelli, D., & Cortesi, L. (2017). Transitive versus intransitive complex gesture representation: A comparison between execution, observation and imagination by fNIRS. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 42 (3), 179191. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10484-017-9365-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Barosso, F., Freedman, N., & Grand, S. (1978). Evocation of two types of hand movements in information processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 4 (2), 321329. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-1523.4.2.321Google Scholar
Benowitz, L. I., Bear, D. M., Rosenthal, R., Mesulam, M. M., Zaidel, E., & Sperry, R. W. (1983). Hemispheric specialization in nonverbal communication. Cortex, 19 (1), 511. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(83)80046-XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blonder, L. X., Bowers, D., & Heilman, K. M. (1991). The role of the right hemisphere in emotional communication. Brain, 114 (3), 11151127. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/114.3.1115CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Blonder, L. X., Burns, A. F., Bowers, D., Moore, R. W., & Heilman, K. M. (1993). Right hemisphere facial expressivity during natural conversation. Brain and Cognition, 21 (1), 4456. https://doi.org/10.1006/brcg.1993.1003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Boldrini, P., Zanella, R., Cantagallo, A., & Basaglia, N. (1992). Partial hemispheric disconnection syndrome of traumatic origin. Cortex, 28 (1), 135143. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-9452(13)80172-4CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bowers, D., Blonder, L. X., Feinberg, T., & Heilman, K. M. (1991). Differential impact of right and left hemisphere lesions on facial emotion and object imagery. Brain, 114 (6), 25932609. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/114.6.2593CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bressem, J., Ladewig, S. H., & Müller, C. (2013). Linguistic annotation system for gestures (LASG) . Müller, In C., Cienki, A., Fricke, E., Ladewig, S. H., McNeill, D. & Teßendorf, S. (Eds.), Body - language - communication: An international handbook on multimodality in human interaction (Vol. 1, pp. 10981124). Berlin, Germany: De Gruyter Mouton. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110261318.1098Google Scholar
Buxbaum, L. J., Schwartz, M. F., Coslett, B. H., & Carew, T. G. (1995). Naturalistic action and praxis in callosal apraxia. Neurocase, 1 (1), 317. https://doi.org/10.1080/13554799508402342CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Church, R. B., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1986). The mismatch between gesture and speech as an index of transitional knowledge. Cognition, 23 (1), 4371. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(86)90053-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cienki, A. (1998). Metaphoric gestures and some of their relations to verbal metaphoric expressions. In Koenig, J.-P. (Ed.), Discourse and cognition: Bridging the gap (pp. 189204). Stanford, CA: Center for the Study of Language and Information.Google Scholar
Cocks, N., Hird, K., & Kirsner, K. (2007). The relationship between right hemisphere damage and gesture in spontaneous discourse. Aphasiology, 21, 299319. https://doi.org/10.1080/02687030600911393CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dalby, J. Gibson, T. D., Grossi, V., & Schneider, R. (1980). Lateralized hand gesture during speech. Journal of Motor Behaviour, 12, 292297. https://doi.org/10.1080/00222895.1980.10735228CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Davis, M., & Hadiks, D. (1995). Demeanor and credibility. Semiotica, 106, 554. https://doi.org/10.1515/semi.1995.106.1-2.5CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davis, M., Walters, S. B., Vorus, N., & Connors, B. (2000). Defensive demeanor profiles. American Journal of Dance Therapy, 22 (2), 103121. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1026582324633CrossRefGoogle Scholar
De Renzi, E. (1999). Agnosia. In Denes, G. & Pizzamiglio, L. (Eds.), Handbook of clinical and experimental neuropsychology (pp. 371403). East Sussex, UK: Psychology Press.Google Scholar
De Renzi, E., Faglioni, P., & Sorgato, P. (1982). Modality-specific and supramodal mechanisms of apraxia. Brain, 105 (2), 301–12. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/105.2.301CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Duffy, R. J., & Duffy, J. R. (1989). An investigation of body part as object (BPO) responses in normal and brain-damaged adults. Brain and Cognition, 10 (2), 220236. https://doi.org/10.1016/0278-2626(89)90054-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Efron, D. (1972). Gesture and culture. The Hague, the Netherlands: Mouton. (Original work published 1941)Google Scholar
Farah, M. J. (1984). The neurological basis of mental imagery: A componential analysis. Cognition, 18 (1–3), 245272. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(84)90026-XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ferstl, E. C., Neumann, J., Bogler, C., & von Cramon, D. Y. (2008). The extended language network: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging studies on text comprehension. Human Brain Mapping, 29 (5), 581593. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.20422CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Feyereisen, P., & Havard, I. (1999). Mental imagery and production of hand gestures while speaking in younger and older adults. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 23 (2), 153171. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1021487510204CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Finkelnburg, C. M. (1870). Niederrheinische Gesellschaft in Bonn. Medicinische Section. Sitzung vom 21. März 1870 [Lower Rhine Society in Bonn. Medical section. Meeting of March 21, 1870]. Berliner Klinische Wochenschrift [Berlin Clinical Weekly], 37, 449450.Google Scholar
Freedman, N. (1972). The analysis of movement behavior during the clinical interview. In Siegman, A.W., & Pope, B. (Eds.), Studies in dyadic communication (pp. 153175). New York, NY: Pergamon.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frey, S. H., Funnell, M. G., Gerry, V. E., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (2005). A dissociation between the representation of tool-use skills and hand dominance: Insights from left- and right-handed callosotomy patients. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17 (2), 262272. https://doi.org/10.1162/0898929053124974CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Fukutake, T. (2002). Apraxia of tool use: An autopsy case of biparietal infarction. European Neurology, 49 (1), 4552. https://doi.org/10.1159/000067027CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Garber, P., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2002). Gesture offers insight into problem-solving in adults and children. Cognitive Science, 26 (6), 817831. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15516709cog2606_5Google Scholar
Gazzaniga, M. S., Bogen, J. E., & Sperry, R. W. (1967). Dyspraxia following division of the cerebral commissures. Archives of Neurology, 16 (6), 606612. https://doi.org/10.1001/archneur.1967.00470240044005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldenberg, G. (1996). Defective imitation of gestures in patients with damage in the left or right hemispheres. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 61 (2), 176180. https://doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.61.2.176CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldenberg, G. (1999). Matching and imitation of hand and finger postures in patients with damage in the left or right hemispheres. Neuropsychologia, 37 (5), 559566. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0028-3932(98)00111-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldenberg, G. (2011). Apraxien [Apraxias]. Göttingen, Germany: Hogrefe Verlag.Google Scholar
Goldenberg, G. (2013). Apraxia in left-handers. Brain, 136 (8), 25922601. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awt181CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldenberg, G., & Hagmann, S. (1997). The meaning of meaningless gestures: A study of visuo-imitative apraxia. Neuropsychologia, 35 (3), 333341. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(96)00085-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldenberg, G., Hartmann, K., & Schlott, I. (2003). Defective pantomime of object use in left brain damage: Apraxia or asymbolia? Neuropsychologia, 41 (12), 15651573. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0028-3932(03)00120-9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldenberg, G., Hentze, S., & Hermsdörfer, J. (2004). The effect of tactile feedback on pantomime of tool use in apraxia. Neurology, 63 (10), 1863–1867. https://doi.org/10.1212/01.wnl.0000144283.38174.07CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldenberg, G., Müllbacher, W., & Nowak, A. (1995). Imagery without perception: A case study of anosognosia for cortical blindness. Neuropsychologia, 33 (11), 13731382. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(95)00070-jCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldenberg, G., Wimmer, A., Holzner, F., & Wessely, P. (1985). Apraxia of the left limbs in a case of callosal disconnection: The contribution of medial frontal lobe damage. Cortex, 21 (1), 135148. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-9452(85)80021-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., Alibali, M. W., & Church, R. B. (1993). Transitions in concept acquisition: Using the hand to read the mind. Psychological Review, 100 (2), 279297. https://doi.org/10.1037//0033-295X.100.2.279CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goodale, M. A., Jakobsen, L. S., & Keillor, J. M. (1994). Differences in the visual control of pantomimed and natural grasping movements. Neuropsychologia, 32 (10), 11591178. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(94)90100-7CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Haaland, K. Y., & Flaherty, D. (1984). The different types of limb apraxia errors made by patients with left vs. right hemisphere damage. Brain and Cognition, 3 (4), 370384. https://doi.org/10.1016/0278-2626(84)90029-0CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hadar, U., Wenkert-Olenik, Krauss, R. , & Soroker, N. (1998). Gesture and processing of speech: Neuropsychological evidence. Brain and Language, 62 (1), 107126. https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.1997.1890CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hampson, E., & Kimura, D. (1984). Hand movement asymmetries during verbal and nonverbal tasks. Canadian Journal of Psychology, 38 (1), 102125. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0080787CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heath, M., Roy, E. A., Black, S. E., & Westwood, D. A. (2001). Intransitive limb gestures and apraxia following unilateral stroke. Journal of Clinical and Experimental Neuropsychology, 23 (5), 628642. https://doi.org/10.1076/jcen.23.5.628.1240CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Heilman, K. M., & Rothi, L. J. G. (1993). Apraxia. In Heilman, K. M., & Valenstein, E. (Eds.), Clinical neuropsychology (pp. 141164). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helmich, I., Holle, H., Rein, R., & Lausberg, H. (2015). Brain oxygenation patterns during the execution of tool use demonstration, tool use pantomime, and body-part-as-object tool use. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 96 (1), 17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2015.03.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Helmich, I., Meyer, C., Voelk, M., Coenen, J. Mueller, S. Schepmann, J., & Lausberg, H. (2022). The pantomime of mental rotation: Left-handers are less lateralized. Neuropsychologia, 176, 108385. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2022.108385CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Helmich, I., Rein, R., Niemann, N., & Lausberg, H. (2013). Hemispheric differences of motor execution: A near-infrared spectroscopy study. Advances in Experimental Medicine and Biology, 789, 5964. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4614-7411-1_9CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Helmich, I., Voelk, M. Coenen, J., Xu, L., Reinhardt, J., Mueller, S., Schepmann, J., & Lausberg, H. (2021). Hemispheric specialization for nonverbal gestures depicting motion and space. Brain and Cognition, 151, 105736. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandc.2021.105736.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hermsdörfer, J., Li, Y., Randerath, J., Goldenberg, G., & Johannsen, L. (2012). Tool use without a tool: Kinematic characteristics of pantomiming as compared to actual use and the effect of brain damage. Experimental Brain Research, 218, 201214. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00221-012-3021-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hermsdörfer, J., Terlinden, G., Mühlau, M., Goldenberg, G., & Wohlschläger, A. M. (2007). Neural representations of pantomimed and actual tool use: Evidence from an event-related fMRI study. NeuroImage, 36, 109118. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.03.037CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hogrefe, K., Rein, R., Skomroch, H., & Lausberg, H. (2016). Co-speech hand movements during narrations: What is the impact of right vs. left hemisphere brain damage. Neuropsychologia, 93 (A), 176188. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.10.015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Imazu, S., Sugio, T., Tanaka, S., & Inui, T. (2007). Differences between actual and imagined usage of chopsticks: An fMRI study. Cortex, 43 (3), 301307. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0010-9452(08)70456-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Jarry, C., Osiurak, F., Delafuys, D., Chauviré, V., Etcharry-Bouyx, F., & Le Gall, D. (2013). Apraxia of tool use: More evidence for the technical reasoning hypothesis. Cortex, 49 (9), 23222333. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2013.02.011CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kelly, S. D., Kravitz, C., & Hopkins, M. (2004). Neural correlates of bimodal speech and gesture comprehension. Brain and Language, 89 (1), 253260. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0093-934X(03)00335-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kimura, D. (1973a). Manual activity during speaking – I. Right-handers. Neuropsychologia, 11 (1), 4550. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(73)90063-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kimura, D. (1973b). Manual activity during speaking – II. Left-handers. Neuropsychologia, 11 (1), 5155. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(73)90064-XCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kita, S., de Condappa, O., & Mohr, C. (2007). Metaphor explanation attenuates the right-hand preference for depictive co-speech gestures that imitate actions. Brain and Language, 101 (3), 185197. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2006.11.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kita, S., & Lausberg, H. (2008). Generation of co-speech gestures based on spatial imagery from the right-hemisphere: Evidence from split-brain patients. Cortex, 44 (2), 131139. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2006.04.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kosslyn, S. M. (1980). Image and the mind. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Króliczak, G., & Frey, S. H. (2009). A common network in the left cerebral hemisphere represents planning of tool use pantomimes and familiar intransitive gestures at the hand-independent level. Cerebral Cortex, 19 (10), 23962410. https://doi.org/10.1093/cercor/bhn261CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Laimgruber, K., Goldenberg, G., & Hermsdörfer, J. (2005). Manual and hemispheric asymmetries in the execution of actual and pantomimed prehension. Neuropsychologia, 43 (5), 682692. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2004.09.004CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lane, R. D., & Schwartz, G. E. (1987). Levels of emotional awareness: A cognitive-developmental theory and its application to psychopathology. American Journal of Psychiatry, 144 (2), 133143. https://doi.org/10.1176/ajp.144.2.133Google ScholarPubMed
Lausberg, H. (2013). Understanding body movement. A guide to empirical research on nonverbal behaviour. With an introduction to the NEUROGES coding system. Frankfurt am Main, Germany: Peter Lang.Google Scholar
Lausberg, H. (2019). The NEUROGES® analysis system for nonverbal behavior and gesture. Berlin, Germany: Peter Lang.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lausberg, H., & Cruz, R. F. (2004). Hemispheric specialization for imitation of hand-head positions and finger configurations: A controlled study in patients with complete callosotomy. Neuropsychologia, 42 (3), 320334. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2003.08.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lausberg, H., Cruz, R.F., Kita, S., Zaidel, E., & Ptito, A. (2003). Pantomime to visual presentation of objects: Left hand dyspraxia in patients with complete callosotomy. Brain, 126 (2), 343360. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awg042CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lausberg, H., Davis, M., & Rothenhäusler, A. (2000). Hemispheric specialization in spontaneous gesticulation in a patient with callosal disconnection. Neuropsychologia, 38 (12), 16541663. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(00)00071-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lausberg, H., Göttert, R., Münßinger, U., Boegner, F., & Marx, P. (1999). Callosal disconnection syndrome in a left-handed patient due to infarction of the total length of the corpus callosum. Neuropsychologia, 37 (3), 253265. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3932(98)00079-7CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lausberg, H., Kazzer, P., Heekeren, H. R., & Wartenburger, I. (2015). Pantomiming tool use with an imaginary tool in hand as compared to demonstration with tool in hand specifically modulates the left middle and superior temporal gyri. Cortex, 71, 114. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2015.05.021CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lausberg, H., & Kita, S. (2003). The content of the message influences the hand choice in co-speech gestures and in gesturing without speaking. Brain and Language, 86 (1), 5769. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0093-934x(02)00534-5CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lausberg, H., Kita, S., Zaidel, E., & Ptito, A. (2003). Split-brain patients neglect left personal space during right-handed gestures. Neuropsychologia, 41 (10), 13171329. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0028-3932(03)00047-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lausberg, H., & Sloetjes, H. (2016). The revised NEUROGES-ELAN system: An objective and reliable interdisciplinary analysis tool for nonverbal behavior and gesture. Behavior Research Methods, 48, 973993. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13428-015-0622-zCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lausberg, H., Zaidel, E., Cruz, R. F., & Ptito, A. (2007). Speech-independent production of communicative gestures: Evidence from patients with complete callosal disconnection. Neuropsychologia, 45 (13), 30923104. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2007.05.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lavergne, J., & Kimura, D. (1987). Hand movement asymmetry during speech: No effect of speaking topic. Neuropsychologia, 25 (4), 689693. https://doi.org/10.1016/0028-3932(87)90060-1CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lewis, J. W., Brefczynski, J. A., Phinney, R. E., Janik, J. J., & DeYoe, E. A. (2005). Distinct cortical pathways for processing tool versus animal sounds. The Journal of Neuroscience, 25 (21), 51485158. https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0419-05.2005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lezak, M. D. (1995). Neuropsychological assessment. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Liepmann, H. (1908). Drei Aufsätze aus dem Apraxiegebiet [Three essays from the area of apraxia]. Berlin, Germany: Verlag von S. Karger.Google Scholar
Liepmann, H., & Maas, O. (1907). Fall von linksseitiger Agraphie und Apraxie bei rechtsseitiger Lähmung [Case of left-sided agraphia and apraxia with right-sided paralysis]. Journal für Psychologie und Neurologie [Journal of Psychology and Neurology], 10, 214227.Google Scholar
McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
McNeill, D., & Pedelty, L. L. (1995). Right brain and gesture. In Emmorey, K., & Reilly, J. (Eds.), Language, gesture, and space (pp. 6385). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Mahl, F. G. (1968). Gestures and body movements in interviews. In Shilen, J. M. (Ed.), Research in psychotherapy (pp. 295–346). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. https://doi.org/10.1037/10546-016Google Scholar
Meyer, C. (2021). Hemisphärische Spezialisierung von Linkshändern in der Produktion nonverbaler Gesten bei mentalen Rotationsaufgaben [Hemispheric specialization of left-handers in the production of nonverbal gestures during mental rotation tasks]. (Unpublished MA thesis). Deutsche Sporthochschule Köln, Germany.Google Scholar
Miller, K., & Franz, E. A. (2005). Bimanual gestures: Expression of spatial representations that accompany speech processes. Laterality, 10 (3), 243265. https://doi.org/10.1080/13576500442000067CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mittelberg, I. (2006). Metaphor and metonymy in language and gesture: Discourse evidence for multimodal models of grammar. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). Cornell University, Ithaca, NY. Retrieved from ProQuest Dissertations Publishing https://www.proquest.com/docview/305321684?parentSessionId=29m%2BkPvqSXlZu51daNo4tdpfBH0Zq5Oih5Bi0bRzB58%3D .Google Scholar
Moscovitch, M., & Olds, J. (1982). Asymmetries in spontaneous facial expressions and their possible relation to hemispheric specialization. Neuropsychologia, 20 (1), 7181.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Motomura, N., & Yamadori, A. (1994). A case of ideational apraxia with impairment of object use and preservation of object pantomime. Cortex, 30 (1), 167170. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0010-9452(13)80332-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Mozaz, M. J., Pena, J., Barraquer, L. L., Marti, J., & Goldstein, L. H. (1993). Use of body part as object in brain-damaged subjects. The Clinical Neuropsychologist, 7, 3947.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Müller, C. (1998). Redebegleitende Gesten – Kulturgeschichte, Theorie, Sprachvergleich [Co-speech gestures: Cultural history, theory, cross-linguistic comparison]. Berlin, Germany: Verlag Arno Spitz.Google Scholar
Ochipa, C., Rothi, L. J. G., & Heilman, K. M. (1994). Conduction apraxia. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, 57 (10), 12411244.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
O’Reilly, A. W. (1995). Using representations: Comprehension and production of actions with imagined objects. Child Development, 66 (4), 9991010.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Piaget, J. (1962). Play, dreams and imitation in childhood. New York, NY: Norton.Google Scholar
Randerath, J., Li, Y., Goldenberg, G., & Hermsdörfer, J. (2009). Grasping tools: Effects of task and apraxia. Neuropsychologia, 47 (2), 497505. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2008.10.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Randerath, J., Goldenberg, G., Spijkers, W., Li, Y., & Hermsdörfer, J. (2011). From pantomime to actual use: How affordances can facilitate actual tool-use. Neuropsychologia, 49 (9), 24102416. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jeuropsychologia.2011.04.017CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rapcsak, S. Z., Ochipa, C., Beeson, P. M., & Rubens, A. B. (1993). Praxis and the right hemisphere. Brain and Cognition, 23 (2), 181202.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ross, E. D., & Mesulam, M. M. (1979). Dominant language functions of the right hemisphere? Prosody and emotional gesturing. Archives of Neurology, 36 (3), 144148.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rousseaux, M., Daveluy, W., & Kozlowski, O. (2010). Communication in conversation in stroke patients. Journal of Neurology, 257, 10991107. https://doi.org/10.1007/s00415-010-5469-8CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Roy, E. A., Heath, M., Westwood, D., Schweizer, T. A., Dixon, M. J., Black, S. E. … & Square, P. A. (2000). Task demands and limb apraxia in stroke. Brain and Cognition, 44 (2), 253279. https://doi.org/10.1006/brcg.2000.1230CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Saucier, D. M., & Elias, L. J. (2001). Lateral and sex differences in manual gesture during conversation. Laterality, 6, 239245. https://doi.org/10.1080/713754416CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schacter, D. L. (1992). Implicit knowledge: New perspectives on unconscious processes. Proceedings of the National Academy of Science of the USA, 89 (23), 1111311117.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Schirmer, A., Alter, K., Kotz, S. A., & Friederici, A. D. (2001). Lateralization of prosody during language production: A lesion study. Brain and Language, 76 (1), 117. https://doi.org/10.1006/brln.2000.2381CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shallice, T. (1982). Specific impairments of planning. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B, Biological Sciences, 298 (1089), 199209.Google ScholarPubMed
Sousa-Posa, J. F., Rohrberg, R., & Mercure, A. (1979). Effects of type of information (abstract-concrete) and field dependence on asymmetry of hand movements during speech. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 48 (3), 13231330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sperry, R. W. (1967). Mental unity following surgical disconnection of the cerebral hemispheres. The Harvey Lectures Series, 62, 293323.Google Scholar
Sperry, R. W. (1968). Hemisphere deconnection and unity in conscious awareness. American Psychologist, 23, 723733.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Stephens, D. (1983). Hemispheric language dominance and gesture hand preference. (Unpublished doctoral dissertation). University of Chicago.Google Scholar
Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18 (6), 643662.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sunderland, A., Wilkins, L., & Dineen, R. (2011). Tool use and action planning in apraxia. Neuropsychologia, 49(5), 12751286. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.01.020CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Trope, I., Fishman, B., Gur, R. C., Sussman, N. M., & Gur, R. E. (1987). Contralateral and ipsilateral control of fingers following callosotomy. Neuropsychologia, 25 (1-B), 287291.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Ulrich, G., & Harms, K. (1979). Video-analytic study of manual kinesics and its lateralization in the course of treatment of depressive syndromes. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 59 (5), 481492.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Volpe, B. T., Sidtis, J. J., Holtzman, J. D., Wilson, D. H., & Gazzaniga, M. S. (1982). Cortical mechanisms involved in praxis: Observations following partial and complete section of the corpus callosum in man. Neurology, 32 (6), 645650.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilkins, D., & de Ruiter, J. P. (1999). Hand preference for representational gestures: A comparison of Arente and Dutch speakers. In Van Geenhoven, V. & Warner, N. (Eds.), Annual report 1999 (pp. 5152). Nijmegen, the Netherlands: Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics.Google Scholar
Zaidel, E., White, H., Sakurai, E., & Banks, W. (1988). Hemispheric locus of lexical congruity effects: Neuropsychological reinterpretation of psycholinguistic Results. In Chiarello, C. (Ed.), Right hemisphere contributions to lexical semantics (pp. 7188). New York, NY: Springer.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

References

Alibali, M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1993). Gesture-speech mismatch and mechanisms of learning: What the hands reveal about a child′s state of mind. Cognitive Psychology, 25(4), 468523. https://doi.org/10.1006/cogp.1993.1012CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Alibali, M., Nathan, M., Wolfgram, M., Church, R. B., Jacobs, S., Johnson Martinez, C., & Knuth, E. J. (2014). How teachers link ideas in mathematics instruction using speech and gesture: A corpus analysis. Cognition and Instruction, 32(1), 65100. https://doi.org/10.1080/07370008.2013.858161CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alibali, M. W., Flevares, L. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1997). Assessing knowledge conveyed in gesture: Do teachers have the upper hand? Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(1), 183193. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.89.1.183CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Alibali, M. W., & Nathan, M. J. (2012). Embodiment in mathematics teaching and learning: Evidence from learners’ and teachers’ gestures. Journal of the Learning Sciences, 21(2), 247286. https://doi.org/10.1080/10508406.2011.611446CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of Behavior and Development, 21(3), 205226.Google Scholar
Beaudoin-Ryan, L., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2014). Teaching moral reasoning through gesture. Developmental Science, 17(6), 984990. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12180CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Behne, T., Liszkowski, U., Carpenter, M., & Tomasello, M. (2012). Twelve-month-olds’ comprehension and production of pointing. The British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 30, 359375. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.2044-835X.2011.02043.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bertenthal, B. I., Boyer, T. W., & Harding, S. (2014). When do infants begin to follow a point? Developmental Psychology, 50(8), 20362048. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0037152CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Booth, A. E., McGregor, K. K., & Rohlfing, K. J. (2008). Socio-pragmatics and attention: Contributions to gesturally guided word learning in toddlers. Language Learning and Development, 4(3), 179202. https://doi.org/10.1080/15475440802143091CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Broaders, S. C., Cook, S. W., Mitchell, Z., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2007). Making children gesture brings out implicit knowledge and leads to learning. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136(4), 539550. https://doi.org/10.1037/0096-3445.136.4.539CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Butterworth, G. (2003). Pointing is the royal road to language for babies. In Kita, S. (Ed.), Pointing: Where language, culture, and cognition meet (pp. 933). Mahway, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.Google Scholar
Capone, N. C., & McGregory, K. K. (2005). The effect of semantic representation on toddlers’ word retrieval. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 48(6), 14681480. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2005/102)CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Carey, S. (2009). The origin of concepts. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Church, R., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1986). The mismatch between gesture and speech as an index of transitional knowledge. Cognition, 23(1), 4371. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(86)90053-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Colonnesi, C., Stams, G. J. J. M., Koster, I., & Noom, M. J. (2010). The relation between pointing and language development: A meta-analysis. Developmental Review, 30(4), 352366. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.dr.2010.10.001CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Congdon, E., Kwon, M.-K., & Levine, S. (2018). Learning to measure through action and gesture: Children’s prior knowledge matters. Cognition, 180, 182190. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2018.07.002CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Congdon, E. L., Novack, M. A., Brooks, N., Hemani-Lopez, N., O’Keefe, L., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2017). Better together: Simultaneous presentation of speech and gesture in math instruction supports generalization and retention. Learning and Instruction, 50, 6574 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.learninstruc.2017.03.005CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Cook, S. W., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2006). The role of gesture in learning: Do children use their hands to change their minds? Journal of Cognition and Development, 7(2), 211232. https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327647jcd0702_4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Cook, S. W., Mitchell, Z., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2008). Gesturing makes learning last. Cognition, 106(2), 10471058. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2007.04.010CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Emmorey, K. (1999). Do signers gesture? In Messing, L. & Campbell, R. (Eds.), Gesture, speech, and sign (pp. 133159). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gibson, D. J., Gunderson, E. A., Spaepen, E., Levine, S. C., & Goldin‐Meadow, S. (2019). Number gestures predict learning of number words. Developmental Science, 22(3), e12791. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12791CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldin-Meadow, S. (2003). Hearing gesture: How our hands help us think. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., Alibali, M. W., & Church, R. B. (1993). Transitions in concept acquisition: Using the hand to read the mind. Psychological Review, 100(2), 279297. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.100.2.279CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldin-Meadow, S., Cook, S. W., & Mitchell, Z. A. (2009). Gesturing gives children new ideas about math. Psychological Science, 20(3), 267272. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2009.02297.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldin-Meadow, S., Kim, S., & Singer, M. (1999). What the teacher’s hands tell the student’s mind about math. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91, 720730. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.91.4.720CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., Levine, S. C., Zinchenko, E., Yip, T. K., Hemani, N., & Factor, L. (2012). Doing gesture promotes learning a mental transformation task better than seeing gesture. Developmental Science, 15(6), 876884. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01185.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldin-Meadow, S., Shield, A., Lenzen, D., Herzig, M., & Padden, C. (2012). The gestures ASL signers use tell us when they are ready to learn math. Cognition, 123(3), 448453. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2012.02.006CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Goldin-Meadow, S., & Singer, M. A. (2003). From children’s hands to adults’ ears: Gesture’s role in the learning process. Developmental Psychology, 39(3), 509520. https://doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.39.3.509CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goodrich, W., & Hudson Kam, C. L. (2009). Co‐speech gesture as input in verb learning. Developmental Science, 12(1), 8187.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Greenfield, P. M., & Smith, J. H. (1976). Structure of communication in early language development. New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Gunderson, E. A., Spaepen, E., Gibson, D., Goldin-Meadow, S., & Levine, S. C. (2015). Gesture as a window onto children’s number knowledge. Cognition, 144, 1428. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.07.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hostetter, A. B. (2011). When do gestures communicate? A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 137(2), 297315. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022128CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Iverson, J. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science, 16(5), 367371. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01542.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Kendon, A. (2004). Gesture: Visible action as utterance. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Koumoutsakis, T., Church, R. B., Alibali, M., Singer, M., & Ayman-Nolley, S. (2016). Gesture in instruction: Evidence from live and video lessons. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 40(4), 301315. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10919-016-0234-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Krehm, M., Onishi, K. H., & Vouloumanos, A. (2014). I see your point: Infants under 12 months understand that pointing is communicative. Journal of Cognition and Development, 15(4), 527538. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2012.736112CrossRefGoogle Scholar
LeBarton, E. S., Goldin-Meadow, S., & Raudenbush, S. (2015). Experimentally induced increases in early gesture lead to increases in spoken vocabulary. Journal of Cognition and Development, 16(2), 199220. https://doi.org/10.1080/15248372.2013.858041CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lucca, K., & Wilbourn, M. P. (2018). Communicating to learn: Infants’ pointing gestures result in optimal learning. Child Development, 89(3), 941960. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12707CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Lucca, K., & Wilbourn, M. P. (2019). The what and the how: Information-seeking pointing gestures facilitate learning labels and functions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 178, 417436. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2018.08.003CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McGregor, K. K., Rohlfing, K. J., Bean, A., & Marschner, E. (2009). Gesture as a support for word learning: The case of under. Journal of Child Language, 36(4), 807828. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0305000908009173CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
McNeill, N. M., Hornburg, C. B., Devlin, B. L., Carrazza, C., & McKeever, M. O. (2019). Consequences of individual differences in children’s formal understanding of mathematical equivalence. Child Development, 90(3), 940956. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12948CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Macedonia, M., Müller, K., & Friederici, A. D. (2011). The impact of iconic gestures on foreign language word learning and its neural substrate. Human Brain Mapping, 32(6), 982998. https://doi.org/10.1002/hbm.21084CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Novack, M. A., Congdon, E. L., Hemani-Lopez, N., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2014). From action to abstraction: Using the hands to learn math. Psychological Science, 25(4), 903910. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797613518351CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Novack, M. A., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2017). Gesture as representational action: A paper about function. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 24(3), 652665. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-016-1145-zCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Novack, M. A., Goldin-Meadow, S., & Woodward, A. L. (2015). Learning from gesture: How early does it happen? Cognition, 142, 138147, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.05.018CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Novack, M. A., Wakefield, E. M., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2016). What makes a movement a gesture? Cognition, 146, 339348. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2015.10.014CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Özçalışkan, Ş. , & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Gesture is at the cutting edge of early language development. Cognition, 96(3), B101B113. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2005.01.001CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Özçalışkan, Ş. , & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2011). Is there an iconic gesture spurt at 26 months? In Stam, G., & Ishino, M. (Eds.), Integrating gestures: The interdisciplinary nature of gesture (pp. 163174). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Perry, M., Church, R. B., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (1988). Transitional knowledge in the acquisition of concepts. Cognitive Development, 3(4), 359400. https://doi.org/10.1016/0885-2014(88)90021-4CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rohlfing, K. J., Longo, M. R., & Bertenthal, B. I. (2012). Dynamic pointing triggers shifts of visual attention in young infants. Developmental Science, 15(3), 426435. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2012.01139.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Rowe, M. L., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2009). Early gesture selectively predicts later language learning. Developmental Science, 12(1), 182187. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00764.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sandler, W. (2009). Symbiotic symbolization by hand and mouth in sign language. Semiotica, 2009(174), 241275. https://doi.org/10.1515/semi.2009.035CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Sarnecka, B. W., & Carey, S. (2008). How counting represents number: What children must learn and when they learn it. Cognition, 108(3), 662674. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cognition.2008.05.007CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sarnecka, B. W., & Lee, M. D. (2009). Levels of number knowledge during early childhood. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 103(3), 325337. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jecp.2009.02.007CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Singer, M. A., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2005). Children learn when their teacher’s gestures and speech differ. Psychological Science, 16(2), 8589. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.00786.xCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Son, J. Y., Ramos, P., DeWolf, M., Loftus, W., & Stigler, J. W. (2018). Exploring the practicing-connections hypothesis: Using gesture to support coordination of ideas in understanding a complex statistical concept. Cognitive Research: Principles and Implications, 3, Article 1. https://doi.org/10.1186/s41235-017-0085-0Google ScholarPubMed
Valenzeno, L., Alibali, M. W., & Klatzky, R. (2003). Teachers’ gestures facilitate students’ learning: A lesson in symmetry. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 28(2), 187204. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0361-476X(02)00007-3CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wakefield, E. M., Congdon, E. L., Novack, M. A., Goldin-Meadow, S., & James, K. H. (2019). Learning math by hand: The neural effects of gesture-based instruction in 8-year-old children. Attention, Perception, and Psychophysics, 81(7), 23432353. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13414-019-01755-yCrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wakefield, E. M., Foley, A. E., Ping, R., Villarreal, J. N., Goldin-Meadow, S., & Levine, S. C. (2019). Breaking down gesture and action in mental rotation: Understanding the components of movement that promote learning. Developmental Psychology, 55(5), 981993. https://doi.org/10.1037/dev0000697CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wakefield, E. M., Hall, C., James, K. H., & Goldin‐Meadow, S. (2018). Gesture for generalization: Gesture facilitates flexible learning of words for actions on objects. Developmental Science, 21(5), e12656. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12656CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wakefield, E. M., & James, K. H. (2015). Effects of learning with gesture on children’s understanding of a new language concept. Developmental Psychology, 51(8), 11051114. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0039471CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wakefield, E., Novack, M. A., Congdon, E. L., Franconeri, S., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2018). Gesture helps learners learn, but not merely by guiding their visual attention. Developmental Science, 21, e12664. https://doi.org/10.1111/desc.12664CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wakefield, E. M., Novack, M. A., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2017). Unpacking the ontogeny of gesture understanding: How movement becomes meaningful across development. Child Development, 89(3), e245e260. https://doi.org/10.1111/cdev.12817Google ScholarPubMed
Woodward, A. L. (2004). Infants’ use of action knowledge to get a grasp on words. In Hall, D. G., & Waxman, S. R. (Eds.), Weaving a lexicon (pp. 149172). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodward, A. L., & Guajardo, J. J. (2002). Infants’ understanding of the point gesture as an object-directed action. Cognitive Development, 17(1), 10611084. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0885-2014(02)00074-6CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wynn, K. (1990). Children’s understanding of counting. Cognition, 36(2), 155193. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(90)90003-3CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wynn, K. (1992). Children’s acquisition of the number words and the counting system. Cognitive Psychology, 24(2), 220251. https://doi.org/10.1016/0010-0285(92)90008-PCrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×