Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-cd9895bd7-gxg78 Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2024-12-25T16:02:57.708Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

16 - Gesture and Sign Language

from Part III - Gestures and Language

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  01 May 2024

Alan Cienki
Affiliation:
Vrije Universiteit, Amsterdam
Get access

Summary

This chapter reviews the relation between gesture and the natural signed languages of deaf communities. Signs were for centuries considered to be unanalyzable depictive gestures. Modern linguistic research has demonstrated that signs are composed of meaningless parts, equivalent to spoken language phonemes, that are combined to form meaningful signs. The chapter discusses a system called homesign used where a deaf child with hearing parents is not exposed to signed languages during language acquisition. Two ways in which gesture may become incorporated into a signed language through the historical process of grammaticalization are described. In the first, gestures are incorporated into a signed language as lexical signs, which go on to develop grammatical meaning. In the second, ways in which the sign is produced, its manner of movement, and certain facial displays, are incorporated not as lexical signs but as prosody or intonation, which may develop grammatical meaning. Finally, the chapter critically examines a new view in which certain signs are considered to be fusions of sign and gesture and proposes a cognitive linguistic analysis based in the theory of cognitive grammar.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2024

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Alibali, M. W., Heath, D. C., & Myers, H. J. (2001). Effects of visibility between speaker and listener on gesture production: Some gestures are meant to be seen. Journal of Memory and Language, 44(2), 169188. https://doi.org/10.1006/jmla.2 000.2752CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Armstrong, D. F., Stokoe, W. C., & Wilcox, S. (1995). Gesture and the nature of language. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bates, E., Camaioni, L., & Volterra, V. (1975). The acquisition of performatives prior to speech. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of Behavior and Development, 21(3), 205226.Google Scholar
Biau, E., & Soto-Faraco, S. (2013). Beat gestures modulate auditory integration in speech perception. Brain and Language, 124(2), 143152. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bandl.2012.10.008CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Biau, E., & Soto-Faraco, S. (2015). Synchronization by the hand: The sight of gestures modulates low-frequency activity in brain responses to continuous speech. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 9, 527. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2015.00527CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bybee, J. (2006). From usage to grammar: The mind’s response to repetition. Language, 82(4), 711733.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J. (2010). Language, usage and cognition. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Capirci, O., Iverson, J. M., Montanari, S., & Volterra, V. (2002). Gestural, signed and spoken modalities in early language development: The role of linguistic input. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 5(1), 2537. https://doi.org/10.1017/S1366728902000123CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Clark, A. (2012). How to write American Sign Language. Burnsville, MN: ASLWrite.Google Scholar
Darwin, C. (1872). The expression of the emotions in man and animals. London, UK: J. Murray.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Dodwell, C. R. (2000). Anglo-Saxon gestures and the Roman stage. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.Google Scholar
Dotter, F. (2018). Most characteristic elements of sign language texts are intricate mixtures of linguistic and non-linguistic parts, aren’t they? Colloquium: New Philologies, 3(1), 162.Google Scholar
Engberg-Pedersen, E. (1996). Iconicity and arbitrariness. In Michael, F., Harder, P., Heltoft, L., & Jakobsen, L. F. (Eds.), Content, expression and structure: Studies in Danish functional grammar (pp. 453468). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing Company.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Erman, A. (1971). The literature of the ancient Egyptians: Poems, narratives, and manuals of instruction from the third and second millennia B.C. New York, NY: Benjamin Blom.Google Scholar
Fenlon, J., Cooperrider, K., Keane, J., Brentari, D., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2019). Comparing sign language and gesture: Insights from pointing. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, 4(1), 126. https://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.499CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Frishberg, N. (1975). Arbitrariness and iconicity: Historical change in American Sign Language. Language, 51(3), 696719.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Geertz, C. (1973). The interpretation of cultures. New York, NY: Basic Books.Google Scholar
Geertz, C. (1974). “From the native’s point of view”: On the nature of anthropological understanding. Bulletin of the American Academy of Arts and Sciences, 28(1), 26045.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldin-Meadow, S., & Brentari, D. (2017). Gesture, sign and language: The coming of age of sign language and gesture studies. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40, e46. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X15001247CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Hockett, C. (1982). The origin of speech. In Wang, W. S.-Y. (Ed.), Human communication: Language and its psychobiological bases (pp. 512). San Francisco, CA: W. H. Freeman and Company.Google Scholar
Hodge, G., & Johnston, T. (2014). Points, depictions, gestures and enactment: Partly lexical and non-lexical signs as core elements of single clause-like units in Auslan (Australian Sign Language). Australian Journal of Linguistics, 34(2), 262291. https://doi.org/10.1080/07268602.2014.887408CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopper, P. J., & Traugott, E. C. (2003). Grammaticalization. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Janzen, T., & Shaffer, B. (2002). Gesture as the substrate in the process of ASL grammaticization. In Meier, R., Quinto, D., & Cormier, K. (Eds.), Modality and structure in signed and spoken languages (pp. 199223). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Jarque, M. J. (2006). The expression of possibility in Catalan Sign Language: The sign PODER. 5th International Conference of the Spanish Cognitive Linguistics Association (AELCO/ SCOLA). Murcia, October 19–21, 2006.Google Scholar
Kendon, A. (2017). Languages as semiotically heterogenous systems. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40, e59. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X15002940CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Klima, E., & Bellugi, U. (1979). The signs of language. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.Google Scholar
Krahmer, E., & Swerts, M. (2007). The effects of visual beats on prosodic prominence: Acoustic analyses, auditory perception and visual perception. Journal of Memory and Language, 57(3), 396414. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2007.06.005CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kusters, A., & Sahasrabudhe, S. (2018). Language ideologies on the difference between gesture and sign. Language & Communication, 60, 4463. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.langcom.2018.01.008CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lane, H. (1980). A chronology of the oppression of sign language in France and the United States. In Lane, H. & Grosjean, F. (Eds.), Recent perspectives on American Sign Language (pp. 119161). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum.Google Scholar
Lane, H. (1984). When the mind hears: A history of the deaf. New York, NY: Random House.Google Scholar
Langacker, R. W. (1987). Foundations of cognitive grammar: Volume I, Theoretical foundations. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press.Google Scholar
Langacker, R. W. (2000). A dynamic usage-based model. In Barlow, M. & Kemmer, S. (Eds.), Usage-based models of language (pp. 163). Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications Center for the Study of Language and Information.Google Scholar
Langacker, R. W. (2001). Discourse in cognitive grammar. Cognitive Linguistics, 12 (2), 143188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Langacker, R. W. (2008). Cognitive grammar: A basic introduction. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Long, J. S. (1918). The sign language: A manual of signs. Iowa City, IA: Athens Press.Google Scholar
Lucas, C. (1989). The sociolinguistics of the Deaf community. San Diego, CA: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Mandel, M. (1977). Iconic devices in American Sign Language. In Friedman, L. A. (Ed.), On the other hand: New perspectives on American Sign Language (pp. 57108). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Martínez, R., & Wilcox, S. (2019). Pointing and placing: Nominal grounding in Argentine Sign Language. Cognitive Linguistics, 30(1), 85121. https://doi.org/10.1515/cog-2018-0010CrossRefGoogle Scholar
McNeill, D. (1992). Hand and mind: What gestures reveal about thought. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
McNeill, D., Levy, E. T., & Duncan, S. D. (2015). Gesture in discourse. In Deborah, T., Heidi, E. H., & Deborah, S. (Eds.), Handbook of discourse analysis (pp. 262290). Oxford, UK: Blackwell.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Meier, R. P., & Lillo-Martin, D. (2013). The points of language. Humana. Mente Journal of Philosophical Studies, 24, 151176.Google ScholarPubMed
Meir, I., Sandler, W., Padden, C., & Aronoff, M. (2010). Emerging sign languages. In Marschark, M. & Spencer, P. E. (Eds.), Oxford handbook of deaf studies, language, and education (pp. 267280). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Mesh, K., & Hou, L. (2018). Negation in Chatino Sign Language. Gesture, 17(3), 330374. https://doi.org/10.1075/gest.18017.mesCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morford, J. P. (2003). Grammatical development in adolescent first-language learners. Linguistics: An Interdisciplinary Journal of the Language Sciences, 41(4), 681721.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morford, J. P., & Hänel‐Faulhaber, B. (2011). Homesigners as late learners: Connecting the dots from delayed acquisition in childhood to sign language processing in adulthood. Language and Linguistics Compass, 5(8), 525537. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1749-818X.2011.00296.xCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Morris, D., Collett, P., Marsh, P., & O’Shaughnessy, M. (1979). Gestures: Their origin and distribution. New York, NY: Stein and Day.Google Scholar
Müller, C. (2018). Gesture and sign: Cataclysmic break or dynamic relations? Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1651. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01651CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Neisser, U. (1967). Cognitive psychology. New York, NY: Appleton-Century-Crofts.Google Scholar
Occhino, C., & Wilcox, S. (2017). Gesture or sign? A categorization problem. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 40, e66. https://doi.org/10.1017/S0140525X15003015CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Okrent, A. (2002). A modality-free notion of gesture and how it can help us with the morpheme vs. gesture question in sign language linguistics (or at least give us some criteria to work with). In Meier, R., Cormier, K., & Quinto-Pozos, D. (Eds.), Modality and structure in signed and spoken languages (pp. 175198). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Özçalışkan, S., & Goldin-Meadow, S. (2009). When gesture-speech combinations do and do not index linguistic change. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24(2), 190217. https://doi.org/10.1080/01690960801956911CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pfau, R., & Steinbach, M. (2011). Grammaticalization in sign languages. In Heine, B. & Narrog, H. (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of grammaticalization (pp. 683695). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Pizzuto, E. (1987). Aspetti morfosintattici. In Volterra, V. (Ed.), La Lingua Italiana dei Segni – La comunicazione visivo-gestuale dei sordi (Italian Sign Language – the visual-gestural communication of the deaf) (pp. 179209). Bologna, Italy: Il Mulino.Google Scholar
Quer, J. (2011). When agreeing to disagree is not enough: Further arguments for the linguistic status of sign language agreement. Theoretical Linguistics, 37(3/4), 189196. https://doi.org/10.1515/THLI.2011.014CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ruth-Hirrel, L., & Wilcox, S. (2018). Speech-gesture constructions in cognitive grammar: The case of beats and points. Cognitive Linguistics, 29(3), 453493. https://doi.org/10.1515/cog-2017-0116CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Sandler, W. (2009). Symbiotic symbolization by hand and mouth in sign language. Semiotica: Journal of the International Association for Semiotic Studies/Revue de l’Association Internationale de Sémiotique, 2009, 174, 241275. https://doi.org/10.1515/semi.2009.035_supp-2CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Senghas, A., & Coppola, M. (2001). Children creating language: How Nicaraguan Sign Language acquired a spatial grammar. Psychological Science, 12(4), 323328.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Shaffer, B., Jarque, M. J., & Wilcox, S. (2011). The expression of modality: Conversational data from two signed languages. In Nogueira, M. T. & Lopes., M. F. V. (Eds.), Modo e modalidade: gramática, discurso e interação (Mode and modality: grammar, discourse and interaction) (pp. 1139). Fortaleza, Brazil: Edições UFC.Google Scholar
Siple, P. (Ed.). (1978). Understanding language through sign language research. New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Stokoe, W. C. (1960). Sign language structure (Studies in Linguistics, Occasional Papers 8). Buffalo, NY: Department of Anthropology and Linguistics, University of Buffalo.Google Scholar
Stokoe, W. C. (1980). Sign language structure. Annual Review of Anthropology, 9, 365470.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stokoe, W. C., Casterline, D., & Croneberg, C. (1965). A dictionary of American Sign Language on linguistic principles. Washington, DC: Gallaudet College Press.Google Scholar
Studdert Kennedy, M. (1987). The phoneme as a perceptuomotor structure. In Allport, D. (Ed.), Language perception and production: relationships between listening, speaking, reading, and writing (pp. 6784). London, UK: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Talmy, L. (2018). The targeting system of language. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Talmy, L. (2020). Targeting in language: Unifying deixis and anaphora. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 2016.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Van Hoek, K. (1997). Anaphora and conceptual structure. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Volterra, V., Capirci, O., Rinaldi, P., & Sparaci, L. (2018). From action to spoken and signed language through gesture: Some basic developmental issues for a discussion on the evolution of the human language-ready brain. Interaction Studies, 19(1–2), 216238. https://doi.org/10.1075/is.17027.volCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wells, G. A. (1987). The origin of language: Aspects of the discussion from Condillac to Wundt. La Salle, IL: Open Court.Google Scholar
Whynot, L. A. (2016). Understanding International Sign: A sociolinguistic study. Washington, DC: Gallaudet University Press.Google Scholar
Wilbur, R. B. (2013). The point of agreement: Changing how we think about sign language, gesture, and agreement. Sign Language and Linguistics, 16(2), 221258. https://doi.org/10.1075/sll.16.2.05wilCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilcox, S. (2004). Gesture and language: Cross-linguistic and historical data from signed languages. Gesture, 4(1), 4373.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilcox, S. (2007). Routes from gesture to language. In Pizzuto, E., Pietrandrea, P., & Simone, R. (Eds.), Verbal and signed languages: Comparing structures, constructs and methodologies (pp. 107131). Berlin, Germany: Mouton de Gruyter.Google Scholar
Wilcox, S. (2009). Symbol and symptom: Routes from gesture to signed language. Annual Review of Cognitive Linguistics, 7(1), 89110. https://doi.org/10.1075/arcl.7.04wilCrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilcox, S., & Martínez, R. (2020). The conceptualization of space: Places in signed language discourse. Frontiers in Psychology: Language Sciences, 11, Article 1406. https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01406CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Wilcox, S., Martínez, R., & Morales, D. (2022). The conceptualization of space in signed languages: Placing the signer in narratives. In Jucker, A. & Hausendorf, H. (Eds.), Pragmatices of space (pp. 6394). Berlin: Mouton de Gruyter. https://doi.org/10.1515/9783110693713-003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilcox, S., & Occhino, C. (2016a). Constructing signs: Place as a symbolic structure in signed languages. Cognitive Linguistics, 27(3), 371404. https://doi.org/10.1515/cog-2016-0003CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilcox, S., & Occhino, C. (2016b). Historical change in signed languages. Oxford handbooks online. https://doi.org/10.1093/oxfordhb/9780199935345CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilcox, S., Rossini, P., & Antinoro Pizzuto, E. (2010). Grammaticalization in sign languages. In Brentari, D. (Ed.), Sign languages (pp. 332354). Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Wilcox, S., & Wilcox, P. P. (1995). The gestural expression of modality in American Sign Language. In Bybee, J. & Fleischman, S. (Eds.), Modality in grammar and discourse (pp. 135162). Amsterdam, the Netherlands: John Benjamins Publishing Company.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodward, J. (1974). Implicational variation in American Sign Language: Negative incorporation. Sign Language Studies, 5, 2030.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodward, J. (1976a). Black Southern Signing. Language in Society, 5(2), 211218.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodward, J. (1976b). Signs of change: Historical variation in American Sign Language. Sign Language Studies 10, 8194.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Woodward, J. (1978). Historical bases of American Sign Language. In Siple, P. (Ed.), Understanding language through sign language research (pp. 333348). New York, NY: Academic Press.Google Scholar
Xavier, A. N., & Wilcox, S. (2014). Necessity and possibility modals in Brazilian Sign Language (Libras). Linguistic Typology, 18, 449488. https://doi.org/10.1515/lingty-2014-0019CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

Available formats
×