Skip to main content Accessibility help
×
Hostname: page-component-745bb68f8f-grxwn Total loading time: 0 Render date: 2025-01-31T11:48:41.312Z Has data issue: false hasContentIssue false

6 - Frequency

Psychological and Methodological Considerations

from Part II - Methodological and Empirical Foundations of Constructional Research

Published online by Cambridge University Press:  30 January 2025

Mirjam Fried
Affiliation:
Univerzita Karlova
Kiki Nikiforidou
Affiliation:
University of Athens, Greece
Get access

Summary

This chapter discusses the role of frequency for Construction Grammar, especially concerning usage-based models of language, and offers definitions of different aspects of frequency, namely token frequency, type frequency, relative frequency, frequency of co-occurrence, and dispersion. It discusses how these aspects can be measured on the basis of corpus data, and how these measurements allow the observation of frequency effects that relate to phenomena such as entrenchment, ease of processing, productivity, phonological reduction, and resistance to regularization. These effects are illustrated by experimental and corpus-based analyses of lexical, morphological, and syntactic constructions. The chapter also addresses open questions regarding the role of frequency in constructionist research. Not only is the relation between corpus frequencies and theoretical notions such as entrenchment far from trivial, it is also important not to attribute effects to token frequency that can be explained by other, correlating variables. The chapter will also examine strategies that can reach beyond the use of frequency values in the future development of Construction Grammar.

Type
Chapter
Information
Publisher: Cambridge University Press
Print publication year: 2025

Access options

Get access to the full version of this content by using one of the access options below. (Log in options will check for institutional or personal access. Content may require purchase if you do not have access.)

References

Adelman, J., Brown, G. D. A., & Quesada, J. F. (2006). Contextual diversity, not word frequency, determines word-naming and lexical decision times. Psychological Science, 17(9), 814823.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Arnon, I. & Snider, N. (2010). More than words: Frequency effects for multi-word phrases. Journal of Memory and Language 62(1), 6782.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Baayen, R. H. (2009). Corpus linguistics in morphology: Morphological productivity. In Lüdeling, A. & Kytö, M., eds., Corpus Linguistics: An International Handbook. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton, pp. 900919.Google Scholar
Baayen, R. H. (2011). Demythologizing the word frequency effect: A discriminative learning perspective. In Jarema, G., Libben, G., & Westbury, C., eds., Methodological and Analytic Frontiers in Lexical Research (Part 1). Special issue of The Mental Lexicon, 5(3), 436461.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Barðdal, J. (2008). Productivity: Evidence from Case and Argument Structure in Icelandic. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Blumenthal-Dramé, A. (2017). Entrenchment from a psycholinguistic and neurolinguistic perspective. In Schmid, H.-J., ed., Entrenchment and the Psychology of Language Learning: How We Reorganize and Adapt Linguistic Knowledge. Boston: APA & Walter de Gruyter, 129152.Google Scholar
Bock, K. (1986). Syntactic persistence in language production. Cognitive Psychology, 18(3), 355387.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Boyd, J. K. & Goldberg, A. E. (2011). Learning what not to say: The role of statistical preemption and categorization in a-adjective production. Language 81(1), 129.Google Scholar
Bresnan, J., Cueni, A., Nikitina, T., & Baayen, H. (2007). Predicting the dative alternation. In Bouma, G., Krämer, I., & Zwarts, J., eds., Cognitive Foundations of Interpretation. Amsterdam: Royal Netherlands Academy of Science, pp. 6994.Google Scholar
Brezina, V. (2018). Statistics in Corpus Linguistics: A Practical Guide. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Brooks, P. & Tomasello, M. (1999). Young children learn to produce passives with nonce verbs. Developmental Psychology, 35(1), 2944.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Brooks, P., Tomasello, M., Dodson, K., & Lewis, L. B. (1999). Young children’s overgeneralizations with fixed transitivity verbs. Child Development, 70(6), 13251337.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Bybee, J. (1995). Regular morphology and the lexicon. Language And Cognitive Processes, 10(5), 425455.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J. (2002). Word frequency and context of use in the lexical diffusion of phonetically conditioned sound change. Language Variation and Change, 14(3), 261290.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J. (2006). From usage to grammar: The mind’s response to repetition. Language, 82(4), 711733.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J. (2010). Language, Usage and Cognition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J. & Scheibman, J. (1999). The effect of usage on degrees of constituency: The reduction of don’t in English. Linguistics, 37(4), 575596.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Bybee, J. & Thompson, S. (1997). Three frequency effects in syntax. Berkeley Linguistic Society, 23, 6585.Google Scholar
Clark, E. V. (1987). The principle of contrast: A constraint on language acquisition. In MacWhinney, B., ed., Mechanisms of Language Acquisition. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 133.Google Scholar
Dąbrowska, E. (2008). The effects of frequency and neighbourhood density on adult speakers – productivity with Polish case inflections: An empirical test of usage-based approaches to morphology. Journal of Memory and Language, 58(4), 931951.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Davies, M. (2008). The Corpus of Contemporary American English (COCA): 400+ Million Words, 1990–present. Available online at www.americancorpus.org.Google Scholar
Davies, M. (2010). The Corpus of Historical American English (COHA): 400+ Million Words, 1810–2009. Available online at http://corpus.byu.edu/coha.Google Scholar
Díaz-Campos, M. & Gradoville, M. (2011). An analysis of frequency as a factor contributing to the diffusion of variable phenomena: Evidence from Spanish data. In Ortiz-López, L. A., ed., Selected Proceedings of the 13th Hispanic Linguistics Symposium. Somerville: Cascadilla Proceedings Project, pp. 224238.Google Scholar
Diessel, H. (2019). The Grammar Network: How Linguistic Structure Is Shaped by Language Use. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ellis, N. C. (2002). Frequency effects in language processing: A review with implications for theories of implicit and explicit language acquisition. Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 24(2), 143188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Ferreira, V. S. & Bock, K. J. (2006). The functions of structural priming. Language and Cognitive Processes, 21, 10111029.CrossRefGoogle ScholarPubMed
Flach, S. (2021). Collostructions: An R implementation for the family of collostructional methods. Package version v.0.2.0, https://sfla.ch/collostructions/.Google Scholar
Fraser, B. (2009). Topic orientation markers. Journal of Pragmatics, 41(5), 892898.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Goldberg, A. E. (1995). Constructions: A Construction Grammar Approach to Argument Structure. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.Google Scholar
Goldberg, A. E. (2006). Constructions at Work: The Nature of Generalization in Language. Oxford: Oxford University Press.Google Scholar
Goldberg, A. E. (2019). Explain Me This: Creativity, Competition, and the Partial Productivity of Constructions. Princeton: Princeton University Press.Google Scholar
Gries, S. Th. (2008). Dispersions and adjusted frequencies in corpora. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics, 13(4), 403437.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gries, S. Th. (2010). Dispersions and adjusted frequencies in corpora: Further explorations. In Gries, S. Th., Wulff, S., & Davies, M., eds., Corpus Linguistic Applications: Current Studies, New Directions. Amsterdam: Rodopi, pp. 197212.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gries, S. Th. (2013). 50-something years of work on collocations: What is or should be next … International Journal of Corpus Linguistics, 18(1), 137165.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gries, S. Th. (2022a). What do (most of) our dispersion measures measure (most)? Dispersion? Journal of Second Language Studies, 5(2), 171205.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gries, S. Th. (2022b). On, or against?, (just) frequency. In Boas, H. C., ed., Directions for Pedagogical Construction Grammar: Learning and Teaching (with) Constructions. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton, pp. 4771.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gries, S. Th. & Divjak, D. S., eds. (2012). Frequency Effects in Language Learning and Processing. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gries, S. Th., Hampe, B., & Schönefeld, D. (2005). Converging evidence: Bringing together experimental and corpus data on the association of verbs and constructions. Cognitive Linguistics, 16(4), 635676.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gries, S. Th. & Stefanowitsch, A. (2004a). Extending collostructional analysis: A corpus-based perspective on ‘alternations’. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics, 9(1), 97129.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Gries, S. Th. & Stefanowitsch, A. (2004b). Co-varying collexemes in the into-causative. In Achard, M. & Kemmer, S., eds, Language, Culture, and Mind. Stanford: CSLI Publications, pp. 225236.Google Scholar
Hay, J. & Foulkes, P. (2016). The evolution of medial /t/ over real and remembered time. Language, 92(2), 298330.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hilpert, M. (2008). The English comparative – language structure and language use. English Language and Linguistics, 12(3), 395417.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hilpert, M. (2015). Historical linguistics. In Dąbrowska, E. & Divjak, D., eds., Handbook of Cognitive Linguistics. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton, pp. 346365.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hilpert, M. (2021). Information structure. In Aarts, B., MacMahon, A., & Hinrichs, L., eds., Handbook of English Linguistics. New York: Wiley, pp. 229248.Google Scholar
Hilpert, M. & Correia Saavedra, D. (2017). Why are grammatical elements more evenly dispersed than lexical elements? Assessing the roles of frequency and semantic generality. Corpora, 12(3), 369392.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hilpert, M. & Flach, S. (2022). A case of constructional contamination in English: Modified noun phrases influence adverb placement in the passive. In Krawczak, K., Lewandowska-Tomaszczyk, B., & Grygiel, M., eds., Analogy and Contrast in Language: Perspectives from Cognitive Linguistics. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins, pp. 283302.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hoffmann, T. (2019). English Comparative Correlatives: Diachronic and Synchronic Variation at the Lexicon–Syntax Interface. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Hopper, P. J. & Traugott, E. C. (2003). Grammaticalization. 2nd edition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Israel, M. (1996). The way constructions grow. In Goldberg, A. E., ed., Conceptual Structure, Discourse and Language. Stanford: CSLI Publications, pp. 217230.Google Scholar
Jaeger, T. F. & Snider, N. (2007). Implicit learning and syntactic persistence: Surprisal and cumulativity. University of Rochester Working Papers in the Language Sciences, 3(1), 2644.Google Scholar
Jucker, A. H. & Smith, S. W. (1998). And people just you know like ‘wow’: Discourse markers as negotiating strategies. In Jucker, A. & Ziv, Y., eds., Discourse Markers: Descriptions and Theory. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins, pp.171201.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Kay, P. & Fillmore, C. J. (1999). Grammatical constructions and linguistic generalizations: The What’s X doing Y? construction. Language, 75(1): 133.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Lorenz, D. (2013). Semi-modal Contractions in English: The Emancipating Effect of Frequency. Freiburg: Rombach.Google Scholar
Perek, F. (2018). Recent change in the productivity and schematicity of the way-construction: A distributional semantic analysis. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory, 14(1), 6597.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pfänder, S. & Behrens, H. (2016). Experience counts: An introduction to frequency effects in language. In Behrens, H. & Pfänder, S., eds., Experience Counts: Frequency Effects in Language. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton, pp. 120.Google Scholar
Pierrehumbert, J. B. (2012). Burstiness of verbs and derived nouns. In Santos, D., Linden, K., & Ng’ang’a, W., eds., Shall We Play the Festschrift Game? Essays on the Occasion of Lauri Carlson’s 60th Birthday. Berlin: Springer, pp. 99116.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pijpops, D. & Van de Velde, F. (2016). Constructional contamination: How does it work and how do we measure it? Folia Linguistica, 50(2), 543581.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Pijpops, D., de Smet, I., & Van de Velde, F. (2018). Constructional contamination in morphology and syntax: Four case studies. Constructions and Frames, 10(2), 269305.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rivas, J. & Brown, E. L. (2012). Stage-level and individual-level distinction in morphological variation. Borealis – An International Journal of Hispanic Linguistics, 1(2), 7390.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Rosemeyer, M. & Schwenter, S. E. (2019). Entrenchment and persistence in language change: The Spanish past subjunctive. Corpus Linguistics and Linguistic Theory, 15(1), 167204.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Schmid, H.-J. (2020). The Dynamics of the Linguistic System. Usage, Conventionalization, and Entrenchment. Oxford: Oxford University Press.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Stefanowitsch, A. & Gries, S. Th. (2003). Collostructions: Investigating the interaction of words and constructions. International Journal of Corpus Linguistics, 8(2), 209243.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Suttle, L. & Goldberg, A. E. (2011). The partial productivity of constructions as induction. Linguistics, 49(6), 12371269.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Torres Cacoullos, R. (2015). Gradual loss of analyzability: Diachronic priming effects. In Adli, A., García García, M., & Kaufmann, G., eds., Variation in Language: System- and Usage-Based Approaches. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton, pp. 265288.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Tottie, G. (1991). Lexical diffusion in syntactic change: Frequency as a determinant of linguistic conservatism in the development of negation in English. In Kastovsky, D., ed., Historical English Syntax. Berlin: De Gruyter Mouton, pp. 439467.Google Scholar
Turney, P. D. & Pantel, P. (2010). From frequency to meaning: Vector space models of semantics. Journal of Artificial Intelligence Research, 37, 141188.CrossRefGoogle Scholar
Yoon, J. & Gries, S. Th., eds. (2016). Corpus-Based Approaches to Construction Grammar. Amsterdam & Philadelphia: John Benjamins.CrossRefGoogle Scholar

Save book to Kindle

To save this book to your Kindle, first ensure [email protected] is added to your Approved Personal Document E-mail List under your Personal Document Settings on the Manage Your Content and Devices page of your Amazon account. Then enter the ‘name’ part of your Kindle email address below. Find out more about saving to your Kindle.

Note you can select to save to either the @free.kindle.com or @kindle.com variations. ‘@free.kindle.com’ emails are free but can only be saved to your device when it is connected to wi-fi. ‘@kindle.com’ emails can be delivered even when you are not connected to wi-fi, but note that service fees apply.

Find out more about the Kindle Personal Document Service.

  • Frequency
  • Edited by Mirjam Fried, Univerzita Karlova, Kiki Nikiforidou, University of Athens, Greece
  • Book: The Cambridge Handbook of Construction Grammar
  • Online publication: 30 January 2025
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009049139.007
Available formats
×

Save book to Dropbox

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Dropbox.

  • Frequency
  • Edited by Mirjam Fried, Univerzita Karlova, Kiki Nikiforidou, University of Athens, Greece
  • Book: The Cambridge Handbook of Construction Grammar
  • Online publication: 30 January 2025
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009049139.007
Available formats
×

Save book to Google Drive

To save content items to your account, please confirm that you agree to abide by our usage policies. If this is the first time you use this feature, you will be asked to authorise Cambridge Core to connect with your account. Find out more about saving content to Google Drive.

  • Frequency
  • Edited by Mirjam Fried, Univerzita Karlova, Kiki Nikiforidou, University of Athens, Greece
  • Book: The Cambridge Handbook of Construction Grammar
  • Online publication: 30 January 2025
  • Chapter DOI: https://doi.org/10.1017/9781009049139.007
Available formats
×